Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T07:47:52.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The developmental psychopathology of alcohol use and alcohol disorders: Research achievements and future directions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2013

Laurie Chassin*
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
Kenneth J. Sher
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
Andrea Hussong
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Patrick Curran
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Laurie Chassin, Psychology Department, Box 871104, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104; E-mail: laurie.chassin@asu.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The last 25 years have seen significant advances in our conceptualization of alcohol use and alcohol use disorders within a developmental framework, along with advances in our empirical understanding that have been potentiated by advances in quantitative methods. These include advances in understanding the heterogeneity of trajectories of alcohol outcomes; new insights about early childhood antecedents, and adolescence and emerging adulthood as important developmental periods for alcohol outcomes; a more nuanced understanding of the influences of developmental transitions, and their timing and contexts; a greater appreciation for the importance of considering multiple levels of analysis (including an increasing number of genetically informative studies); a continuing focus on studying multiple pathways underlying alcohol outcomes; and an increasing focus on studying the effects of alcohol exposure on future development. The current paper reviews these advances and suggests directions for future study.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

It seems particularly fitting that this 25th Anniversary Issue of Development and Psychopathology should include reflections on the past 25 years of research achievements in the study of alcohol use and alcohol use disorders (AUDs) because a fundamental achievement during this time period was the reconceptualization of alcohol use and AUDs within a developmental framework, including the explicit labeling of AUDs as “developmental disorders of young adulthood” (Sher & Gotham, Reference Sher and Gotham1999). Early roots of this reconceptualization can be seen in Cloninger, Bohman, and Sigvardsson's (Reference Cloninger, Bohman and Sigvardsson1981) distinction between alcoholism subtypes based on their differing ages of onset and Zucker's (Reference Zucker1986) description of the “four alcoholisms” based on differences both in age of onset and developmental course as well as earlier typologies (e.g., Knight, Reference Knight1937). Moreover, in addition to identifying age-related patterns of alcohol use and AUDs, researchers during this period discovered the value of bringing a developmental psychopathology approach to the study of etiological factors. In an editorial accompanying a 1999 Special Issue of Development and Psychopathology, Cicchetti and Luthar argued that a developmental psychopathology approach was able to integrate theories and findings that had previously emerged from different disciplines working in isolation. They noted the importance of studying transitions from substance use to substance abuse, understanding why some people avoided this transition and why some substance use problems were developmentally limited, whereas others persisted into adulthood. They also argued for the importance of studying multiple etiological pathways underlying the development of substance use disorders. These themes, as applied specifically to alcohol use and AUDs, were reiterated and elaborated in the recent (2008) special issue of Pediatrics, which was devoted to studies of underage drinking within a developmental framework (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Faden, Zucker and Spear2008). These themes were also illustrated in the strategic plan (2007–2011) of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, which described existing and future alcohol research opportunities using a life span developmental framework as the organizing principle (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2006). Finally, the growing importance of a developmental perspective over the last 25 years is reflected in research attention to the developmental appropriateness of diagnostic criteria as applied to AUDs in adolescence (Chung & Martin, Reference Chung and Martin2005; Winters, Martin, & Chung, Reference Winters, Martin and Chung2011).

In this paper, we attempt to summarize some of the central research achievements in the past 25 years, many of which have resulted from applying a developmental psychopathology perspective to the study of alcohol use and AUDs. These include advances in understanding the heterogeneity of trajectories of alcohol outcomes, new insights into early antecedents and into adolescence and emerging adulthood as important developmental periods for alcohol outcomes, the influences of developmental tasks and the timing of developmental transitions, the importance of considering multiple levels of analysis (including an increasing number of genetically informative studies), the importance of studying multiple pathways underlying alcohol outcomes, and the importance of considering the effects of alcohol exposure on future development. We do not attempt a comprehensive review, but rather we illustrate some of the major accomplishments and point to directions for future research. Moreover, we restrict our focus to issues of developmental course and etiology, and do not attempt to cover advances in intervention research (see Spoth, Greenberg, & Turrisi, Reference Spoth, Greenberg and Turrisi2008, for a review of alcohol prevention research and Deas, Reference Deas2008, for a review of adolescent alcohol treatment research).

It is interesting that alcohol research during the last 25 years has been conducted against the backdrop of general declines in drinking among adolescents (Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman & Schulenberg, Reference Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman and Schulenberg2012) and adults (in the largely white Framingham Atudy sample; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Guo, Saitz, Levy, Sartini and Niu2008). It is more difficult to assess parallel changes in AUDs because of changes in diagnostic systems over time. Over the past 30 years, there have been several large-scale, population-based epidemiological surveys using structured diagnostic interviews in the United States that have provided estimates of AUDs. These include the Epidemiologic Catchment Area study (Helzer, Burnam, & McEnvoy, Reference Helzer, Burnam, McEnvoy, Robins and Regier1991; Robins & Price, Reference Robins and Price1991); the National Comorbidity Survey (NCS; Kessler, Crum, Warner, & Nelson, Reference Kessler, Crum, Warner and Nelson1997; Kessler, McGonagle, Zhao, Nelson, Hughes, Eshleman, et al., Reference Kessler, McGonagle, Zhao, Nelson, Hughes and Eshleman1994); the National Comorbidity Survey—Replication (NCS-R; Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, & Walters, Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin and Walters2005; Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, Reference Kessler, Chiu, Demler and Walters2005), the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey (NLAES; Grant, Reference Grant1997; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Harford, Dawson, Chou, Dufour and Pickering1994; Grant & Pickering, Reference Grant and Pickering1996), and the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Stinson, Dawson, Chou, Dufour and Compton2004). All of these studies report very high past year and lifetime prevalence rates of AUDs (13.8% lifetime and 6.8% past year DSM-III in Epidemiologic Catchment Area; 23.5% lifetime and 7.7% past year DSM-III-R in NCS; 18.2% lifetime and 7.41% past year DSM-IV in NLAES; 30.3% lifetime and 8.46% past year DSM-IV in NESARC; and 18.6% lifetime and 4.4% past year DSM-IV in NCS-R). Although it is hard to compare across different diagnostic systems and different measurement approaches, NLAES and NESARC were highly similar in design (albeit with some very subtle changes in instrumentation) and separated by a 10-year period. This allows some ability to compare rates from the early 1990s to the early 2000s. Although the overall rates of past-year AUDs were roughly similar, Grant et al. (Reference Grant, Stinson, Dawson, Chou, Dufour and Compton2004) note that this overall trend reflects a decrease in dependence that was more than offset by an increase in abuse (despite the general declines in consumption over this period). However, it is difficult to determine if these changes reflect true changes in prevalence or are an artifact of very subtle variations in instrumentation (Vergés, Littlefield, & Sher, Reference Vergés, Littlefield and Sher2011).

Advances in Quantitative Methods

As we will describe in this paper, significant advances have been made in the past 25 years in the conceptualization of alcohol use and AUDs within a developmental framework, including complex and dynamic hypotheses about individual variability in continuous developmental trajectories; the joint influence of time and timing; the contribution of multiple environmental and biological contexts; the impact of transition periods and role acquisition; and the articulation of dynamic and bidirectional pathways of onset, escalation, maintenance, and desistence of alcohol use. However, these increasingly complex questions can only be tested using designs and statistical models that directly correspond to the research hypotheses under study (e.g., Curran & Willoughby, Reference Curran and Willoughby2003; Wohlwill, Reference Wohlwill1991). Here we briefly review the developments in quantitative methods that have occurred over the past 25 years that have allowed for corresponding advances in our understanding of the developmental psychopathology of alcohol use and disorders.

As with many areas of scientific inquiry, early empirical studies of child and adolescent alcohol use and abuse were primarily based on cross-sectional designs. However, the developmental psychopathology perspective requires an understanding of development over time, better revealed by longitudinal data. Twenty-five years ago, perhaps the most common statistical modeling framework applied to longitudinal data was the autoregressive cross-lagged (ARCL) panel model (e.g., Hertzog & Nesselroade, Reference Hertzog and Nesselroade1987; Mayer & Carroll, Reference Mayer and Carroll1987). However, although the ARCL approach offered many advantages, a particularly salient disadvantage was that this statistical model did not correspond well to the increasingly complex developmental theories of child and adolescent alcohol use. Whereas the ARCL model was focused on a series of time-adjacent relations among measured variables, contemporary developmental theories hypothesized the existence of individual differences in dynamic developmental trajectories of alcohol use and abuse. It was almost exactly 25 years ago when methodological and computational advances first allowed for the estimation of these hypothesized individual trajectories of development and growth. This class of analytical techniques is generally referred to as growth curve modeling.

Two broad lines of statistical methods led to the development of growth curve models. First, the multilevel modeling (MLM) framework approached this problem from the perspective of hierarchical structure such that repeated assessments were naturally nested within individuals (e.g., Bryk & Raudenbush, Reference Bryk and Raudenbush1987). This in turn allowed for the incorporation of time as a continuous explanatory variable in studies of stability and change. Second, the structural equation modeling (SEM) framework approached this problem from the perspective of repeated measures as multiple indicators that defined one or more underlying latent factors (e.g., Meredith & Tisak, Reference Meredith and Tisak1990). These latent “growth” factors were hypothesized to represent unobserved continuous trajectories of change over time. The ability to empirically estimate individual variability in smoothed developmental trajectories using either the MLM or the SEM approach has allowed for a much greater correspondence between the theoretical models and corresponding statistical models.

Growth modeling methods have been widely used in alcohol research in the last 25 years. For example, in our own work, Chassin, Curran, Hussong, and Colder (Reference Chassin, Curran, Hussong and Colder1996) used a latent growth curve model to test the relation between parental alcoholism and trajectories of adolescent substance use as mediated by parenting, temperament, and stress and negative affect. Curran, Stice, and Chassin (Reference Curran, Stice and Chassin1997) extended this model to examine the simultaneous relations between trajectories of adolescent substance use and trajectories of deviant peer affiliations. Similarly, Jackson, Sher, and Schulenberg (Reference Jackson, Sher and Schulenberg2005) applied multivariate growth models to study the conjoint development of problem behaviors and young adult alcohol use.

The design and dissemination of both the MLM and the SEM growth models has greatly enhanced our ability to estimate individual trajectories of alcohol use and to test an entire class of research hypotheses in ways not previously possible. However, these growth models imposed assumptions that may not always be met in practice. One key assumption is that the sample under study is a random one draw from a homogeneous population in which all individuals are governed by the same parametric form of the growth trajectory; any differences among individuals is reflected in the magnitude of these trajectory parameters.

Substantial problems can arise if there are subsets of individuals within the sample who are characterized by fundamentally different trajectories. For example, a subset of children may follow an increasing and then stable trajectory of alcohol use while another subset follows an increasing but then decreasing trajectory of use. To fit a single parametric function to the pooled subset of individuals would not accurately capture the relevant trajectories for either of the groups. This concern prompted the development of a broad collection of models commonly termed latent class analysis or growth mixture modeling (e.g., Muthen, Reference Muthen, Sayer and Collins2001; Muthen & Shedden, Reference Muthen and Shedden1999; Nagin, Reference Nagin1999; Nagin & Tremblay, Reference Nagin and Tremblay2001).

These latent class models do not assume that the sample represents a random one drawn from a homogeneous population. They instead allow for the potential existence of two or more discrete groups (or classes) of individuals where class membership is not directly observed (and are thus latent). The typical goal of the analysis is to first identify these latent classes and then assign individuals to the most likely class based on information that was observed in the sample. These latent class models have been applied to test a variety of questions in the development of alcohol use, most notably concerning multiple age-related trajectories of alcohol outcomes (described in more detail below).

Growth mixture models offer an important alternative to the sometimes restrictive assumption of homogeneity of functional form imposed by standard MLM and SEM growth models (e.g., Muthen, Reference Muthen2003; Nagin, Reference Nagin2004). Although highly intriguing from a theoretical perspective, a variety of concerns have been voiced about the utility of these methods in practice (e.g., Bauer, Reference Bauer2007; Bauer & Curran, Reference Bauer and Curran2003, Reference Bauer and Curran2004; Eggleston, Laub & Sampson, Reference Eggleston, Laub and Sampson2004; Sher, Jackson & Steinley, Reference Sher, Jackson and Steinley2011). A detailed discussion of these concerns is well beyond the scope of the current review, but researchers should be aware of these potential threats to validity when using these approaches.

In addition to the introduction of growth modeling and growth mixture modeling methods, there are a plethora of additional advances over the past 25 years in design, measurement, and analysis that have enhanced our ability to test developmental theories of alcohol use and AUDs. Examples include the ability to model multiple levels of context in development (e.g., repeated measures nested within a child, and a child nested within a family; e.g., Raudenbush & Bryk, Reference Raudenbush and Bryk2002); advances in estimation methods that allow for the modeling of dependent variables that are continuously but nonnormally distributed or discrete scales such as binary, ordinal, or counts (e.g., Hedeker & Mermelstein, Reference Hedeker and Mermelstein2000); methods for studying separate “trait” and age-related “state” components of alcohol use and alcohol problems (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Lee, Cho, Wang, Agrawal and Sher2012; Park, Sher, Todorov, & Heath, Reference Park, Sher, Todorov and Heath2011); methods that provide for the incorporation of partially missing data structures both within and across time (e.g., Enders, Reference Enders2010); and the development of intensive repeated measures designs that permit the gathering of multiple data points on a daily or even hourly basis (e.g., Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, Reference Bolger, Davis and Rafaeli2003). These advances allow us to test our ever increasingly complex theories of the developmental course, causes, and consequences of alcohol use with appropriate analytic methods.

Age-Related Trajectories of Alcohol Use and Alcohol Problems

There is little debate that, at least in most Western cultures, average trajectories of both heavy use and alcohol-related difficulties are characterized by escalation in adolescence, peak levels of use and prevalence of diagnosis in the early 20s, and a decline from this peak into later adulthood (albeit with some variation as a joint function of sex and ethnicity; e.g., Grant et al., Reference Grant, Stinson, Dawson, Chou, Dufour and Compton2004). However, these average trajectories are a mixture of different patterns, some of which can deviate dramatically from the average pattern. In order to characterize this heterogeneity, many investigators, employing a range of approaches (e.g., cluster analysis or growth mixture modeling) empirically attempted to “pull apart” the mean trajectory into its component parts.

As summarized in Sher et al. (Reference Sher, Jackson and Steinley2011), several broad classes of trajectories have consistently emerged: a low or nonusing trajectory (“low”), a chronic/persistently high use trajectory (“high”), a trajectory marked by high use that gradually declines over the timespan (“decrease”), and a trajectory marked by low use that gradually increases over the time span (“increase”). In addition, small group of studies also have identified a “fling” or “time-limited” trajectory (e.g., Schulenberg, O'Malley, Bachman, Wadsworth, & Johnson, Reference Schulenberg, O'Malley, Bachman, Wadsworth and Johnston1996) or a trajectory with moderate levels of alcohol involvement (e.g., Colder, Campbell, Ruel, Richardson, & Flay, Reference Colder, Campbell, Ruel, Richardson and Flay2002). It is perhaps surprising that, with the exception of those studies whose designs likely censored the ability to resolve either “decreasing trajectories” or “increasing trajectories,” the number and forms of trajectories obtained were highly similar despite study variation with respect to baseline age of the sample, the observation period covered, the frequency of measurement occasions, and the specific drinking-related measure employed. That is, the observed trajectories tended to follow, but not invariably so, the “cat's cradle” pattern (i.e., low, high, increase, decrease), although their relative prevalences vary as a function of age in expected ways. This cat's cradle phenomenon is observed in other areas of substance use research (e.g., tobacco; Hu, Muthen, Schaffran, Griesler, & Kandel, Reference Hu, Muthén, Schaffran, Griesler and Kandel2008) and in the study of conduct problems (Odgers et al., Reference Odgers, Caspi, Broadbent, Dickson, Hancox and Harrington2007). The seeming ubiquity of these patterns of trajectories, independent of the stage of development being studied, suggests that we should be cautious when interpreting the meaning of these trajectories and always be mindful that these techniques are not “carving nature at her joints” and therefore should avoid reifying them (Nagin & Tremblay, Reference Nagin and Tremblay2005).

Moreover, when thinking about trajectories of alcohol involvement, it may often be more fruitful to shift the emphasis of the “intercept” or start point of the trajectory from baseline age to a baseline drinking milestone (e.g., first time used or first time drunk) as is often done in studies of telescoping (Hussong, Bauer, & Chassin, Reference Hussong, Bauer and Chassin2008; Jackson, Reference Jackson2010) and shift our level of analysis to stage of use and use age as a moderator because the correlates of use at a given age may be different than the correlates of stage of use (Sher, Gotham, & Watson, Reference Sher, Gotham and Watson2004). More traditional trajectory modeling may obscure this important difference.

The Importance of Adolescence as a Developmental Period

Although adolescence has long been recognized as an important developmental period for the onset and escalation of drinking behavior, the past 25 years have seen significant new insights into the neurobiology of adolescent development as well as the impact of alcohol use on the adolescent brain and cognitive functions, both of which have influenced our understanding of the developmental psychopathology of AUDs. Recent studies have suggested that adolescent development is characterized by a gap between changes in dopaminergic reward systems (producing increases in sensation seeking and reward seeking beginning at puberty) and the slower and more gradual development of top-down cognitive control, which is correlated with increased myelination both within the prefrontal cortex and between the cortical and subcortical areas (Paus, Reference Paus2005; for a review of adolescent neurobiological development and its implications for risk-taking behavior, see Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2008). This gap between increased neurobiologically based increases in reward seeking and slower developing cognitive control systems predisposes adolescents toward risk-taking behavior. Moreover, although it has long been recognized that adolescent alcohol use is heavily influenced by peer contexts (including peer modeling and reinforcement of alcohol use as well as peer provision of drinking opportunities), recent neuroscience approaches have revealed that, for adolescents, the presence of peers itself activates the same reward centers that lead to risky behavior (Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert, & Steinberg, Reference Chein, Albert, O'Brien, Uckert and Steinberg2011). Thus, peers may serve to accentuate reward seeking and to make alcohol use particularly rewarding for adolescents. Note that this neurobiological underpinning of adolescent risk taking is consistent with an evolutionary perspective, which points to increases in risk taking and exploration during adolescence across animal species and the adaptive value of adolescent exploration and risk taking for promoting independence and mate selection (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Del Guidice, Dishion, Figueredo, Gray and Griskevicius2012). Nevertheless, although exploration and risk taking may have adaptive value, risk taking that manifests in the form of adolescent heavy episodic drinking also results in significant elevation in risk for short-term negative consequences (including physical assault, rape, and traffic accidents; Hingson, Zha, & Weitzman, Reference Hingson, Zha and Weitzman2009) as well as long-term risk for AUDs (e.g., Grant et al., Reference Grant, Scherrer, Lynskey, Lyons, Eisen and Tsuang2006).

Moreover, recent and accumulating evidence suggests that adolescents may be particularly sensitive to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol, with associated neurocognitive damage that may be relatively durable. Heavy exposure to alcohol in adolescence has been associated with structural and functional brain deficit, as well as deficits in cognitive functioning (Clark, Thatcher, & Tapert, Reference Clark, Thatcher and Tapert2008; De Bellis et al., Reference De Bellis, Clark, Beers, Soloff, Boring and Hall2000; Hanson, Medina, Padula, Tapert, & Brown, Reference Hanson, Medina, Padula, Tapert and Brown2011; Hargreaves, Quinn, Kashem, Matsumoto, & McGregor, Reference Hargreaves, Quinn, Kashem, Matsumoto and McGregor2009; Tapert, Brown, Baratta, & Brown, Reference Tapert, Brown, Baratta and Brown2004; Zeigler et al., Reference Zeigler, Wang, Yoast, Dickinson, McCaffree and Robinowitz2005). The apparent heightened sensitivity of the adolescent brain to alcohol-related insult is thought to be associated with neurodevelopmental vulnerability to disruption of the extensive remodeling of the brain that takes place in adolescence (e.g., synaptic pruning; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Thatcher and Tapert2008). The associated neurocognitive deficits, especially those associated with deficits in prefontal and limbic systems (Monti et al., Reference Monti, Miranda, Nixon, Sher, Swartzwelder and Tapert2005), could pose added risk for heavier alcohol involvement and engagement in a range of externalizing behaviors (e.g., Bava & Tapert, Reference Bava and Tapert2010). Although a definite causal relation in humans has yet to be established due to the paucity of truly prospective designs that assess youths prior to their first alcohol exposures, rodent models of adolescent ethanol exposure (Crews, Braun, Hoplight, Switzer, & Knapp, Reference Crews, Braun, Hoplight, Switzer and Knapp2000; Spear, Reference Spear2000; Swartzwelder, Wilson, & Tayyeb, Reference Swartzwelder, Wilson and Tayyeb1995) suggest that adolescence is a time of heightened sensitivity to persistent neurologic damage (i.e., greater deficits associated with adolescent exposures than with preadolescent or adult exposures). In addition, these rodent models allow molecular analyses of mechanisms underlying neurotoxic effects that appear heightened in adolescence and could presage enhanced susceptibility to addiction (Crews & Vetreno, Reference Crews and Vetreno2011). Multiple research programs are currently tracking cognitive functioning, neurophysiological correlates of cognition, and structural brain changes along with alcohol (and other drug) exposures. These studies will more definitively establish temporal ordering of these exposures and aberrant neurodevelopment and cognitive deficits, characterize dose–response relationships, and identify specific risk factors that increase neurologic vulnerability to alcohol-related (and other-substance related) neurological insult.

Importance of Emerging Adulthood as a Developmental Period

That alcohol use and AUDs often decline when individuals reach their early 20s and take on adult work and marital roles has been recognized in the scientific literature for more than 25 years. For example, in the mid-1980s, Zucker (Reference Zucker1986) described “developmentally limited” AUDs and Yamaguchi and Kandel (Reference Yamaguchi and Kandel1985) linked age-related declines in marijuana use to role socialization pressures that are associated with the demands of adult roles. However, in the last 25 years there have been important advances in our understanding of the importance of emerging adulthood as a developmental stage. Arnett introduced the term “emerging adulthood” in Reference Arnett2000 to describe the period of exploration in between adolescence and the full assumption of adult roles that occurs in some cultures. The last 25 years have also seen prospective longitudinal studies that have documented age-related patterns of alcohol use (e.g., Chen & Kandel, Reference Chen and Kandel1995). We now know that “maturing out” of alcohol use and AUDs is more complex than originally proposed. For example, Lee and Chassin (in press) found that declines in drinking were not uniform, but rather they were more common among heavier, problem drinkers than among other types of drinkers, and declines among heavy drinkers reflected moderation of drinking rather than cessation of drinking. Vergés et al. (Reference Vergés, Jackson, Bucholz, Grant, Trull and Wood2012) using NESARC data found that, although persistence in alcohol dependence was somewhat lower in early adulthood than later in life, age-related declines in alcohol dependence were largely produced by reductions in new onset (i.e., decreasing hazard rates). Their findings question the notion that “developmentally limited” alcohol dependence should be considered a distinct subtype of AUD, suggesting instead that alcohol dependence might be thought of as either “short duration” or “chronic and episodic” (while acknowledging that short duration AUDs may be more common at earlier ages). They also note that role transitions influence alcohol dependence at all ages, not just during emerging adulthood, albeit perhaps in different ways for men and women, for different roles and at different ages.

Moreover, although research has confirmed the relation of role transitions to changes in alcohol use and AUDs, studies have now revealed other changes that occur during emerging adulthood that also contribute to age-related changes in alcohol outcomes. As described earlier, neurobiological research has documented the gradual maturation of cognitive control systems that continues into the early to mid-20s (Paus, Reference Paus2005; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2008) and these increases in cognitive control would be expected to reduce risk-taking behavior in general and alcohol use more specifically.

Accompanying neurodevelopment and a shifting landscape of developmental roles and responsibilities are changes in personality. A major change that has occurred in our understanding of human development that has been increasing gaining recognition is that personality is not a fixed characteristic that is immutable over the life course but, rather, shows fairly dramatic normative changes throughout adolescence and later adulthood (e.g., Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, Reference Roberts, Walton and Viechtbauer2006) with increasing psychosocial maturity evidenced by decreases in negative affectivity and increases in conscientiousness and related self-control. Recent studies have demonstrated that individual differences in age-related changes in personality, including declines in behavioral disinhibition/impulsivity and negative emotionality/neuroticism and increases in conscientiousness, are correlated with declines in alcohol use in emerging adulthood and early adulthood (Littlefield, Sher, & Wood, Reference Littlefield, Sher and Wood2009). It is important that the association between personality change and change in drinking in emerging adulthood persists even when adult role occupancies are considered (Littlefield et al., Reference Littlefield, Sher and Wood2009). Moreover, even in late adolescence when (on average) rates of alcohol use and levels of use are increasing at the same time that impulsivity and sensation seeking are decreasing, we see that smaller decreases in these traits are associated with bigger increases in alcohol (and other substance) use (Quinn & Harden, Reference Quinn and Harden2013).

In addition, the association between individual differences in personality change and changes in drinking is further associated with concomitant changes in drinking motivation (Littlefield, Sher, & Wood, Reference Littlefield, Sher and Wood2010) placing the well-documented association between personality and drinking motivation (e.g., Cooper, Frone, Russell, & Mudar, Reference Cooper, Frone, Russell and Mudar1995) firmly within a developmental context. That is, change in personality is associated with changes in the reasons that drinkers report for their drinking.

Of course, the association between changes in personality and changes in alcohol use may reflect either the influence of personality on drinking or the influence of drinking on personality. It has been known for many years that some personality traits in alcoholics (especially those related to negative affectivity) become more normalized over a period of abstinence. Such findings are consistent with basic neurobiological findings showing that neuroadaptation to chronic alcohol (or other drug) use creates a persistent negative affective state motivating continued use (e.g., Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, Reference Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie and Fiore2004; Koob & Le Moal, Reference Koob and Le Moal2008). What is less clear is the degree to which less severe drinking patterns alter personality traits and, if they do, how persistent such changes are. There is some recent evidence that alcohol use might influence age-related personality change, but the results are not totally consistent. Hicks, Durbin, Blonigen, Iacono, and McGue (Reference Hicks, Durbin, Blonigen, Iacono and McGue2012) found that individuals whose AUD began in adolescence and was persistent into young adulthood did not show normative declines in negative emotionality, and they suggested that alcohol use interfered with age-related declines in negative emotionality. However, Littlefield, Vergés, Wood, and Sher (Reference Littlefield, Vergés, Wood and Sher2012) found that the effects of alcohol involvement on personality appeared to operate on shorter timeframes and to be dependent upon the developmental period under investigation (see also Quinn, Stappenbeck, & Fromme, Reference Quinn, Stappenbeck and Fromme2011). At this point, the relation between personality change and drinking patterns has yet to be fully characterized, but existing data suggest that there is a fairly strong association and that the relation between personality and drinking is a dynamic one.

Influences of the Timing and Context of Developmental Transitions

Another clearly emerging trend in the last 25 years is the increased consideration of the timing and context surrounding developmental transitions. Accumulating findings generally reflect a core tenet of developmental theory; namely, the salience not only of developmental milestones but also of the timing and social context surrounding those milestones. Two exemplars of these trends are studies of pubertal timing and role transitions involving leaving home, parenthood, and marriage.

In terms of pubertal timing, recent studies replicate early findings that girls with an earlier age of pubertal onset, even within the same family (Dick, Rose, Viken & Kaprio, Reference Dick, Rose, Viken and Kaprio2000), show an increased risk for alcohol use (Aro & Taipale, Reference Aro and Taipale1987; Stattin & Magnusson, Reference Stattin and Magnusson1990). For boys, findings are less consistent. Although studies of European boys find a greater likelihood of alcohol use among early maturers (Bratberg, Nilsen, Holmen, & Vatten, Reference Bratberg, Nilsen, Holmen and Vatten2007; Dick & Mustanski, Reference Dick, Mustanski, Pulkkinen, Kaprio and Rose2006), studies of American boys have reported a greater risk of alcohol use for both early maturers (Costello, Sung, Worthman, & Angold, Reference Costello, Sung, Worthman and Angold2007) and late maturers (e.g., Ge et al., Reference Ge, Jin, Natsuaki, Gibbons, Brody and Cutrona2006; Graber, Seeley, Brooks-Gunn, & Lewinsohn, Reference Graber, Seeley, Brooks-Gunn and Lewinsohn2004). Finally, there is evidence of “catch-up” effects in which the effects of early maturing are reduced by late adolescence and early adulthood (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Rose, Viken and Kaprio2000; Graber et al., Reference Graber, Seeley, Brooks-Gunn and Lewinsohn2004; Taga, Markey, & Friedman, Reference Taga, Markey and Friedman2006). However, longer term effects can still be detected. For example, early-maturing boys, though not girls, have reported continued elevations in alcohol use in late adolescence (Kaltiala-Heino, Koivisto, Marttunen, & Frojd, Reference Kaltiala-Heino, Koivisto, Marttunen and Frojd2011) and a greater onset of AUDs in the transition to adulthood (Graber et al., Reference Graber, Seeley, Brooks-Gunn and Lewinsohn2004).

Studies of mechanisms indicate that morphological rather than hormonal changes accompanying puberty are more strongly predictive of alcohol involvement (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Sung, Worthman and Angold2007). Such morphological changes may increase risk for drinking, particularly in girls, because they are a social signal of maturity that increases the likelihood of associating with older (and thus more alcohol involved) peers. Support for this hypothesis remains mixed, however. For example, Westling, Andrews, Hampson, and Peterson (Reference Westling, Andrews, Hampson and Peterson2008) showed that a lack of parental monitoring strengthened the relation between early pubertal timing and alcohol use for boys and girls, but deviant peer affiliations only mediated the pubertal timing effect in girls. Moreover, other studies fail to find that peer affiliations account for this association between pubertal timing and alcohol use (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Rose, Viken and Kaprio2000). Finally, moderators of early pubertal maturation have been identified such that the risk for substance use associated with early pubertal maturation is increased by lax parental supervision (for girls) and a family history of substance use, psychiatric problems, or crime (for boys; Costello et al., Reference Costello, Sung, Worthman and Angold2007) as well as urban residence (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Rose, Viken and Kaprio2000).

Studies have also examined the association between alcohol use and the timing of role transitions, including the transition to independent living (i.e., leaving the parental home), marriage, and parenthood. Youths who leave the family home earlier, with greater conflict, disagreement, and negative feelings, and motivated by more risk-promoting reasons, are more likely to come from families with well-established risk factors, such as parental alcoholism (Hussong & Chassin, Reference Hussong and Chassin2002). Although not directly studied in relation to drinking outcomes, early transitions out of the family home for reasons such as seeking freedom and unhappiness were, in turn, associated with greater adjustment problems (i.e., internalizing and externalizing symptomatology) in young adulthood.

In terms of the timing of marriage, Leonard and Eiden (Reference Leonard and Eiden2007) note that alcohol use may lead both to early marriage as part of a pattern of risky decision making and to delayed marriage owing to alcohol-related difficulties in interpersonal relationship functioning. The data provide little clarity on which of these processes may be more common, with studies showing that drinking predicts both early and late marriage as well as that drinking is unassociated with the timing of marriage (Bachman, Wadsworth, O'Malley, Johnston, & Schulenberg, Reference Bachman, Wadsworth, O'Malley, Johnston and Schulenberg1997; Fu & Goldman, Reference Fu and Goldman1996; Newcomb & Bentler, Reference Newcomb and Bentler1986). In terms of parenthood, pregnancy reduces substance use in women, though not in men, but the transition to parenthood does produce a reduction in men's drinking (O'Malley, Reference O'Malley2004). However, these effects differ as a function of the timing of parenting. For example, Little, Handley, Leuthe, and Chassin (Reference Little, Handley, Leuthe and Chassin2009) showed that early parenthood was associated with increases in substance use among men and did not produce the typical reductions in substance use for women, whereas an older and more normative age of parenthood was associated with declines in use.

Although we have focused on developmental transitions (and the timing of transitions) occurring in the adolescent and early adult years, difficulties in negotiating earlier developmental transitions (e.g., school entry) may also be critically important (Zucker, Donovan, Masten, Mattson, & Moss, Reference Zucker, Donovan, Masten, Mattson and Moss2008). Difficulties in negotiating early developmental transitions may initiate cascading processes of risk (see the 2010 Special Issue of Development and Psychopathology for studies of developmental cascades). Similarly, later life transitions such as retirement also influence drinking, although, as we have discussed, the specific context of the transition (in this case involuntary job loss versus voluntary retirement) in interplay with characteristics of the individual determine the effect (for a review, see Kuerbis & Sacco, Reference Kuerbis and Sacco2012).

Advances in Etiology: Modeling Multiple Pathways

As shown in our discussion to this point, the past 25 years have seen important research advances in identifying heterogeneity in trajectories of alcohol use and AUDs in relation to developmental milestones, particularly during adolescence and emerging adulthood. Similar advances have been seen in our understanding of etiological mechanisms. Consistent with a developmental psychopathology perspective, there has been an emphasis on equifinality; that is, multiple pathways leading to the development of AUDs (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009). Each of these pathways reflects the interplay between variables at multiple levels, from genetic risk to broad societal and historical context (Burnette & Cicchetti, Reference Burnette and Cicchetti2012; Cicchetti & Dawson, Reference Cicchetti and Dawson2002). Moreover, these models propose cascading effects over development (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010), thus tracing risk and resilience pathways from prenatal exposure, early adversity, child maltreatment, and early childhood characteristics to adult alcohol outcomes (for a review of advances in understanding of these early developmental antecedents, see Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Donovan, Masten, Mattson and Moss2008). Here we describe some of the advances in three major biopsychosocial etiological models of AUD: deviance proneness (externalizing) models, stress and negative affect (internalizing) models, and alcohol effects models. Note that these models are not mutually exclusive and are systematically interrelated (Sher, Reference Sher1991).

Deviance Proneness Pathways

An “externalizing” or “deviance proneness” pathway to alcohol use and AUDs has long been recognized and has been associated with an early onset of AUD (Cloninger et al., Reference Cloninger, Bohman and Sigvardsson1981; Iacono, Malone, & McGue, Reference Iacono, Malone and McGue2008; Zucker, Reference Zucker1986) These models view adolescent alcohol use and AUDs within a broader externalizing spectrum (Iacono et al., Reference Iacono, Malone and McGue2008) and provide one explanatory mechanism both for the development of adolescent alcohol use in general and for the intergenerational transmission of AUDs. In these models, children of parents with AUDs are at risk for a heritable predisposition to “behavioral undercontrol” (Sher, Reference Sher1991) or “behavioral disinhibition” (Iacono et al., Reference Iacono, Malone and McGue2008). The effects of behavioral undercontrol are thought to be exacerbated by poor parenting, which includes low levels of parental support, lack of monitoring, and lack of moderate, consistent discipline. Such poor parenting is likely to be provided by parents who themselves have AUDs and is also likely to be evoked by undercontrolled children (Barnow, Schuckit, Lucht, John, & Freyberger, Reference Barnow, Schuckit, Lucht, John and Freyberger2002; Chassin, Pillow, Curran, Molina, & Barrera, Reference Chassin, Pillow, Curran, Molina and Barrera1993; Eiden, Edwards, & Leonard, Reference Eiden, Edwards and Leonard2007; Mezzich et al., Reference Mezzich, Tarter, Kirisci, Feske, Day and Gao2007). In addition to experiencing poor parenting, children who are behaviorally “disinhibited” or “undercontrolled” are at risk for school failure and for ejection from mainstream peer groups (Veronneau, Vitaro, Brendgen, Dishion, & Tremblay, Reference Véronneau, Vitaro, Brendgen, Dishion and Tremblay2010), which leaves them exposed to similarly undercontrolled peers (Sijtsema, Lindenberg, & Veenstra (Reference Sijtsema, Lindenberg and Veenstra2010) who provide opportunities and norms that encourage substance use behavior (Haller, Handley, Chassin, & Bountress, Reference Haller, Handley, Chassin and Bountress2010).

Although deviance proneness pathways have long been recognized, the last 25 years have seen important progress toward understanding these etiological mechanisms. Consistent with a developmental psychopathology perspective, these models have been formally articulated as probabilistic risk pathways involving the interplay of factors at multiple levels (Sher, Reference Sher, Baer, Marlatt and Joseph1993). That is, a heritable propensity to behavioral undercontrol exerts its effects in interplay with family, peer, school, neighborhood, and larger societal influences.

In recognizing the multilevel nature of these models, an important advance in the past 25 years has been the increasing number of tests of gene–environment interaction from a developmental perspective. Twin studies suggest that the heritability of alcohol use is low in adolescence and increases with age, and heritability also increases in environments with greater alcohol availability and exposure (Kendler, Gardner, & Dick, Reference Kendler, Gardner and Dick2011; Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen and Prescott2008; Young-Wolff, Enoch, & Prescott, Reference Young-Wolff, Enoch and Prescott2011). Moreover, findings from twin studies suggest that gene–environment interactions likely operate at two broad levels (Sher et al., Reference Sher, Dick, Crabbe, Hutchison, O'Malley and Heath2010): environments that interact with genes to affect underlying vulnerability (e.g., affectivity or self-regulation) and environments that interact with genes to facilitate the expression of vulnerability (e.g., permissive environments). The task of selecting environments to study in the context of gene–environment interplay is daunting because of the extensive range of potential environmental influences, including life-stage specific factors (e.g., prenatal exposures and various social roles that vary over the life course). Given the number of possible single gene by single environment interactions, the task of identifying valid gene–environment interactions becomes quite challenging. Strategies must be developed that enhance the likelihood that valid, meaningful interactions are detected, and spurious ones minimized, including a need for careful replication.

With this caveat in mind, the last 25 years have seen a noteworthy increase in the number of studies of various environmental factors that have incorporated measured genes into their assessments (Dick, Latendresse & Riley, Reference Dick, Latendresse and Riley2011; Young-Wolff et al., Reference Young-Wolff, Enoch and Prescott2011). These studies suggest that genetic risk for adolescent alcohol use is magnified in the presence of poor parenting and reduced in the presence of good parenting. For example, studies have found that parental supervision and involvement reduced the risk for adolescent substance use that was associated with the methionine allele of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype (Laucht et al., Reference Laucht, Blomeyer, Buchmann, Treutlein, Schmidt and Esser2012) and the serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region gene (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Beach, Philibert, Chen, Lei and Murry2009) and that parental rule setting reduced the risk for adolescent alcohol use that is associated with the TAQA1 genotype (Van der Zwaluw et al., Reference Van der Zwaluw, Engels, Vermulst, Franke, Buitelaar and Verkes2010) although this genotype did not show differential effects as a function of parent rejection, overprotection, or warmth (Creemers et al., Reference Creemers, Harakeh, Dick, Meyers, Vollebergh and Ormel2011). Less work has tested the evocative effects of adolescents’ genotypes on parenting, and this is an important direction for future study (Leve, Harold, Ge, Neiderhiser, & Patterson, Reference Leve, Harold, Ge, Neiderhiser and Patterson2010). Moreover, the challenge for future research is to employ sufficiently large samples (either by studying large cohorts or by pooling samples through data sharing) to have adequate power to conduct meaningful tests of gene–environment interaction, while allowing for multiple testing with adequate control of Type 1 errors.

Another important advance in studies of deviance proneness models has been the recognition that behavioral undercontrol is a complex construct that is actually composed of multiple different propensities for impulsive behavior or “rash action” and that these propensities are differentially related to alcohol outcomes. Multiple taxonomies have been proposed to capture dimensions of behavioral undercontrol including a differentiation between top-down and bottom-up processes, and between the inability to control behavior in “hot” motivational contexts involving response to rewards and punishments versus “cold” contexts in which rewards and punishments are not salient (Castellanos-Ryan, Rubia, & Conrod, Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Rubia and Conrod2011; Handley et al., Reference Handley, Haller, Bountress, Beltran, Dandreaux and Chassin2011; Nigg, Reference Nigg2000; Whiteside & Lynam, Reference Whiteside and Lynam2001). Moreover, multiple measures have been developed to assess these propensities, including trait questionnaire measures (e.g., the UPPS; Whiteside & Lynam, Reference Whiteside and Lynam2001) and behavioral tasks of response inhibition, attentional control, and working memory (see Dick et al., Reference Dick, Smith, Olausson, Mitchell, Leeman and O'Malley2010; Nigg, Reference Nigg2000). In addition, behavioral tasks have been developed that draw on multiple dimensions of self-regulation that affect behaviors such as decision making (e.g., the Iowa Gambling Task; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, Reference Bechara, Damasio, Damasio and Anderson1994), risk taking (e.g., the Balloon Analogue Risk Task; Lejuez et al., Reference Lejuez, Read, Kahler, Richards, Ramsey and Stuart2002), and the delay discounting of rewards (e.g., the Monetary Choice Questionnaire; Kirby, Petry, & Bickel, Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999). Although measures that tap different aspects of behavioral undercontrol are only weakly related and self-report measures are generally weakly related to behavioral tasks (Birkley & Smith, Reference Birkley and Smith2011; Dick et al., Reference Dick, Smith, Olausson, Mitchell, Leeman and O'Malley2010; White et al., Reference White, Moffitt, Caspi, Bartusch, Needles and Stouthamer-Loeber1994), many of these measures predict alcohol use outcomes (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Smith, Olausson, Mitchell, Leeman and O'Malley2010; Lejuez et al., Reference Lejuez, Magidson, Mitchell, Sinha, Stevens and DeWit2010), with weaker and less consistent relations being found for lack of perseverance, poor response inhibition, and working memory (Birkley & Smith, Reference Birkley and Smith2011; Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Rubia and Conrod2011; Handley et al., Reference Handley, Haller, Bountress, Beltran, Dandreaux and Chassin2011). As described below, traits reflecting controlled, effortful processes such as working memory and response inhibition may be better thought of as moderators that either limit or enhance the influence of more automatic, motivational, approach tendencies toward alcohol (such as traits like sensation seeking, positive and negative urgency, as well as automatically activated positive associations to alcohol use). The idea that controlled, reflective processes moderate the effects of automatically activated alcohol-related associations is the central proposition of dual process models of alcohol use (see, e.g., Wiers, Ames, Hofmann, Krank, & Stacy, Reference Wiers, Ames, Hofmann, Krank and Stacy2010).

Deviance proneness models view affiliation with alcohol-use-promoting peers as the proximal mediator that leads to alcohol use among “behaviorally undercontrolled” individuals. Although peer influences on alcohol use have long been recognized, the last 25 years have seen advances both in methods of investigation and in our understanding of peer influences. Genetically informative studies have helped to disentangle the effects of peer selection from the effects of peer influence. In support of a peer selection effect, genetic factors have been shown to influence peer affiliation (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Lee, Cho, Wang, Agrawal and Sher2012; Cleveland, Wiebe, & Rowe, Reference Cleveland, Wiebe and Rowe2005; Fowler, Settle, & Christakia, Reference Fowler, Settle and Christakis2011; Hill, Emery, Harden, Mendle, & Turkheimer, Reference Hill, Emery, Harden, Mendle and Turkheimer2008). However, peers further influence substance use outcomes over and above genetically mediated peer selection effects (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Lee, Cho, Wang, Agrawal and Sher2012; Harden, Hill, Turkheimer, & Emery, Reference Harden, Hill, Turkheimer and Emery2008). Thus, peer influences may serve to mediate the effects of genetic risk on alcohol outcomes. There is also support for interactions between genetic risk and peer influences. For example, Harden et al. (Reference Harden, Hill, Turkheimer and Emery2008) found that adolescents who were genetically at risk for alcohol and tobacco use were also the most vulnerable to influences from their closest friends. Moreover, Park et al. (Reference Park, Sher, Todorov and Heath2011) found that carriers of the dopamine receptor D4 long allele were more affected by sorority/fraternity involvement in influencing alcohol dependence in young adulthood.

Another recent methodological advance has been to move beyond individuals’ reports of their peers’ drinking and map the individual's social network. Social network analysis not only identifies the characteristics of networks that are associated with alcohol use but also models the spread of drinking through a social network. For example, Ennett et al. (Reference Ennett, Bauman, Hussong, Faris, Foshee and Cai2006) found that adolescent substance users were less embedded in a social network, had greater status in the network, and had great social proximity to other peers who used substances. Rosenquist, Murabito, Fowler, and Christakis (Reference Rosenquist, Murabito, Fowler and Christakis2010) found that changes in the drinking behavior of an individual's social network predicted later changes in the individual's drinking (with female network members having stronger influence than male network members). Drinkers could be influenced either toward abstinence or toward increased drinking, suggesting that the social network can have either positive or negative influence. However, an individual's drinking was not influenced by the drinking behavior of neighbors or coworkers. In addition, alcohol use itself may function to facilitate the formation of social groups. Sayette et al. (Reference Sayette, Creswell, Dimhoff, Fairairn, Cohn and Heckman2012) administered alcohol to small groups of social drinkers who were initially unacquainted with each other. Those who were given alcohol (compared to placebo) showed more nonverbal social bonding behavior and also self-reported more social bonding.

Stress and Negative Affect or Internalizing Pathways

Over the years, research regarding the roles of stress and negative affect in the development of alcohol use and AUD has increasingly focused on identifying mechanisms of risk, moderating factors that indicate contexts and intrapersonal resources that exacerbate or mitigate this risk, and the developmental unfolding of these processes over time and drinking history. In part driving these foci was the need to address inconsistent support in the literature for the association between indicators of stress/negative affect and alcohol involvement, particularly for adolescents.

Although much of the focus on stress-drinking relations concern adolescent and adult samples, exposure to stress and trauma early in development have also been shown to have long-term impact on alcohol involvement. For example, childhood maltreatment and exposure to other traumatic events are associated with alcohol dependence in adolescence and adulthood (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Lesnick and Hegedus1997; Fenton et al., Reference Fenton, Geier, Keyes, Skodol, Grant and Hasin2012). Current developmental psychobiological models view stress influences as dynamic and likely to change with development and with different patterns of stress exposure, caregiving support, and genetic vulnerability (Gunnar & Quevado, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2007). Early trauma and chronic stress, in particular, are posited to inhibit neurogenesis, and frequent activation of the stress response will tax finite resources, increasing overall allostatic load and resulting in disruptions of neuronal plasticity and neurotoxicity. In these ways, early severe or chronic stress exposure impacts not only the immediate stress response but also future neurobiological stress responding, such that those with early stress exposure, including various forms of trauma and maltreatment, are more vulnerable to the detrimental effects of later stress exposure, forming a negative feedback loop over development. Andersen and Teicher (Reference Andersen and Teicher2009) also propose that early stress exposure produces anhedonia, a state that increases motivation for substance use. De Bellis (Reference De Bellis2001) extended these links among environmental stress, physiological stress responding, and problem behavior in development. He notes that dysregulation in the major biological stress response systems associated with childhood trauma and maltreatment have adverse influences on brain development that enhance vulnerability to psychopathology, including posttraumatic stress disorder and depression that precede AUDs.

In adolescence, there are consistent findings that those who experience high levels of life stress are more likely to use alcohol and to escalate the quantity and frequency of their use over time (Chassin et al., Reference Chassin, Curran, Hussong and Colder1996; Hussong & Chassin, Reference Hussong and Chassin2004; Wills, Vaccaro, McNamara, & Hirky, Reference Wills, Vaccaro, McNamara and Hirky1996). However, empirical support for negative affect as the mediator of the association between stress and alcohol use is more consistent in studies of adults than in studies of adolescents. Studies of adolescents support a more nuanced association, with negative affect predicting drinking behavior only for some youths and in some contexts (see Colder, Chassin, Lee, & Villalta, Reference Colder, Chassin, Lee, Villalta and Kassel2010).

Although there are several mechanisms that may underlie the association between negative affect and alcohol use, an important emerging distinction is between mechanisms that identify between-person patterns of risk versus within-person patterns of risk. The increased use of temporally informative designs (e.g., diary studies, experience sampling, events sampling, and ecological momentary assessment) have provided a novel approach to distinguishing affect as an indicator of when a given individual is at risk (i.e., on days when they have more negative affect than usual, a within-person indicator) as well as which individuals are at risk (i.e., those with greater negative affect or stress, a between-person indicator). This distinction has important implications not only for prevention and intervention program development but also for refining our etiological theories of the developmental mechanisms that underlie these drinking behaviors.

Key within-person mechanisms implicated in the relation between negative affect and drinking include variants of the classic negative reinforcement model (i.e., drinking to alleviate distress; Jellinek, Reference Jellinek1960; Wikler, Reference Wikler1948). Much of the intensive daily assessment research that evaluates this mechanism focuses on early adulthood, when drinking is more frequent and more easily observed on a daily basis. In general, these studies show that on days when adults report higher levels of negative mood than is typical for themselves, they also tend to report higher rates of consumption, urges to drink, and alcohol-related problems (Armeli, Tennen, Affleck, & Kranzler, Reference Armeli, Tennen, Affleck and Kranzler2000; Litt, Cooney, & Morse, Reference Litt, Cooney and Morse2000; Simons, Gaher, Oliver, Bush, & Palmer, Reference Simons, Gaher, Oliver, Bush and Palmer2005). Although this association is generally posited to be stronger in those with coping motives for drinking, evidence has both supported the cross-level interaction between coping motives and daily negative affect predicting drinking (Arbeau, Kuiken, & Wild, Reference Arbeau, Kuiken and Wild2011) and failed to do so (Armeli, Conner, Cullum, & Tennen, Reference Armeli, Conner, Cullum and Tennen2010; Hussong, Hicks, Levy, & Curran, Reference Hussong, Hicks, Levy and Curran2001; Todd, Armeli, Tennen, Carney, & Affleck, Reference Todd, Armeli, Tennen, Carney and Affleck2003). Identifying additional potential moderators of this risk, other studies show that individuals with daily intense negative emotions are less likely to consume alcohol on a given day if they are better at identifying and differentiating discrete forms of negative emotion (Kashdan, Ferssizidis, Collins, & Muraven, Reference Kashdan, Ferssizidis, Collins and Muraven2010) and if they have positive social support in their close friendships (Hussong et al., Reference Hussong, Hicks, Levy and Curran2001). A challenge with these models is that the interval over which to test the association between negative affect and drinking (i.e., hours, days, or weeks) is unclear. Moreover, survival analyses of daily assessment data suggest that the size of the interval may also be psychologically meaningful, because factors such as coping motives and greater alcohol-related consequences predict a shorter interval between peak levels of negative emotion and subsequent drinking (Hussong, Reference Hussong2007).

Many fewer studies use these methods to study drinking in adolescence (although, for a review of this work predicting adolescent smoking, see Mermelstein, Hedeker, & Weinstein, Reference Mermelstein, Hedeker, Weinstein and Kassel2010). Following a small sample of rising ninth graders using a 21-day experience sampling paradigm, we have found higher rates of drinking on days following elevated negative mood only in youths who report more depressive symptoms, fewer conduct problems, and poorer parent emotion socialization (Gould, Hersh, & Hussong, in press; Hersh & Hussong, Reference Hersh and Hussong2009; Hussong, Feagans-Gould, & Hersh, Reference Hussong, Feagans-Gould and Hersh2008; Reimuller, Shadur, & Hussong, Reference Reimuller, Shadur and Hussong2011). Moreover, some of these effects may strengthen over time. For example, we found that after (but not before) the transition to high school, adolescents who reported more sadness than usual were also more likely to report same-day drinking, but only if they had lower levels of parental involvement in their lives (an indicator of social support; Gottfredson & Hussong, Reference Gottfredson and Hussong2011). Thus, associations between negative affect and drinking could strengthen over times of transition, stress, or developmental gain, particularly for youths lacking alternative coping skills or resources.

Augmenting the intensive daily assessment designs and physiological studies of negative affect-drinking associations are studies of the potential genetic underpinnings of this risk mechanism. There are fewer genetically informative studies of stress-negative affect pathways than of the deviance proneness pathways that were described earlier (though see Nurnberger, Foroud, Flury, Meyer, & Wiegand, Reference Nurnberger, Foroud, Flury, Meyer and Wiegand2002). Although a review of the literature by Saraceno, Munafó, Heron, Craddock, and Van Den Bree (Reference Saraceno, Munafó, Heron, Craddock and Van Den Bree2009) found an emerging set of potential markers for co-occurring alcohol problem use and internalizing symptoms (e.g., the serotonin transporter short allele, the monoamine oxidase A low-activity alleles, and the dopamine D2 receptor Taq A1 allele), this literature is increasingly focused on identifying gene–gene and gene–environment interactions underlying this association.

Recognition is also emerging in the literature that more integrative models are needed to understand the association between negative affect and drinking associations within a larger developmental context. For example, Hussong, Jones, Stein, Baucom, and Boeding (Reference Hussong, Jones, Stein, Baucom and Boeding2011) define the potentially unique risk processes underlying the internalizing pathway as emphasizing problems with emotion regulation across the life span. This pathway recognizes negative reinforcement as a central process translating deficits in emotion regulation into alcohol-related behaviors and risk for addiction, particularly pertinent for predicting a negative affect form of AUD as a salient outcome. Drawing from a larger developmental literature, this pathway posits that risk for later AUD may first emerge as inhibited temperament and emotion dysregulation in early childhood. Studies showing that these early temperament markers predict later alcohol use further support the salience of early behavioral inhibition for the internalizing pathway to AUD. For example, Caspi, Moffitt, Newman, and Silva (Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Newman and Silva1996) found that inhibited (fearful, shy, and easily upset) 3-year-olds, compared to their peers, had higher rates of depression and, for boys, alcohol-related problems at age 21. Other studies also suggest that indices of internalizing behavior between ages 3 and 10 are predictive of more alcohol-related problems and disorder in midadolescence to early adulthood (for a review, see Zucker, Reference Zucker, Cicchetti and Cohen2006). For those following this pathway, accumulated risks associated with continued emotion dysregulation over development further increase risk for later AUD, particularly for youths in at-risk homes marked by parental alcoholism and comorbid affective disorders with well-documented associations with poor child outcomes (Hussong, Flora, Curran, Chassin, & Zucker, Reference Hussong, Flora, Curran, Chassin and Zucker2008; Hussong et al., Reference Hussong, Wirth, Edwards, Curran, Chassin and Zucker2007).

In turn, early emotion dysregulation and related internalizing symptoms are associated with interpersonal skill deficits and difficulties in peer contexts (Graber & Sontag, Reference Graber, Sontag, Lerner and Steinberg2009; Nelson, Rubin, & Fox, Reference Nelson, Rubin and Fox2005; Rubin & Mills, Reference Rubin and Mills1991). Some forms of negative affect, particularly those that may relate to cautious behavior and withdrawal from peers, may actually reduce risk for drinking during adolescence. For example, Kaplow, Curran, Angold, and Costello (Reference Kaplow, Curran, Angold and Costello2001) showed that young teens with a separation anxiety disorder (often linked to reduced peer interaction) delayed the onset of alcohol use compared to their peers, whereas teens with a generalized anxiety disorder (which may not pull youths out of peer contexts) had an earlier onset of alcohol use. Whether or not adolescents engage in drinking associated with negative affect may also be moderated by a host of factors (Colder et al., Reference Colder, Chassin, Lee, Villalta and Kassel2010; Sher, Reference Sher1991). For example, theorized moderators associated with the internalizing pathway predict that adolescents with more positive expectations for the effects of alcohol use, interpersonal skill and coping deficits that lead to associations with deviant peers or to social withdrawal and the desire to self-medicate, and coping motives for alcohol use may be more likely to drink in response to cues for negative affect and internalizing symptoms. Supporting this assertion is evidence suggesting that a stronger endorsement of coping motives increases the risk for drinking on days characterized by greater fear and shyness (Hussong, Galloway, & Feagans-Gould, Reference Hussong, Galloway and Feagans-Gould2005) and that more disengaged coping increases the association between stress and substance use in adolescents (Wills, Sandy, Yaeger, Cleary, & Shinar, Reference Wills, Sandy, Yaeger, Cleary and Shinar2001). However, by young to midadulthood, access to alcohol is easier and may not be as strongly mediated by the peer and social context. To the extent that drinking behavior is initiated, positive expectancies for alcohol use and related coping motives for drinking may be reinforced by experience, creating the potential for cyclical patterns of negative affect and drinking implicated in negative reinforcement models of addiction (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie and Fiore2004; McCarthy, Curtin, Piper, & Baker, Reference McCarthy, Curtin, Piper, Baker and Kassel2010).

An active area of research concerns the intersection of negative affect and behavioral undercontrol indicators of risk. Studies that control for externalizing symptoms when predicting alcohol use from internalizing symptoms often fail to find unique effects of negative affect (Capaldi, Reference Capaldi1991; Capaldi & Stoolmiller, Reference Capaldi and Stoolmiller1999; Miller-Johnson, Lochman, Coie, Terry, & Hyman, Reference Miller-Johnson, Lochman, Coie, Terry and Hyman1998). However, studies indicate that the two forms of symptomatology may interact. Some studies suggest that the two together exacerbate risk (Simons et al., Reference Simons, Gaher, Oliver, Bush and Palmer2005; Wardell, O'Connor, Read, & Colder, Reference Wardell, O'Connor, Read and Colder2012). In addition, borderline personality disorder, a condition associated with high levels of negative affectivity, affective instability, and poor impulse control, is associated with high levels of substance use and dependence (Trull, Sher, Minks-Brown, Durbin, & Burr, Reference Trull, Sher, Minks-Brown, Durbin and Burr2000). However, other studies, including those using experience sampling data, indicate that negative affect may be a stronger predictor of drinking behavior in the absence of externalizing symptomatology (Dierker, Vesel, Sledjeski, Costello, & Perrine, Reference Dierker, Vesel, Sledjeski, Costello and Perrine2007; Hussong, Feagans-Gould, et al., Reference Hussong, Bauer and Chassin2008). Physiological data provide another perspective for considering this interaction. Some laboratory data suggest that individuals who are temperamentally “underregulated” may derive the strongest psychophysiological stress-response-dampening benefits from consuming alcohol (Levenson, Oyama, & Meek, Reference Levenson, Oyama and Meek1987), consistent with a hypothesized stronger link between stress or negative affect and substance use for individuals who are low in self-regulation. In general, although deviance proneness and “stress and negative affect models” have often been studied in isolation, the interrelations between these models are an important future research direction.

In addition, it is important to understand how negative affect influences drinking not only over development but also over the course of drinking history. In a reformulation of the classic negative reinforcement model, Baker et al. (Reference Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie and Fiore2004; McCarthy et al., Reference McCarthy, Curtin, Piper, Baker and Kassel2010) suggest that after problematic drinking patterns become entrenched, negative affect and internalizing symptoms that motivate alcohol use may occur outside of awareness, triggered by interceptive cues that precede affective symptoms of withdrawal. As a result, the phenomenological experience of negative affect as a cue for drinking may change over the course of drinking history, necessitating changes not only in our conceptualization of this association but also in our methods for assessing it.

Finally, research is needed concerning specific types of negative affect that signal risk for drinking. For example, Shoal, Castaneda, and Giancola (Reference Shoal, Castaneda and Giancola2005) suggest that worry may reduce risk for substance use in adolescents who are high in negative affect. Moreover, the role of positive affect remains an area of increasing study. Although distinct models for drinking related to negative affect (i.e., negative reinforcement models driven by coping motives for drinking) and positive affect (i.e., positive reinforcement models driven by enhancement motives for drinking; Cooper, Reference Cooper1994) have received support over the years, alternative revised models of reinforcement sensitivity (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000) may suggest ways in which these models may also be integrated in future research.

Alcohol Effects Pathways

Alcohol affects virtually all major neurotransmitter systems, especially at levels associated with intoxication, and these neurotransmitter systems play a key role in regulating cognition, affect, and behavior (e.g., Vengeliene, Bilbao, Molander, & Spanagel, Reference Vengeliene, Bilbao, Molander and Spanagel2008). For purposes of discussion, the subjective and hedonic effects of alcohol can be separated into three broad classes: (a) positive reinforcing effects (e.g., euphoric or arousing) that are thought to be mediated, like other drugs of abuse, by dopamine reward pathways; (b) negatively reinforcing effects (e.g., anxiolytic or antidepressant) that are thought to be largely mediated via GABA-ergic pathways; and (c) punishing effects such as acute sedation and discomfort that could arise from a number of factors (e.g., peripheral effects of alcohol or its metabolite, acetaldehyde, on the gastrointestinal and vascular system or direct effects on brain systems related to sedation). From the perspective of pharmacological vulnerability, one could be at increased risk for the development of alcohol problems because of individual differences in any of these effects (e.g., heightened reward or decreased sensitivity to punishment). Early evidence suggested that individuals at risk for alcohol problems experienced a low level of response to alcohol effects (Schuckit, Reference Schuckit1984; Schuckit & Smith, Reference Schuckit and Smith1996) with later modifications suggesting a low level of response to aversive effects but increased levels of response to reinforcing effects (Newlin & Thompson, Reference Newlin and Thomson1990; see Morean & Corbin, Reference Morean and Corbin2010; Quinn & Fromme, Reference Quinn and Fromme2011, for recent reviews).

In the past 25 years, there has been considerable debate as to how much various effects of alcohol are attributable to direct effects of alcohol on brain systems associated with reward or punishment versus how much is mediated via effects on cognition in interaction with environment. Less than 25 years ago, Steele and Joseph (Reference Steele and Josephs1990) proposed the “alcohol myopia” theory of alcohol effects, which posits that the effect of alcohol is contingent upon information processing of more or less salient features of the drinking context, and such an interaction between cognition and the environment could lead to reinforcement, punishment, or disinhibition depending upon the nature of the situational context. After a decade of research, Lang, Patrick, and Stritzke (Reference Lang, Patrick, Stritzke, Leonard and Blane1999) reviewed the available human literature on the effects of alcohol and emotion and concluded that “evidence of intrinsic reward or selective stress reduction seems neither powerful enough nor reliable enough to account for the widespread appeal of alcohol and the prevalence of alcoholism” (p. 360). They also argued that most of alcohol's effects on affect and emotional responding were secondary to effects on cognition (and subsequent processing of relevant contextual cues and their relevance to the self). However, it has long been known that intermediate doses of alcohol have unpredictable effects on negative emotions, though as the dose of alcohol approaches those associated with “binge” levels of intoxication, negatively and positively reinforcing effects tend to be observed reliably, independent of context (see Sher, Reference Sher, Leonard and Blane1987). More recent studies (e.g., Donohue, Curtin, Patrick, & Lang, Reference Donohue, Curtin, Patrick and Lang2007; Sher, Bartholow, Peuser, Erickson, & Wood, Reference Sher, Bartholow, Peuser, Erickson and Wood2007) have clearly demonstrated unconditional effects on negative emotions across diverse measures and experimental paradigms (see Sher & Grekin, Reference Sher, Grekin and Gross2007). From an etiological perspective, the evolving research evidence suggests that different processes may be involved in understanding the reinforcing properties of alcohol at lower and higher doses, and suggests that there are different individual difference risk factors for negative consequences at lower versus higher doses.

The reinforcing effects of alcohol typically are experienced on the ascending limb of the blood alcohol concentration (BAC) curve (measured from the time that drinking is initiated until individuals reach their peak BAC), and the punishing effects typically occur on the descending limb of the BAC curve (measured at the time subsequent to that when individuals have reached their peak BAC), rendering the net effect of alcohol to be one that is biphasic (Sher, Wood, Richardson, & Jackson, Reference Sher, Wood, Richardson, Jackson and Earleywine2005). These biphasic effects are observable in the laboratory, and individuals also report expecting to experience such effects prior to drinking (Earleywine, Reference Earleywine1994; Earleywine & Martin, Reference Earleywine and Martin1993). Heavier drinkers have been found more likely to experience stronger stimulant effects relative to sedative effects, whereas lighter drinkers have been found to experience the opposite. The biphasic effects of alcohol are subject to individual differences (see Sher & Wood, Reference Sher, Wood and Earleywine2005; Sher et al., Reference Sher, Wood, Richardson, Jackson and Earleywine2005).

From the point of view of developmental psychopathology, an important advance in studies of pharmacological vulnerability models is evidence that adolescents may show unique patterns of reactivity to ethanol that make them particularly susceptible to drinking at high levels. Spear and Varlinskaya (Reference Spear and Varlinskaya2005) review studies with rodents indicating that adolescents are less sensitive than are their adult counterparts to a variety of punishing effects that could serve to limit consumption both acutely (e.g., sedation or motor impairment) and following intoxication (e.g., hangover-like symptoms). In addition, adolescents appear to be especially sensitive to alcohol-related social reward, which can goad further drinking. This configuration of factors is similar to the pattern of effects noted for children of alcoholics (e.g., Newlin & Thomson, Reference Newlin and Thomson1990) and could represent an added level of risk associated with adolescence. In addition to these different levels of sensitivity, chronic alcohol administration effects on tolerance development appears to differ between adolescents and adult rats in complex ways (Morales, Varlinskaya, & Spear, Reference Morales, Varlinskaya and Spear2011). For ethical reasons, there are few highly controlled studies of alcohol consumption in adolescents, but tolerance is one of the most commons symptoms of dependence in high school drinkers (among those who meet criteria for an AUD and among those who do not; Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, Reference Lewinsohn, Rhode and Seeley1996). Moreover, during emerging adulthood, rates of tolerance decrease even among those who maintain heavy drinking patterns over an extended period of time (O'Neill & Sher, Reference O'Neill and Sher2000), findings consistent with adolescence being a period of time of altered alcohol sensitivity.

From a theoretical perspective, individual differences in alcohol effects should influence beliefs and expectancies about alcohol, which further influence drinking behavior. The last 25 years have seen the recognition that consciously held beliefs and expectancies about alcohol are important, but so are positive and negative associations to alcohol that are more automatic and less likely to be in conscious awareness (such as implicit attitudes and automatic approach or avoidance tendencies). There have been important developments in methods for measuring these implicit associations as well as evidence that these automatic associations predict alcohol outcomes (Roefs et al., Reference Roefs, Huiding, Smulders, MacLeod, de Jong and Wiers2011; Stacy and Wiers, Reference Stacy and Wiers2010). In addition to global scores on these measures, researchers have just begun to apply process models of these tasks to alcohol outcomes. For example, O'Connor, Lopez-Vergara, and Colder (Reference O'Connor, Lopez-Vergara and Colder2012) found that (for children ages 10–12), those who had begun to drink had weaker automatic activation of negative alcohol-related associations than did those who were abstainers. Moreover, the relation between automatic associations and drinking outcomes varies for individuals at differing genetic risk, in terms of μ-opioid receptor M1, dopamine receptor D4 (Pieters et al., Reference Pieters, Van Der Vorst, Burk, Schoenmakers, Van Den Wildenberg and Smeets2011), aldehyde dehydrogenase 2, and catechol-O-methyltransferase (Hendershot, Lindren, Liang, & Hutchison, Reference Hendershot, Lindren, Liang and Hutchison2012). The last 25 years have also seen the development of dual process models of drinking behavior, in which the relation between automatically activated alcohol associations and drinking behavior is moderated by reflective, conscious, controlled processes (for a review, see Wiers et al., Reference Wiers, Ames, Hofmann, Krank and Stacy2010). In support of these models, the relation between drinking behavior and implicit associations or automatic approach tendencies toward alcohol has been shown to be weaker for individuals with higher levels of working memory and for individuals with higher levels of response inhibition (Houben & Wiers, Reference Houben and Wiers2009; Peeters et al., Reference Peeters, Wiers, Monshouwer, Van de Schoot, Janssen and Vollebergh2012; Thush et al., Reference Thush, Wiers, Ames, Grenard, Susman and Stacy2008).

Conclusions and Future Directions

The last 25 years have seen impressive advances in our understanding of the developmental psychopathology of alcohol use and AUDs. Much has been learned about the early antecedents and cascading processes of risk that set the stage for later alcohol problems, and one future direction that emerges from this work is the need to understand the ways in which early adversity and maltreatment influence the later development of risk for AUD. Moreover, although the last 25 years have seen an expansion of a developmental psychopathology approach as applied to alcohol outcomes in adolescence and emerging adulthood, there has been less application of a developmental psychopathology perspective in terms of understanding alcohol outcomes in midlife and later life. Age-specific etiological factors in midlife and late-life (including the effects of role transitions) are in need of future study. Future research on aging samples is warranted, particularly because higher alcohol use among the baby boomer cohort forecasts increases in alcohol problems among older individuals (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2000). These alcohol outcomes are not only important in their own right but also are of potential significance in influencing alcohol use in offspring and grandchildren. Thus, future research should continue to expand the study of risk and resilience processes across the life span and across generations.

Much has been learned about the importance of adolescence and emerging adulthood as developmental stages for the initiation and decline in alcohol use and AUDs, and these recent advances illuminate areas of needed future research. Prospective studies are needed to identify potential neurotoxic effects of adolescent alcohol exposure and the effects of adolescent alcohol use on cognitive functioning. These studies require multiple levels of measurement (including neuropsychological assessment and imaging studies of underlying neurocircuitry). These studies should identify the dose–response relation between adolescent drinking and potential cognitive sequelae, and should specify the role of adolescent drinking in the context of correlated risk factors such as other forms of drug use. Such research is currently underway through a National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism initiative. Moreover, research is needed to illuminate the influence of adolescent exposure to alcohol on the normative development of cognitive control from adolescence to adulthood. Although there appear to be complex, bidirectional relations between alcohol use and personality development, little is known about the dosage and duration of alcohol intake that might influence the development of cognitive control and psychosocial maturity at these ages. In addition, further research is needed to clarify the nature of sensitivity to alcohol's rewarding and aversive effects during adolescence and the ways in which such developmentally specific alterations in sensitivity may influence trajectories of alcohol consumption.

The recent concerns with potential neurotoxic effects of adolescent exposure to alcohol as well as adolescents’ altered sensitivity to the rewarding and aversive effects of alcohol have potential implications for policy. In particular, they raise concerns with recent proposals (such as the Amethyst initiative) to lower the minimum legal drinking age. Many arguments against the lowering of the minimum legal drinking age cite public health protections against the short-term adverse consequences of drinking, such as reductions in fatal and nonfatal accidents and crime (e.g., Carpenter & Dobkin, Reference Carpenter and Dobkin2011), and these are clearly important arguments. However, if adolescent alcohol exposure does prove to have neurotoxic effects and creates cognitive impairment, the potential long-term consequences provide additional arguments against lowering the drinking age. A better quantification of the ages of vulnerability and dose effects that produce negative effects will better inform such policy debates. Moreover, if adolescent alcohol exposure is found to impair the normative age-related development of cognitive control and psychosocial maturity, this may also have policy issues in terms of adolescents’ legal culpability. Steinberg and Scott (Reference Steinberg and Scott2003) argued that adolescents have reduced criminal culpability because of their immature decision-making capacity. If adolescent alcohol exposure reduces the development of cognitive control and psychosocial maturity, it might result in a longer duration of immaturity or a greater lack of maturity, both of which can be factors in determining criminal culpability.

For methodological issues, the increasing sophistication of quantitative methods requires continuing improvement in measurement and in understanding measurement equivalence across developmental periods. Such a developmental approach to measurement will be important in assessing the performance of the new DSM-5. Moreover, although assessments of AUDs are currently based on self-report, in the future it may be possible to incorporate the use of behavioral and psychophysiological methods that tap into underlying processes of addiction. Finally, given the growing interest in gene–environment interaction, it is important for future studies to adopt strategies that provide sufficient statistical power to detect interactions, use appropriate methods to probe the form of interactions, and minimize spurious findings through replication and controls for multiple testing. Studies of gene–environment interaction also need to test hypotheses within a developmental context and within the context of gene–environment correlation (i.e., passive, active, and evocative effects).

In terms of etiological pathways, both the deviance proneness pathway and the stress and negative affect pathway are in need of studies that provide greater specificity in terms of the facets of behavioral undercontrol and “negative affect” that predict alcohol outcomes. Understanding these facets on multiple levels (e.g., behavioral tasks and psychophysiological measures) is a particularly important and challenging task. Applying process models that illuminate the multiple processes underlying behavioral task performance may be particularly helpful in achieving this goal. Moreover, future studies should identify potential contextual influences (such as parenting and family influences) that affect the development of behavioral undercontrol and affect regulation. Identifying these potentially modifiable contextual factors is particularly important, because they are potential targets for preventive and treatment interventions. In this regard, it will be important to study alcohol problems in terms of their boundaries with other forms of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology as well as their boundaries with other forms of substance use disorders. Moreover, although we have made substantial progress in understanding the deviance proneness, stress and negative affect, and alcohol effects models, these models are not meant to be considered in isolation of each other, but rather they are systematically interrelated. For example, individuals with high levels of behavioral undercontrol may experience greater stress response dampening benefits of alcohol and thus be more likely to use alcohol to cope with stress. Moreover, early trauma exposure may also be a common antecedent with important implications for both emotional and behavioral dysregulation. These mechanisms integrate all three hypothesized pathways, and future studies should examine such integrative effects.

Finally, future research should also continue to test these questions on multiple levels. We have already described the increasing number of genetically informative studies with specific suggestions for future methods, but other multiple-level studies are needed that include identifying underlying neural mechanisms, social network influences, and neighborhood and social policy influences. It is particularly important to continue to develop theory and empirical evidence to identify the ways in which developmental pathways of risk and resilience to AUDs might be influenced by varying cultural values across demographic groups (race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and gender). Given the gains of the past 25 years and the current amount of research interest and activity, we anticipate that the next 25 years of advances in the developmental psychopathology of alcohol use and AUDs will be equally productive.

References

Andersen, S., & Teicher, M. (2009). Desperately driven and no brakes: Developmental stress exposure and subsequent risk for substance abuse. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 516524.Google Scholar
Arbeau, K. J., Kuiken, D., & Wild, T. C. (2011). Drinking to enhance and to cope: A daily process study of motive specificity. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 11741183.Google Scholar
Armeli, S., Conner, T. S., Cullum, J., & Tennen, H. (2010). A longitudinal analysis of drinking motives moderating the negative affect-drinking association among college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24, 3847.Google Scholar
Armeli, S., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & Kranzler, H. R. (2000). Does affect mediate the association between daily events and alcohol use? Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 61, 862871.Google Scholar
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469480.Google Scholar
Aro, H., & Taipale, V. (1987). The impact of timing of puberty on psychosomatic symptoms among fourteen- to sixteen-year-old Finnish girls. Child Development, 58, 261268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachman, J. G., Wadsworth, K. N., O'Malley, P. M., Johnston, L. D., & Schulenberg, J. E. (1997). Smoking, drinking, and drug use in young adulthood: The impacts of new freedoms and new responsibilities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R., & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111, 3351.Google Scholar
Barnow, S., Schuckit, M., Lucht, M., John, U., & Freyberger, H. (2002). The importance of a positive family history of alcoholism, parental rejection and emotional warmth, behavioral problems and peer substance use for alcohol problems in teenagers: A path analysis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 305315.Google Scholar
Bauer, D. J. (2007). Observations on the use of growth mixture models in psychological research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 757786.Google Scholar
Bauer, D. J., & Curran, P. J. (2003). Distributional assumptions of growth mixture models: Implications for overextraction of latent trajectory classes. Psychological Methods, 8, 338363.Google Scholar
Bauer, D. J., & Curran, P. J. (2004). The integration of continuous and discrete latent variable models: Potential problems and promising opportunities. Psychological Methods, 9, 329.Google Scholar
Bava, S., & Tapert, S. F. (2010). Adolescent brain development and the risk for alcohol and other drug problems. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 398413.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 715.Google Scholar
Birkley, E., & Smith, G. T. (2011). Recent advances in understanding the personality underpinnings of impulsive behavior and their role in risk for addictive behaviors. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 4, 215227.Google Scholar
Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579616.Google Scholar
Bratberg, G. H., Nilsen, T. I. L., Holmen, T. L., & Vatten, L. J. (2007). Perceived pubertal timing, pubertal status and the prevalence of alcohol drinking and cigarette smoking in early and late adolescence: A population based study of 8950 Norwegian boys and girls. Acta Paediatrica, 96, 292295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Beach, S. R., Philibert, R. A., Chen, Y. F., Lei, M. K., Murry, V. M., et al. (2009). Parenting moderates a genetic vulnerability factor in longitudinal increases in youths’ substance use. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 111.Google Scholar
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1987). Application of hierarchical linear models to assessing change. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 147158.Google Scholar
Burnette, M. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2012). Multilevel approaches toward understanding antisocial behavior: Current research and future directions. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 703704.Google Scholar
Capaldi, D. M. (1991). Co-occurrence of conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: I. Familial factors and general adjustment at Grade 6. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 277300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capaldi, D. M., & Stoolmiller, M. (1999). Co-occurrence of conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: III. Prediction to young-adult adjustment. Development and Psychology, 11, 5984.Google Scholar
Carpenter, C., & Dobkin, C. (2011). The minimum legal drinking age and public health. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 35, 133156.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Newman, D. L., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 10331039.Google Scholar
Castellanos-Ryan, N., Rubia, K., & Conrod, P. (2011). Response inhibition and reward response biases mediate the predictive relationships between impulsivity and sensation seeking and common and unique variance in conduct disorder and substance misuse, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 115.Google Scholar
Chassin, L., Curran, P. J., Hussong, A. M., & Colder, C. R. (1996). The relation of parent alcoholism to adolescent substance use: A longitudinal follow-up study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 7080.Google Scholar
Chassin, L., Lee, M., Cho, Y-Il., Wang, F., Agrawal, A., Sher, K., et al. (2012). Testing multiple levels of influence in the intergenerational transmission of alcohol disorders from a developmental perspective: The example of alcohol use promoting peers and μ-opioid receptor M1 variation. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 953967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chassin, L., Pillow, D. R., Curran, P. J., Molina, B. S. G., & Barrera, M. (1993). Relation of parental alcoholism to early adolescent substance use: A test of three mediating mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 449454.Google ScholarPubMed
Chein, J., Albert, D., O'Brien, L., Uckert, K., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain's reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14, 110.Google Scholar
Chen, K., & Kandel, D. B. (1995). The natural history of drug use from adolescence to the mid-thirties in a general population sample. American Journal of Public Health, 85, 4147.Google Scholar
Chung, T., & Martin, C. (2005). What were they thinking? Adolescents’ interpretations of DSM-IV alcohol dependence symptoms queries and implications for diagnostic validity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 80, 191200.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Dawson, G. (2002). Editorial: Multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 417420.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Luthar, S. S. (1999). Developmental approaches to substance use and abuse. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 655656.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology [Editorial]. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597600.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1625.Google Scholar
Clark, D. B., Lesnick, L., & Hegedus, A. (1997). Traumas and other adverse life events in adolescents with alcohol abuse and dependence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 17441751.Google Scholar
Clark, D. B., Thatcher, D. L., & Tapert, S. F. (2008). Alcohol, psychological dysregulation, and adolescent brain development. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 375385.Google Scholar
Cleveland, H. H., Wiebe, R., & Rowe, D. C. (2005). Genetic influences on associations with substance using peers. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 153169.Google Scholar
Cloninger, C. R., Bohman, M., & Sigvardsson, S. (1981). Inheritance of alcohol abuse: Cross-fostering analysis of adopted men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 861869.Google Scholar
Colder, C. R., Campbell, R. T., Ruel, E., Richardson, J. L., & Flay, B. R. (2002). A finite mixture model of growth trajectories of adolescent alcohol use: Predictors and consequences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 976985.Google Scholar
Colder, C. R., Chassin, L., Lee, M. R., & Villalta, I. K. (2010). Developmental perspectives: Affect and adolescent substance use. In Kassel, J. D. (Ed.), Substance abuse and emotion (pp. 109135). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 6, 117128.Google Scholar
Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Mudar, P. (1995). Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 9901005.Google Scholar
Costello, E. J., Sung, M., Worthman, C., & Angold, A. (2007). Pubertal maturation and the development of alcohol use and abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88, S50S59.Google Scholar
Creemers, H., Harakeh, Z., Dick, D., Meyers, J., Vollebergh, W., Ormel, J., et al. (2011). DRD2 and DRD4 in relation to regular alcohol and cannabis use among adolescents: Does parenting modify the impact of genetic vulnerability? The TRAILS study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 115, 3542.Google Scholar
Crews, F. T., Braun, C. J., Hoplight, B., Switzer, R. C., & Knapp, D. J. (2000). Binge ethanol consumption causes differential brain damage in young adolescent rats compared with adult rats: Alcohol effects on the fetus, brain, liver, and other organ systems. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 17121723.Google Scholar
Crews, F. T., & Vetreno, R. P. (2011). Addiction, adolescence, and innate immune gene induction. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2, 19.Google Scholar
Curran, P. J., Stice, E., & Chassin, L. (1997). The relation between adolescent and peer alcohol use: A longitudinal random coefficients model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 130140.Google Scholar
Curran, P. J., & Willoughby, M. J. (2003). Reconciling theoretical and statistical models of developmental processes. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 581612.Google Scholar
Deas, D. (2008). Evidence-based treatments for alcohol use disorders in adolescents. Pediatrics, 121S4, S348S354.Google Scholar
De Bellis, M. D. (2001). Developmental traumatology: The psychobiological development of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 539564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bellis, M. D., Clark, D. B., Beers, S. R., Soloff, P. H., Boring, A. M., Hall, J., et al. (2000). Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 737744.Google Scholar
Dick, D. M., Latendresse, S., & Riley, B. (2011). Incorporating genetics into your studies: A guide for social scientists. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., & Mustanski, B. S. (2006). Pubertal development and health-related behavior. In Pulkkinen, L., Kaprio, J., & Rose, R. J. (Eds.), Socioemotional development and health from adolescence to adulthood (pp. 108125). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dick, D. M., Rose, R. J., Viken, R. J., & Kaprio, J. (2000). Pubertal timing and substance use: Associations between and within families across late adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 36, 180189.Google Scholar
Dick, D. M., Smith, G., Olausson, P., Mitchell, S. H., Leeman, R. F., O'Malley, S. S., et al. (2010). Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addiction Biology, 15, 217226.Google Scholar
Dierker, L. C., Vesel, F., Sledjeski, E. M., Costello, D., & Perrine, N. (2007). Testing the dual pathway hypothesis to substance use in adolescence and young adulthood. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 87, 8393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donohue, K. F., Curtin, J. J., Patrick, C. J., & Lang, A. R. (2007). Intoxication level and emotional response. Emotion, 7, 103112.Google Scholar
Earleywine, M. (1994). Anticipated biphasic effects of alcohol vary with risk for alcoholism: A preliminary report. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 18, 711714.Google Scholar
Earleywine, M., & Martin, C. S. (1993). Anticipated stimulant and sedative effects of alcohol vary with dosage and limb of the blood alcohol curve. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 17, 135139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eggleston, E. P., Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (2004). Methodological sensitivities to latent class analysis of long-term criminal trajectories. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 20, 126.Google Scholar
Eiden, R. D., Edwards, E. P., & Leonard, K. E. (2007). A conceptual model for the development of externalizing behavior problems among kindergarten children of alcoholic families: Role of parenting and children's self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 43, 11871201.Google Scholar
Ellis, B., Del Guidice, M., Dishion, T., Figueredo, A., Gray, P., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2012). The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: Implications for science, policy, and practice. Developmental Psychology, 48, 598623.Google Scholar
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ennett, S., Bauman, K., Hussong, A., Faris, R., Foshee, V., & Cai, L. (2006). The peer context of adolescent substance use: Findings from social network analysis. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 159186.Google Scholar
Fenton, M., Geier, T., Keyes, K., Skodol, A., Grant, B., & Hasin, D. (2012). Combined role of child maltreatment, family history, and gender in the risk for alcohol dependence. Psychological Medicine, 10, 113.Google Scholar
Fowler, J., Settle, J., & Christakis, N. (2011). Correlated genotypes in friendship networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 19931997.Google Scholar
Fu, H., & Goldman, N. (1996). Incorporating health into models of marriage choice: Demographic and sociological perspectives. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 740758.Google Scholar
Ge, X., Jin, R., Natsuaki, M. N., Gibbons, F. X., Brody, G. H., Cutrona, C. E., et al. (2006). Pubertal maturation and early substance use risks among African American children. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20, 404414.Google Scholar
Gottfredson, N. C., & Hussong, A. M. (2011). Parental involvement protects against self-medication behaviors during the high school transition. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 12461252.Google Scholar
Gould, L. F., Hersh, M. A., & Hussong, A. M. (in press). Emotional distress may increase risk for self-medication and lower risk for mood-related drinking consequences in adolescents. International Journal of Emotional Education.Google Scholar
Graber, J. A., Seeley, J. R., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (2004). Is pubertal timing associated with psychopathology in young adulthood? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 718726.Google Scholar
Graber, J. A., & Sontag, L. M. (2009). Internalizing problems during adolescence. In Lerner, R. M. & Steinberg, L. (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Grant, B. F. (1997). Prevalence and correlates of alcohol use and DSM-IV alcohol dependence in the United States: Results of the National Longitudinal Epidemiologic Survey. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 58, 464473.Google Scholar
Grant, B. F., Harford, T. C., Dawson, D. A., Chou, P., Dufour, M., & Pickering, R. (1994). Prevalence of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United States, 1992. Alcohol Health & Research World, 18, 243248.Google Scholar
Grant, B. F., & Pickering, R. P. (1996). Comorbidity between DSM-IV alcohol and drug use disorders: Results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. Alcohol Health & Research World, 20, 6772.Google Scholar
Grant, B. F., Stinson, F. S., Dawson, D. A., Chou, S. P., Dufour, M. C., Compton, W., et al. (2004). Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independent mood and anxiety disorders: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 807816.Google Scholar
Grant, J., Scherrer, J., Lynskey, M., Lyons, M., Eisen, S., Tsuang, M., et al. (2006). Adolescent alcohol use is a risk factor for adult alcohol and drug dependence: Evidence from a twin design. Psychological Medicine, 36, 109118.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145173.Google Scholar
Haller, M., Handley, E., Chassin, L., & Bountress, K. (2010). Developmental cascades: Linking adolescent substance use, affiliation with substance use promoting peers, and academic achievement to adult substance use disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 899916.Google Scholar
Handley, E., Haller, M., Bountress, K., Beltran, I., Dandreaux, D., & Chassin, L. (2011). Executive functioning and sensation seeking as mediators of parental alcoholism effects on externalizing spectrum outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 528542.Google Scholar
Hanson, K. L., Medina, K. L., Padula, C. B., Tapert, S. F., & Brown, S. A. (2011). Impact of adolescent alcohol and drug use on neuropsychological functioning in young adulthood: 10-year outcomes. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 20, 135154.Google Scholar
Harden, K. P., Hill, J. E., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. (2008). Gene–environment correlation and interaction in peer effects on adolescent alcohol and tobacco use. Behavioral Genetics, 38, 339347.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, G. A., Quinn, H., Kashem, M. A., Matsumoto, I., & McGregor, I. S. (2009). Proteomic analysis demonstrates adolescent vulnerability to lasting hippocampal changes following chronic alcohol consumption. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 8694.Google Scholar
Hedeker, D., & Mermelstein, R. J. (2000). Analysis of longitudinal substance use outcomes using ordinal random-effects regression models. Addiction, 95, S381S394.Google Scholar
Helzer, J. E., Burnam, A., & McEnvoy, L. T. (1991). Alcohol abuse and dependence. In Robins, L. N. & Regier, D. A. (Eds.), Psychiatric disorders in American: The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study (pp. 81115). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hendershot, C., Lindren, K., Liang, T., & Hutchison, K. (2012). COMT and ALDH2 polymorphisms moderate associations of implicit drinking motives with alcohol use. Addiction Biology, 17.Google Scholar
Hersh, M. A., & Hussong, A. M. (2009). The association between observed parental emotion socialization and adolescent self-medication. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 493506.Google Scholar
Hertzog, C., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1987). Beyond autoregressive models: Some implications of the trait–state distinction for the structural modeling of developmental change. Child Development, 58, 93109.Google Scholar
Hicks, B., Durbin, C., Blonigen, D., Iacono, W., & McGue, M. (2012). Relationship between personality change and the onset and course of alcohol dependence in young adulthood. Addiction, 107, 540548.Google Scholar
Hill, J., Emery, R., Harden, K. P., Mendle, J., & Turkheimer, E. (2008). Alcohol use in adolescent twins and affiliation with substance using peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 8194.Google Scholar
Hingson, R., Zha, W., & Weitzman, E. (2009). Magnitude of and trends in alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24, 1998–2005. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 16S, 1220.Google Scholar
Houben, K., & Wiers, R. (2009). Response inhibition moderates the relationship between implicit associations and drinking behavior. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 626633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, M., Muthén, B., Schaffran, C., Griesler, P. C., & Kandel, D. B. (2008). Developmental trajectories of criteria of nicotine dependence in adolescence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 98, 94104.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M. (2007). Predictors of drinking immediacy following daily sadness: An application of survival analysis to experience sampling data. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 10541065.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., Bauer, D., & Chassin, L. (2008). Telescoped trajectories from alcohol initiation to disorder in children of alcoholic parents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 6378.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., & Chassin, L. (2002). Parent alcoholism and the leaving home transition. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 139157.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., & Chassin, L. (2004). Stress and coping among children of alcoholic parents through the young adult transition. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 9851006.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., Feagans-Gould, L. A., & Hersh, M. A. (2008). Conduct problems moderate self-medication and mood-related drinking consequences in adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69, 296307.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., Flora, D. B., Curran, P. J., Chassin, L. A., & Zucker, R. A. (2008). Defining risk heterogeneity for internalizing symptoms among children of alcoholic parents. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 165193.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., Galloway, C. A., & Feagans-Gould, L. A. (2005). Coping motives as a moderator of daily mood-drinking covariation. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 344353.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., Hicks, R. E., Levy, S. A., & Curran, P. J. (2001). Specifying the relations between affect and heavy alcohol use among young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 449461.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., Jones, D. J., Stein, G. L., Baucom, D. H., & Boeding, S. (2011). An internalizing pathway to alcohol use and disorder. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25, 390404.Google Scholar
Hussong, A. M., Wirth, R. J., Edwards, M. C., Curran, P. J., Chassin, L. A., & Zucker, R. A. (2007). Externalizing symptoms among children of alcoholic parents: Entry points for an antisocial pathway to alcoholism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 529542.Google Scholar
Iacono, W., Malone, S., & McGue, M. (2008). Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early onset addiction: Common and specific influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 325348.Google Scholar
Jackson, K. M. (2010). Progression through early drinking milestones in an adolescent treatment sample. Addiction, 105, 438449.Google Scholar
Jackson, K. M., Sher, K. J., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2005). Conjoint developmental trajectories of young adult alcohol and tobacco use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 612626.Google Scholar
Jellinek, E. M. (1960). The disease concept of alcoholism. New Brunswick, NJ: Hillhouse.Google Scholar
Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2012). Monitoring the Future national results on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 2011. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
Kaltiala-Heino, R., Koivisto, A., Marttunen, M., & Frojd, S. (2011). Pubertal timing and substance use in middle adolescence: A 2-year follow-up study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 12881301.Google Scholar
Kaplow, J. B., Curran, P. J., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2001). The prospective relation between dimensions of anxiety and the initiation of adolescent alcohol use. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 30, 316326.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Ferssizidis, P., Collins, R. L., & Muraven, M. (2010). Emotion differentiation as resilience against excessive alcohol use: An ecological momentary assessment in underage social drinkers. Psychological Science, 21, 13411347.Google Scholar
Kendler, K., Gardner, C., & Dick, D. (2011). Predicting alcohol consumption in adolescence from alcohol-specific and general externalizing genetic risk factors, key environmental exposures, and their interaction. Psychological Medicine, 41, 15071516.Google Scholar
Kendler, K., Schmitt, E., Aggen, S., & Prescott, C. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 674682.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593602.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 617627.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Crum, R. M., Warner, L. A., & Nelson, C. B. (1997). Lifetime co-occurrence of DSM-III-R alcohol abuse and dependence with other psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 313321.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 819.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 7887.Google Scholar
Knight, R. P. (1937). The dynamics and treatment of chronic alcohol addiction. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 1, 233250.Google Scholar
Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2008). Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 2953.Google Scholar
Kuerbis, A., & Sacco, P. (2012). The impact of retirement on the drinking patterns of older adults: A review. Addictive Behaviors, 37, 587595.Google Scholar
Lang, A., Patrick, C., & Stritzke, W. (1999). Alcohol and emotional response: A multidimensional–multilevel analysis. In Leonard, K. E. & Blane, H. T. (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism (2nd ed., pp. 328371). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Laucht, M., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A., Treutlein, J., Schmidt, M., Esser, G., et al. (2012). Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val(158) Met genotype, parenting practices, and adolescent alcohol use: Testing the differential susceptibility hypotheses. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 351359.Google Scholar
Lee, M. R., & Chassin, L. (in press). Maturing out of alcohol involvement: Transitions in latent drinking statuses from late adolescence to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology.Google Scholar
Lejuez, C. W., Magidson, J., Mitchell, S., Sinha, R., Stevens, M., & DeWit, H. (2010). Behavioral and biological indicators of impulsivity in the development of alcohol use, problems and disorders. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34, 13341345.Google Scholar
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., et al. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk-taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 7584.Google Scholar
Leonard, K. E., & Eiden, R. D. (2007). Marital and family processes in the context of alcohol use and alcohol disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 285310.Google Scholar
Leve, L., Harold, G., Ge, X., Neiderhiser, J., & Patterson, G. (2010). Refining intervention targets in family-based research: Lessons from quantitative behavioral genetics. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 11, 516526.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W., Oyama, O. N., & Meek, P. S. (1987). Greater reinforcement from alcohol for those at risk: Parental risk, personality risk, and sex. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 242253.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rhode, P., & Seeley, J. R. (1996). Alcohol consumption in high school adolescents: Frequency of use and dimensional structure of associated problems. Addiction, 91, 375390.Google Scholar
Litt, M. D., Cooney, N. L., & Morse, P. (2000). Reactivity to alcohol-related stimuli in the laboratory and in the field: Predictors of craving in treated alcoholics. Addiction, 95, 889900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, M., Handley, L., Leuthe, E., & Chassin, L. (2009). Substance use and early parenthood in a high risk sample. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 661682.Google Scholar
Littlefield, A. K., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2009). Is “maturing out” of problematic alcohol involvement related to personality change? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 360374.Google Scholar
Littlefield, A. K., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2010). Do changes in drinking motives mediate the relation between personality change and “maturing out” of problem drinking? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 93105.Google Scholar
Littlefield, A., Vergés, A., Wood, P., & Sher, K. J. (2012). Transactional models between personality and alcohol involvement: A further examination. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 778783.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491495.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Faden, V. B., Zucker, R. A., & Spear, L. P. (2008). Underage drinking: A developmental framework. Pediatrics, 121S4, S235S251.Google Scholar
Mayer, L. S., & Carroll, S. S. (1987). Testing for lagged, co-temporal, and total dependence in cross-lagged panel analysis. Sociological Methods and Research, 16, 187217.Google Scholar
McCarthy, D. E., Curtin, J. J., Piper, M. E., & Baker, T. B. (2010). Negative reinforcement: Possible clinical implications of an integrative model. In Kassel, J. (Ed.), Substance abuse and emotion (pp. 1542). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Meredith, W., & Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107122.Google Scholar
Mermelstein, R., Hedeker, D., & Weinstein, S. (2010). Ecological momentary assessment of mood-smoking relationships in adolescent smokers. In Kassel, J. D. (Ed.), Substance abuse and emotion (pp. 217236). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Mezzich, A., Tarter, R., Kirisci, L., Feske, U., Day, B., & Gao, Z. (2007). Reciprocal influence of parent discipline and child's behavior on risk for substance use disorder: A 9-year prospective study. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 33, 851867.Google Scholar
Miller-Johnson, S., Lochman, J. E., Coie, J. D., Terry, R., & Hyman, C. (1998). Comorbidity of conduct and depressive problems at sixth grade: Substance use outcomes across adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 221232.Google Scholar
Monti, P. M., Miranda, R., Nixon, K., Sher, K. J., Swartzwelder, H. S., Tapert, S. F., et al. (2005). Adolescence: Booze, brains and behavior. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 207220.Google Scholar
Morales, M., Varlinskaya, E. I., & Spear, L. P. (2011). Age differences in the expression of acute and chronic tolerance to ethanol in male and female rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 16141624.Google Scholar
Morean, M. E., & Corbin, W. R. (2010). Subjective response to alcohol: A critical review of the literature. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34, 385395.Google Scholar
Muthen, B. O. (2001). Second-generation structural equation modeling with a combination of categorical and continuous latent variables: New opportunities for latent class/latent growth modeling. In Sayer, A. & Collins, L. (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 291322). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Muthen, B. O. (2003). Statistical and substantive checking in growth mixture modeling: Comment on Bauer and Curran (2003). Psychological Methods, 8, 369377.Google Scholar
Muthen, B., & Shedden, K. (1999). Finite mixture modeling with mixture outcomes using the EM algorithm. Biometrics, 55, 463469.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semi-parametric, group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139157.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. (2004). Response to “Methodological sensitivities to latent class analysis of long-term criminal trajectories.” Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 20, 2735.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Analyzing developmental trajectories of distinct but related behaviors: A group-based method. Psychological Methods, 6, 1834.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2005). Developmental trajectory groups: Fact or a useful statistical fiction? Criminology, 43, 873904.Google Scholar
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2000, June). 10th Special report to the U.S. Congress on alcohol and health (NIH Publication No. 00-1583). Bethesda, MD: NIH.Google Scholar
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2006). Alcohol across the lifespan: Five-year strategic plan FY07–11. Bethesda, MD: NIH.Google Scholar
Nelson, L. J., Rubin, K. H., & Fox, N. A. (2005). Social withdrawal, observed peer acceptance, and the development of self-perceptions in children ages 4 to 7 years. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20, 185200.Google Scholar
Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1986). Drug use, educational aspirations, and work force involvement: The transition from adolescence to young adulthood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 303321.Google Scholar
Newlin, D. B., & Thomson, J. B. (1990). Alcohol challenge with sons of alcoholics: A critical review and analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 383402.Google Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220246.Google Scholar
Nurnberger, J. I., Foroud, T., Flury, L., Meyer, E. T., & Wiegand, R. (2002). Is there a genetic relationship between alcoholism and depression? Alcohol Research & Health, 26, 233239.Google Scholar
O'Connor, R. M., Lopez-Vergara, H. I., & Colder, C. R. (2012). Implicit cognition and substance use: The role of controlled and automatic processes in children. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 73, 134143.Google Scholar
Odgers, C. L., Caspi, A., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H. L., et al. (2007). Prediction of differential adult health burden by conduct problem subtypes in males. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 476484.Google Scholar
O'Malley, P. M. (2004). Maturing out of problematic alcohol use. Alcohol Research and Health, 28, 202204.Google Scholar
O'Neill, S. E., & Sher, K. J. (2000). Physiological alcohol dependence symptoms in early adulthood: A longitudinal perspective. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8, 493508.Google Scholar
Park, A., Sher, K., Todorov, A., & Heath, A. (2011). Interaction between the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism and proximal and distal environments in alcohol dependence during emerging and young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 585595.Google Scholar
Paus, T. (2005). Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 6068.Google Scholar
Peeters, M., Wiers, R., Monshouwer, K., Van de Schoot, R., Janssen, T., & Vollebergh, W. (2012). Automatic processes in at-risk adolescents: The role of alcohol-approach tendencies and response inhibition in drinking behavior. Addiction, 107, 19391946.Google Scholar
Pieters, S., Van Der Vorst, H., Burk, W., Schoenmakers, T., Van Den Wildenberg, E., Smeets, H., et al. (2011). The effect of the OPRM1 and DRD4 polymorphisms on the relation between attentional bias and alcohol use in adolescence and young adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 591599.Google Scholar
Quinn, P. D., & Fromme, K. (2011). Subjective response to alcohol challenge: A quantitative review. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 35, 17591770.Google Scholar
Quinn, P. D., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Differential changes in impulsivity and sensation seeking and the escalation of substance use from adolescence to early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 223239.Google Scholar
Quinn, P. D., Stappenbeck, C. A., & Fromme, K. (2011). Collegiate heavy drinking prospectively predicts change in sensation seeking and impulsivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 543556.Google Scholar
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Reimuller, A., Shadur, J., & Hussong, A. M. (2011). Parental social support as a moderator of self-medication in adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 203208.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E, & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 125.Google ScholarPubMed
Robins, L. N., & Price, R. K. (1991). Adult disorders predicted by childhood conduct problems: Results from the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area project. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 54, 116132.Google Scholar
Roefs, A., Huiding, J., Smulders, F., MacLeod, C., de Jong, P., Wiers, R., et al. (2011). Implicit measures of association in psychopathology research. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 149193.Google Scholar
Rosenquist, J. N., Murabito, J., Fowler, J., & Christakis, N. (2010). The spread of alcohol consumption in a large social network. Annals of Internal Medicine, 152, 425441.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. (1991). Conceptualizing developmental pathways to internalizing disorders in childhood. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 23, 300317.Google Scholar
Saraceno, L., Munafó, M., Heron, J., Craddock, N., & Van Den Bree, M. B. M. (2009). Genetic and non-genetic influences on the development of co-occurring alcohol problem use and internalizing symptomatology in adolescence: A review. Addiction, 104, 11001121.Google Scholar
Sayette, M., Creswell, K., Dimhoff, J., Fairairn, C., Cohn, J., Heckman, B., et al. (2012). Alcohol and group formation: A multimodal investigation of the effects of alcohol on emotion and social bonding. Psychological Science, 23, 869878.Google Scholar
Schuckit, M. A. (1984). Subjective responses to alcohol in sons of alcoholics and control subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 879884.Google Scholar
Schuckit, M. A., & Smith, T. L. (1996). An 8-year follow-up of 450 sons of alcoholic and control subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 202210.Google Scholar
Schulenberg, J., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Wadsworth, K. N., & Johnston, L. D. (1996). Getting drunk and growing up: Trajectories of frequent binge drinking during the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 57, 289304.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J. (1987). Stress-response dampening. In Leonard, K. E. & Blane, H. T. (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism (pp. 227271). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J. (1991). Children of alcoholics: A critical appraisal of theory and research. University of Chicago Press: Chicago.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J. (1993). Children of alcoholics and the intergenerational transmission of alcoholism: A biopsychosocial perspective. In Baer, J. S., Marlatt, G. A., & Joseph, R. J. (Eds.), Addictive behaviors across the life span: Prevention, treatment, and policy issues (pp. 333). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., Peuser, K., Erickson, D. J., & Wood, M. D. (2007). Stress-response dampening effects of alcohol: Attention as a mediator and moderator. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 362377.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., Dick, D. M., Crabbe, J. C., Hutchison, K. E., O'Malley, S. S., & Heath, A. C. (2010). Review: Consilient research approaches in studying Gene × Environment interactions in alcohol research. Addiction Biology, 15, 200216.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., & Gotham, H. J. (1999). Pathological alcohol involvement: A developmental disorder of young adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 933956.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., Gotham, H. J., & Watson, A. L. (2004). Trajectories of dynamic predictors of disorder: Their meanings and implications. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 825856.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., & Grekin, E. R. (2007). Alcohol and affect regulation. In Gross, J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 560580). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., Jackson, K. M., & Steinley, D. (2011). Alcohol use trajectories and the ubiquitous cat's cradle: Cause for concern? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 322335.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., & Wood, M. D. (2005). Subjective effects of alcohol: II. Individual differences. In Earleywine, M. (Ed.), Mind-altering drugs: The science of subjective experience (pp. 135153). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sher, K. J., Wood, M. D., Richardson, A. E., & Jackson, K. M. (2005). Subjective effects of alcohol: I. Effects of the drink and drinking context. In Earleywine, M. (Ed.), Mind-altering drugs: Scientific evidence for subjective experience (pp. 86134). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shoal, G. D., Castaneda, J. O., & Giancola, P. R. (2005). Worry moderates the relation between negative affectivity and affect-related substance use in adolescent males: A prospective study of maladaptive emotional self-regulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 475485.Google Scholar
Sijtsema, J. J., Lindenberg, S. M., & Veenstra, R. (2010). Do they get what they want or are they stuck with what they can get? Testing homophily against default selection for friendships of highly aggressive boys. The TRAILS study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 803813.Google Scholar
Simons, J. S., Gaher, R. M., Oliver, M. N. I., Bush, J. A., & Palmer, M. A. (2005). An experience sampling study of associations between affect and alcohol use and problems among college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 459469.Google Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 417463.Google Scholar
Spear, L. P., & Varlinskaya, E. I. (2005). Adolescence: Alcohol sensitivity, tolerance, and intake. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 17, 143159.Google Scholar
Spoth, R., Greenberg, M. T., & Turrisi, R. (2008). Preventive interventions addressing underage drinking: State of the evidence and steps toward public health impact. Pediatrics, S121, S311S336.Google Scholar
Stacy, A., & Wiers, R. (2010). Implicit cognition and addiction: A tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 551575.Google Scholar
Stattin, H., & Magnusson, D. (1990). Pubertal maturation in female development: Vol 2. Paths through Life. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45, 921933.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78106.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Scott, E. (2003). Less guilt by reason of adolescence: Developmental immaturity, diminished responsibility, and the juvenile death penalty. American Psychologist, 58, 10091018.Google Scholar
Swartzwelder, H. S., Wilson, W. A., & Tayyeb, M. I. (1995). Age-dependent inhibition of long-term potentiation by ethanol in immature versus mature hippocampus. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 19, 14801485.Google Scholar
Taga, K. A., Markey, C. N., & Friedman, H. S. (2006). A longitudinal investigation of associations between boys’ pubertal timing and adult behavioral health and well-being. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 401411.Google Scholar
Tapert, S. F., Brown, G. G., Baratta, M. V., & Brown, S. A. (2004). fMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women. Addictive Behavior, 29, 3350.Google Scholar
Thush, C., Wiers, R., Ames, S., Grenard, J., Susman, S., & Stacy, A. (2008). Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 94, 116124.Google Scholar
Todd, M., Armeli, S., Tennen, H., Carney, M. A., & Affleck, G. (2003). A daily diary validity test of drinking to cope measures. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17, 303311.Google Scholar
Trull, T., Sher, K. J., Minks-Brown, C., Durbin, J., & Burr, R. (2000). Borderline personality disorder and substance use disorders: A review and integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 235253.Google Scholar
Van der Zwaluw, C., Engels, R., Vermulst, A., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J., Verkes, R. et al. . (2010). Interaction between dopamine D2 receptor genotype and parental rule-setting in adolescent alcohol use: Evidence for a gene-parenting interaction. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 727735.Google Scholar
Vengeliene, V., Bilbao, A., Molander, A., & Spanagel, R. (2008). Neuropharmacology of alcohol addiction. British Journal of Pharmacology, 154, 299315.Google Scholar
Vergés, A., Jackson, K., Bucholz, K., Grant, J., Trull, T., Wood, P., et al. (2012). Deconstructing the age-prevalence curve of alcohol dependence: Why “maturing out” is only a small piece of the puzzle. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 511523.Google Scholar
Vergés, A., Littlefield, A. K., & Sher, K. J. (2011). Did lifetime rates of alcohol use disorders increase by 67% in 10 years? A comparison of NLAES and NESARC. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 868877.Google Scholar
Véronneau, M., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Dishion, T. J., & Tremblay, R. E. (2010). Transactional analysis of the reciprocal links between peer experiences and academic achievement from middle childhood to early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 46, 773790.Google Scholar
Wardell, J. D., O'Connor, R. M., Read, J. P., & Colder, C. R. (2012). Behavioral approach system moderates the prospective association between the behavioral inhibition system and alcohol outcomes in college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 73, 10281036.Google Scholar
Westling, E., Andrews, J. A., Hampson, S. E., & Peterson, M. (2008). Pubertal timing and substance use: The effects of gender, parental monitoring and deviant peers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 555563.Google Scholar
White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles, D. J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). Measuring impulsivity and examining its relationship to delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 192205.Google Scholar
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669689.Google Scholar
Wiers, R., Ames, L., Hofmann, W., Krank, M., & Stacy, A. (2010). Impulsivity, impulsive and reflective processes and the development of alcohol use and misuse in adolescents and young adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 144.Google Scholar
Wikler, A. (1948). Recent progress in research on the neurophysiologic basis of morphine addiction. American Journal of Psychiatry, 105, 329338.Google Scholar
Wills, T. A., Sandy, J. M., Yaeger, A. M., Cleary, S. D., & Shinar, O. (2001). Coping dimensions, life stress, and adolescent substance use: A latent growth analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 309323.Google Scholar
Wills, T. A., Vaccaro, D., McNamara, G., & Hirky, A. E. (1996). Escalated substance use: A longitudinal grouping analysis from early to middle adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 166180.Google Scholar
Winters, K. C., Martin, C. S., & Chung, T. (2011). Substance use disorders in DSM-V when applied to adolescents. Addiction, 106, 882884.Google Scholar
Wohlwill, J. F. (1991). Relations between method and theory in developmental research: A partial-isomorphism view. Annals of Theoretical Psychology, 7, 91138.Google Scholar
Yamaguchi, K., & Kandel, D. (1985). On the resolution of role incompatibility: Life event history analysis of family roles and marijuana use. American Journal of Sociology, 90, 12841325.Google Scholar
Young-Wolff, K. C., Enoch, M., & Prescott, C. A. (2011). The influence of gene–environment interactions on alcohol consumption and alcohol use disorders: A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 800816.Google Scholar
Zeigler, D. W., Wang, C. C., Yoast, R. A., Dickinson, B. D., McCaffree, M. A., Robinowitz, C. B., et al. (2005). The neurocognitive effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Preventive Medicine, 40, 2332.Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Guo, X., Saitz, R., Levy, D., Sartini, E., Niu, J., et al. (2008). Secular trends in alcohol consumption over 50 years: The Framingham Study. American Journal of Medicine, 121, 695701.Google Scholar
Zucker, R. A. (1986). The four alcoholisms: A developmental account of the etiologic process. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 34, 2783.Google Scholar
Zucker, R. A. (2006). Alcohol use and the alcohol use disorders: A developmental–biopsychosocial systems formulation covering the life course. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 620656). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Zucker, R. A., Donovan, J., Masten, A., Mattson, M., & Moss, H. B. (2008). Early developmental processes and the continuity of risk for underage drinking and problem drinking. Pediatrics, 121(Suppl. 4), S252S272.Google Scholar