Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T00:16:21.875Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural development and psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2018

José M. Causadias*
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
Dante Cicchetti
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: José M. Causadias, T. Denny Sanford School of Social and Family Dynamics, Arizona State University, Cowden Family Resources Building, 850 South Cady Mall, Tempe, AZ 85281; E-mail: jose.causadias@asu.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Culture plays a pivotal role in adaptive and maladaptive development. However, culture remains disconnected from theory, research, training, assessment, and interventions in developmental psychopathology, limiting our understanding of the genesis and epigenesis of mental health. Cultural development and psychopathology research can help overcome this limitation by focusing on the elucidation of cultural risk, protective, and promotive factors, at the individual and social levels, that initiate, derail, or maintain trajectories of normal and abnormal behavior. The goal of this Special Issue is to showcase research on the association between culture, development, and psychopathology that investigates equifinality and multifinality in cultural development, the interplay between culture and biology, cultural assessment and interventions, and cultural differences and similarities.

Type
Special Issue Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

In its pursuit of understanding development in all its complexity, developmental psychopathology offers a profoundly humanistic view of individual agency. Individuals develop adaptation and maladaptation over time in response to previous experiences, current circumstances, and social and personal resources (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984, Reference Cicchetti1993; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984). No longer should individuals be studied in isolation, decontextualized, in a mechanistic fashion, or seen as bounded by essential features (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1990, Reference Sroufe1997). Emergence, continuity, and change in the development of normal and abnormal behavior can only be captured reliably by considering all levels of human experience (Cicchetti & Dawson, Reference Cicchetti and Dawson2002; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe and Masten2007). Thus, research should incorporate a multiple levels of analysis approach and an interdisciplinary perspective (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2015). However, social, emotional, cognitive, and neurobiological processes are not only levels of analysis but also core features of human development. The same is true for culture (Rogoff, Reference Rogoff2003).

Yet, culture remains disconnected from theory, research, training, assessment, and interventions in developmental psychopathology, limiting our understanding of the genesis and epigenesis of mental health. Neglecting culture as a central dimension of human development threatens the validity of developmental psychopathology as a field of science and as a research paradigm. The pervasive disregard of culture is a roadblock for the progression of developmental psychology (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Lamberty, Jenkins, Pipes McAdoo, Crnic, Wasik and Vázquez García1996) and developmental psychopathology (García Coll, Akerman, & Cicchetti, Reference García Coll, Akerman and Cicchetti2000). Moreover, cultural experiences, influences, and processes play an important role in the emergence of adaptation and maladaptation (Serafica & Vargas, Reference Serafica, Vargas, Cicchetti and Cohen2006). Ultimately, ignoring the role of culture not only diminishes the scientific potential of developmental psychopathology but also betrays its humanistic promise of portraying human development and agency in all its richness. After all, culture is what makes humans unique from all other species (Whiten, Hinde, Laland, & Stringer, Reference Whiten, Hinde, Laland and Stringer2011).

Cultural Development and Psychopathology

In response to these challenges, this Special Issue focuses on cultural development and psychopathology, the emerging field that aims at advancing theory, research, assessment, and interventions that improves our understanding of the intersection of culture, development, and psychopathology. Cultural development and psychopathology is concerned with understanding the cultural risk, protective, and promotive factors, operating at multiple levels, that initiate, contribute, and maintain trajectories of normal and abnormal behavior (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). Its central aim is to integrate culture and developmental psychopathology in a way that advances our understanding of human development. It promotes the study of the cultural processes related to developmental continuity and change, as well as elucidating the mechanisms and outcomes involved in the resolution of stage-salient developmental tasks (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013).

Cultural development and psychopathology acknowledges the contentious and multifaceted nature of the term culture, defined as a system of behaviors, symbols, values, and ideals that are generated and shared by a community, continued and changed from one generation to the next (Adams & Markus, Reference Adams, Markus, Schaller and Crandall2004; Cohen, Reference Cohen2009). Culture is not restricted to the experience of racial/ethnic minorities (henceforth, minorities), as it is commonly misunderstood (Betancourt & López, Reference Betancourt and López1993; Causadias, Vitriol, & Atkin, Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018a, Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018b), but is a normative dimension of human development (Jensen, Reference Jensen2012; Rogoff, Reference Rogoff2003; Shweder et al., Reference Shweder, Goodnow, Hatano, LeVine, Markus, Miller, Damon and Lerner2006). All humans are cultural beings because they are immersed in systems of practices and symbols, even if they differ in content. For instance, the use of language is present in all cultural, ethnic, and racial groups, even if they vary in important ways (Chomsky, Reference Chomsky2014).

Consistent with developmental psychopathology's emphasis on multiple levels of analysis (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2015), culture is better understood as unfolding both at the individual and the social levels (Kitayama & Uskul, Reference Kitayama and Uskul2011). The recognition of this dual nature of culture can help elucidate the cultural mechanisms that shape adaptation and maladaptation, and contribute to the etiology, expression, course, remission, or persistence of mental illness (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). Culture operates at the social level because it provides meaning and goals to interpersonal, group, institutional, societal, and national systems. Culture is the software that runs our social hardware. It goes beyond single individuals because it is shared, created, and transmitted by a community (Rogoff, Reference Rogoff2003; Shweder et al., Reference Shweder, Goodnow, Hatano, LeVine, Markus, Miller, Damon and Lerner2006). This social dimension of culture is demonstrated in its connection to ethnicity and race, two concepts employed to represent group categorizations based on real or perceived commonalities, the social hierarchies these taxonomies reflect and enforce, and the biases and stereotypes they engender (Causadias et al., Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018a). For these reasons, culture is also related to issues of diversity and inclusion, to the problem of under- and misrepresentation of minorities in developmental sciences (Causadias et al., Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018b; García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Akerman and Cicchetti2000).

Culture also functions at the individual level, as everyday participation in cultural communities’ shapes social and cognitive development (Rogoff, Reference Rogoff2003). Culture influences individuals as they engage directly or indirectly with cultural organizations, rituals, and ideals (Adams & Markus, Reference Adams, Markus, Schaller and Crandall2004). Cultural socialization in the family, schools, peer groups, and neighborhoods shapes the way individuals see themselves and how they deal with developmental challenges (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). This comprehensive socialization into systems of meaning, together with individual predispositions and agency, shapes the emergence of the cultural self, a cohesive organization that integrates ethnic–racial identity, social and gender roles, group belonging, moral commitments, cultural patterns of emotional display and regulation, learning styles, and problem-solving strategies (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). This dual nature of culture, functioning at the social and individual levels, is embodied in cultural risk, protective, and promotive factors, as organizing forces in normal and abnormal development. Because these cultural factors are not static, but dynamic and subject to change, they are consistent with developmental psychopathology's probabilistic understanding of development (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1997).

Cultural risk factors are the processes that increase the likelihood of starting or sustaining maladaptive developmental trajectories (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). What makes these processes cultural is that they emerge from values, practices, symbols, and institutions deeply embedded in particular societies. For instance, racism in the United States exemplifies a cultural risk factor because it is a cohesive system of values, symbols, and practices that are learned and transmitted from one generation to the next, and serve the purpose of subordinating minorities to Whites (Bonilla-Silva, Reference Bonilla-Silva2017). At the social level, it is perpetuated in laws, traditions, and institutions. At the individual level, it has a damaging effect in the development of minority youth exposed to its enactment through racial discrimination (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Lamberty, Jenkins, Pipes McAdoo, Crnic, Wasik and Vázquez García1996).

Cultural protective factors are the processes that diminish the likelihood of developing psychopathology by buffering against adversity and risk (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). For example, cultural coping strategies may ameliorate the effects of racial discrimination, including cultural pride (Gaylord-Harden, Burrow, & Cunningham, Reference Gaylord-Harden, Burrow and Cunningham2012), community support (Cooper, Brown, Metzger, Clinton, & Guthrie, Reference Cooper, Brown, Metzger, Clinton and Guthrie2013), and spirituality (Constantine, Donnelly, & Myers, Reference Constantine, Donnelly and Myers2002). These cultural protective factors challenge deficit models of minorities in which values, rituals, traditions, and community practices are portrayed as a source of risk although they often account for resilient functioning in the face of adversity (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Lamberty, Jenkins, Pipes McAdoo, Crnic, Wasik and Vázquez García1996, Reference García Coll, Akerman and Cicchetti2000).

Cultural promotive factors are the processes that increase the probability of initiating and maintaining adaptive developmental trajectories (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). While cultural risk and protective factors focus on psychopathology, cultural promotive factors relate to the development of health and well-being. These processes highlight that health is not merely the absence of illness, but competent adaptation to developmental and ecological demands. For example, family orientation, positive parenting, community practices, and bicultural adaptation among Latinos in the United States are related to well-being (Fuller & García Coll, Reference Fuller and García Coll2010). However, cultural risk, protective, and promotive factors are dynamic and context dependent. A cultural protective factor at one age and setting can be protective, but it can become risky at another.

The goal of this Special Issue is to showcase articles that advance cultural development and psychopathology as an emerging field that studies the multiple ways in which culture influences adaptive and maladaptive development. These papers focus on four major themes: equifinality and multifinality in cultural development, the interplay between culture and biology, cultural assessment and interventions, and cultural differences and similarities.

Equifinality and Multifinality in Cultural Development

Several articles in this Special Issue investigate how culture is related to diversity in developmental processes and outcomes, one of the hallmarks of developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984). They examine the multiplicity of contributing factors to normal or abnormal outcomes, how they vary in their impact across individuals, and the numerous pathways to any specific developmental outcome (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996). These articles illustrate equifinality (i.e., the same outcome may be reached from different early cultural experiences and through various cultural processes) and multifinality (i.e., different developmental outcomes may result from similar cultural experiences and processes) in cultural development.

Cross, Hoffman, Constante, and Rivas-Drake (Reference Cross, Hoffman, Constante and Rivas-Drake2018) investigated the concurrent and prospective associations of ethnic–racial identity content (i.e., centrality, private regard, and public regard) and depressive symptomatology with a longitudinal study of 148 Latino adolescents. Results showed that higher ethnic–racial centrality at Waves 1 and 2 predicted fewer depressive symptoms at Waves 2 and 3, respectively. Thus, ethnic–racial identity content may function as a cultural protective factor linked to lower depressive symptoms among Latino adolescents.

Gonzales, Johnson, et al. (Reference Gonzales, Johnson, Shirtcliff, Tein, Eskenazi and Deardorff2018) studied the role of bicultural adaptation, familism, and family conflict in Mexican American adolescents’ cortisol reactivity. They assessed Mexican and Anglo cultural orientations, prospective associations between their patterns of bicultural orientation, and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis cortisol reactivity to an adapted Trier Social Stress Test in a sample 264 Mexican American adolescents at ages 12 and 14. Bicultural individuals showed high cortisol responsivity, which may be adaptive in the context of a strong acute stressor, whereas individuals endorsing only high levels of Anglo orientation had a blunted cortisol response.

Updegraff, Umaña-Taylor, Zeiders, Bravo, and Jahromi (Reference Updegraff, Umaña-Taylor, Zeiders, Bravo and Jahromi2018) studied the development of familism in the transition to adolescent motherhood with a sample of 191 Mexican-origin pregnant adolescents who were having their first child. These individuals completed interviews during pregnancy and annually for 5 years after (Waves 1 through 6). Familism values were related to adolescent–mother figure warmth and conflict, coparenting communication, and social support from mother figures; however, no associations emerged for coparental conflict, adolescents’ depressive symptoms, or self-esteem.

Gonzales, Knight, and colleagues (Reference Gonzales, Knight, Gunn, Tein, Tanaka and White2018) tested the cultural gap-distress theory, which predicts increased parent–adolescent conflict and adolescent psychopathology over time when adolescents become less aligned with Mexican heritage values compared to their parents. With a sample of 749 families, they examined parallel trajectories of adolescents’ and both their mothers’ and fathers’ heritage cultural values, and their associations with parent–adolescent conflict and psychopathology. Of the six profiles that emerged across dyads, only one was consistent with the hypothesized problem gap pattern.

Several papers included in the Special Issue focused on racism, racial discrimination, racial segregation, and unfair treatment as cultural risk factors in development. Gibbons, Fleischli, Gerrard, and Simons (Reference Gibbons, Fleischli, Gerrard and Simons2018) investigated the prospective relations between racial discrimination and subsequent negative affect (anger and depressive symptoms) and smoking in a sample of 889 African American children. Racial discrimination at Wave 1 predicted smoking at Wave 6, controlling for multiple factors. Cultural socialization was associated with lower rates of adolescent smoking, and it buffered the relation between racial discrimination and anger.

Ong and Burrow (Reference Ong and Burrow2018) studied whether individual differences in affective reactivity in response to daily racial discrimination, using 14-day diary reports, predicted subsequent depressive symptoms among 174 African American graduate and postgraduate students. Depressive symptoms were measured at two assessment points one year apart. Participants with increases in negative affect on days when racial discrimination occurred had higher depressive symptoms one year later.

Juang, Shen, Costigan, and Hou (Reference Juang, Shen, Costigan and Hou2018) investigated the link between racial discrimination and adjustment across adolescence, gender, nativity, and region with a sample of 498 Chinese-heritage youth from the United States and Canada. Racial discrimination was consistently associated with poorer adjustment across all ages, but these associations were stronger in early adolescence for males compared to females, in middle adolescence for first-generation compared to second-generation adolescents, and in early adolescence for US compared to Canadian youth.

White, Zeiders, and Safa (Reference White, Zeiders and Safa2018) studied the role of racial residential segregation on the development of internalizing, externalizing, prosocial behaviors, and ethnic–racial identity resolution in a sample of 733 Mexican-origin adolescents assessed at three time points. Higher neighborhood Latino concentration during early adolescence predicted greater ethnic–racial identity exploration and lower discrimination from peers in middle adolescence. These benefits, in turn, were associated with lower externalizing and internalizing and higher ethnic–racial identity resolution, and prosocial behaviors in late adolescence.

Finally, Lam and colleagues (Reference Lam, Miller, Chiang, Levine, Le, Shalowitz and Chen2018) examined whether perceived social status and unfair treatment moderated the link between shift-and-persist (shifting the self to stressors while finding meaning) and asthma symptoms in a sample of 308 youth who completed 2 weeks of daily diaries. Parents reported on perceived family social status. Shift-and-persist was associated with better asthma profiles only among youth with lower parent-reported perceived social status and only among youth who experienced more unfair treatment.

Investigating the Interplay of Culture and Biology in Development

Another main avenue to advance the emerging field of cultural development and psychopathology is through research on culture and biology interplay, or how culture, biology, and environments influence one another and shape development (Causadias, Telzer, & Gonzales, Reference Causadias, Telzer, Gonzales, Causadias, Telzer and Gonzales2018; Causadias, Telzer, & Lee, Reference Causadias, Telzer and Lee2017). Consistent with developmental psychopathology, this approach underlines complex and dynamic relationships among various areas of functioning (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013). Research on culture and biology interplay has been structured into different domains that focus on the relationship between cultural processes and multiple levels of analysis, including cultural genomics, cultural neurobiology, and cultural neuroscience.

Several articles in this Special Issue focus on cultural genomics, or the numerous ways in which cultural experiences are influenced by, affect, and covary with the genome and the environment to shape behavior and cognition at the social, developmental, and evolutionary levels (Causadias & Korous, Reference Causadias, Korous, Causadias, Telzer and Gonzales2018; Moya & Henrich, Reference Moya and Henrich2016). Using a sample of 479 Mexican American and European American adolescents, Elam, Chassin, and Pandika (Reference Elam, Chassin and Pandika2018) examined polygenic risk scores for aggression in evocative gene–environment correlations and family cohesion and alcohol use. More family cohesion was associated with lower levels of alcohol use in early adulthood, and this link was stronger for Mexican American than for European American adolescents.

Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Dishion, Shaw, and Wilson (Reference Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Dishion, Shaw and Wilson2018) investigated the role of genes in sensitivity to the effects of the Family Check-Up intervention on children's internalizing symptoms. Participants were a diverse sample of 515 youth and their families drawn from a multisite randomized prevention trial followed longitudinally. More sensitive children assigned to treatment had fewer internalizing symptoms than sensitive children assigned to the control condition.

Su, Kuo, Meyers, Guy, and Dick (Reference Su, Kuo, Meyers, Guy and Dick2018) examined if polygenic risk for alcohol problems, peer deviance, and interpersonal trauma influenced trajectories of alcohol use disorder symptoms in a sample of 1,119 African American students across the college years. Polygenic risk did not predict trajectory of alcohol use. While peer deviance and interpersonal trauma were associated with more alcohol use disorder symptoms across college years, these effects were not moderated by either alcohol dependence polygenic risk scores or family history of alcohol problems.

Lehrner and Yehuda (Reference Lehrner and Yehuda2018) reviewed evidence on the intergenerational transmission of cultural trauma through epigenetic inheritance. They described epigenetics as the pathway through which environmental influences direct transcriptional activity and the expression or suppression of genes. They discussed challenges facing research on cultural trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder, and potential epigenetic mechanisms for transmission.

Another domain of research on culture and biology interplay represented in this Special Issue is cultural neurobiology, or the study of the transactions among cultural processes and stress-sensitive neurobiology across development, including the autonomic nervous system, the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, and the immune system (Doane, Sladek, & Adam, Reference Doane, Sladek, Adam, Causadias, Telzer and Gonzales2018). Doane, Sladek, Breitenstein, and colleagues (Reference Doane, Sladek, Breitenstein, Park, Castro and Kennedy2018) investigated whether familial influences were associated with indicators of typical physiological stress processes with a sample of 209 Latino adolescents. They examined familism values, perceptions of parent support, and daily family assistance behaviors in relation to hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis diurnal patterns, indexed by salivary cortisol five times a day for three days. Parental support was related with greater cortisol awakening responses, whereas familism values were not associated with diurnal cortisol patterns.

Lei, Beach, and Simons (Reference Lei, Beach and Simons2018) tested the degree to which neighborhood characteristics shape accelerated cardiometabolic aging with a longitudinal sample of 408 African Americans assessed from ages 18 to 29. Accelerated aging mediated the link between neighborhood disadvantage and chronic illness, even after adjusting for neighborhood selection effects. However, neighborhood collective efficacy buffered the link between neighborhood disadvantage and biological aging.

Hill and Hoggard (Reference Hill and Hoggard2018) studied the influence of rumination on the relationship between race-related stress and depressive symptoms in a sample of 69 young adult African American women. They also tested the moderating effects of John Henryism, a form of persistent and determined goal striving, and vagally mediated heart rate variability, a biomarker of coping. Race-related stress was associated with depressive symptoms through rumination, and both John Henryism and heart rate variability moderated the link between stress and rumination.

The third domain of research culture and biology interplay represented in this Special Issue is cultural neuroscience, the study of the interactions between culture, psychological processes, and the brain through neuroimaging techniques and other methods (Lin & Telzer, Reference Lin, Telzer, Causadias, Telzer and Gonzales2018). Muscatell, McCormick, and Telzer (Reference Muscatell, McCormick and Telzer2018) studied subjective social status and neural processing of race in a sample of 23 Mexican American adolescents. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging while they viewed Black and White faces in a standard labeling task, adolescents rated their subjective social status in American society. They found a negative link between subjective social status and neural responses in the amygdala, fusiform face area, and medial prefrontal cortex when adolescents viewed Black in contrast to White faces, suggesting enhanced salience of race for these youth.

Assessing and Intervening in Cultural Development

A third avenue to advance cultural development and psychopathology presented in this Special Issue is assessment and interventions in cultural development. Culture is often inferred based on demographic characteristics and approached cross-sectionally as a fixed and static influence (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013), failing to capture how cultural processes develop over time (Rogoff, Reference Rogoff2003). This surface-level approach to culture may also explain the paucity in interventions that target cultural risk, protective, and promotive factors, and the individual and social levels. Translating this research into interventions that impact the development of psychopathology is a major challenge in the field (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2006, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009). Several articles showcased in this Special Issue address these issues.

Meca, Schwartz, Martinez, and McClure (Reference Meca, Schwartz, Martinez and McClure2018) employed a three-year longitudinal data set of 216 immigrant Latino youth to examine the psychometric properties of the Bicultural Involvement Questionnaire—Short Version and the Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans—II. They reported factor structures for these measures that differed from their hypothesized structure, as well as developmental divergence between them and indices of psychopathology.

Knight, Safa, and White (Reference Knight, Safa and White2018) proposed a developmental and contextual framework of multiple psychological dimensions and social identities to advance research on the role of cultural orientation in development and psychopathology. They emphasize how reliable and valid measures of cultural orientation, indexed by individuals’ social identities, are essential for improving our understanding of the role of cultural orientation in development.

Korous, Causadias, Bradley, and Luthar (Reference Korous, Causadias, Bradley and Luthar2018) conducted a second-order meta-analysis to investigate the magnitude of the association between behavior problems and specific measures of socioeconomic status (i.e., income, educational attainment, and occupational prestige) and overall social status. They identified 12 meta-analyses including 474 primary studies and 327,617 participants and found small negative associations between both internalizing and externalizing and income and education, while only externalizing was related to overall socioeconomic status.

Umaña-Taylor (Reference Umaña-Taylor2018) discusses how research on ethnic–racial identity offers a heuristic model for how culture can be examined developmentally and in relation to psychopathology. She presents the Identity Project intervention program and discusses how its outcomes deliver empirical support for the idea that cultural development can be modified with interventions, and that these changes can lead to mental health and social benefits for adolescents.

Yaylaci (Reference Yaylaci2018) discusses the importance of interventions tailored to alleviate the impact of war, violent conflict, and displacement on the development and mental health of refugee children in general, and Syrian refugee children and families in Turkey in particular. She examines evidence of the role of war trauma on parenting and child development, and intervention strategies that can foster resilient functioning and well-being.

Understanding Cultural Differences and Similarities in Development

The last avenue to advance cultural development and psychopathology showcased in this Special Issue is through research on cultural differences and similarities in development. Group comparisons are central to developmental psychopathology. After all, considering normal and abnormal development together is the essence of the field (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1990). However, cultural, ethnic, and racial comparisons are fraught with challenges. For instance, comparing minorities to Whites may reinforce deficit models that depict minorities as inherently flawed, lacking resources, and underperforming (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Lamberty, Jenkins, Pipes McAdoo, Crnic, Wasik and Vázquez García1996, Reference García Coll, Akerman and Cicchetti2000). Exaggerating cultural differences can also support a deficit by difference approach, in which cultural differences reinforce deficit models (Causadias et al., Reference Causadias, Vitriol and Atkin2018b). Nevertheless, there is no theoretical justification or empirical evidence to support the notion that developmental processes operate differently between White and minority individuals (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Lamberty, Jenkins, Pipes McAdoo, Crnic, Wasik and Vázquez García1996). Research on cultural development and psychopathology can help elucidate the extent of differences and similarities in development between cultural, ethnic, and/or racial groups in an international context and in the United States. Several articles in the Special Issue pursue this aim.

Lansford and colleagues (Reference Lansford, Godwin, Bornstein, Chang, Deater-Deckard, Di Giunta and Bacchini2018) investigated parenting, culture, and the development of externalizing behaviors in nine countries, including China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States. They examined mother-, father-, and child-reported (N = 1,336 families) externalizing behavior problem trajectories from age 7 to 14. Children's externalizing behavior trajectories varied both across individuals within countries and across countries, and variation was larger at the individual level than at the national level.

Navarrete, Silva, van IJzendoorn, and Cárcamo (Reference Navarrete, Silva, van IJzendoorn and Cárcamo2018) examined physical and psychosocial development of Mapuche and nonindigenous Chilean toddlers in a longitudinal cohort of 12,398 children. Mapuches are the largest indigenous group in Chile, amounting to nearly 10% of the country's population. Mapuches showed fewer externalizing problems than nonindigenous Chilean toddlers. Socioeconomic status, quality of the home environment, and parenting stress were stronger predictors of socioemotional development than race/ethnicity.

Other studies focused on differences between Whites and minorities in the United States. Deer, Shields, Ivory, Hostinary, and Telzer (Reference Deer, Shields, Ivory, Hostinar and Telzer2018) used a sample of sample of 370 adolescents to examine White–minority differences in affect and diurnal cortisol patterns, including diurnal cortisol slopes, cortisol awakening response, and diurnal cortisol output. Minorities exhibited flatter diurnal cortisol slopes and reported lower levels of positive affect compared to White youth. However, racial differences in affect did not explain differences in cortisol.

Dismukes and colleagues (Reference Dismukes, Shirtcliff, Jones, Zeanah, Theall and Drury2018) studied the effect of two dyadically based stress paradigms, the Still Face paradigm and the Strange Situation procedure, across the first year of life on cortisol reactivity in a sample of 207 Black and White infants. Racial differences in cortisol were not present at 4 months but emerged at 12 months of age, with Black infants having higher cortisol. Maternal reports of racial discrimination were linked to cortisol differences within Black infants, suggesting that caregiver experiences of racial discrimination may have early damaging effects in children.

Finally, Causadias, Korous, and Cahill (Reference Causadias, Korous and Cahill2018) studied the cultural differences hypothesis (i.e., that there are large differences between Whites and minorities, while there are small differences between- and within-minority groups) and the cultural similarities hypothesis (i.e., that there are small differences between Whites and minorities, and these differences are equal or lesser than differences between- and within-minorities). They conducted a second-order meta-analysis on levels of psychopathology with 16 meta-analyses on 493 primary studies (N = 3,036,749) and found support for the cultural similarities hypothesis.

Conclusions

The articles included in this Special Issue advance research on cultural development and psychopathology by examining how risk, protective, and promotive cultural factors, operating at the individual and social levels, shape developmental pathways of normal and abnormal behavior. These articles display a diverse array of topics, methods, designs, samples, and theories that will hopefully advance our understanding of cultural development. This is crucial because an interdisciplinary approach is necessary to capture the complexity of adaptive and maladaptive development (Cicchetti & Dawson, Reference Cicchetti and Dawson2002). Moreover, these articles offer valuable insights that can inform prevention and intervention research. Future randomized control trials should include measures at multiple levels of analysis to better document the effects of our interventions and the accuracy of our theories (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2015). Assessing and intervening on cultural development at the individual and social levels are important steps in this direction.

It is also important to approach the research enterprise as a cultural phenomenon in itself. Scientific training is a process of cultural socialization into specific principles, concerns, assumptions, and ways of thinking about what is important and what is not (Cicchetti & Richters, Reference Cicchetti and Richters1997). For this reason, we need to change the way we think about culture. Change is also needed to the way we understand cultural risk, protection, and promotion at the individual and social levels. It is crucial to understand that cultural adversity is not destiny. Change is possible at any point in life because development is an active and dynamic process in which the meaning attributed to experiences shapes their consequences (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996). At the same time, there is coherence and continuity in the development of the person (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe and Masten2007), and cultural adversity places those who experience it in probabilistic pathways of risk and maladaptation. Improving theory, research, training, assessment, and interventions in cultural development and psychopathology can help us understand and promote health, well-being, and competent development.

Footnotes

We thank Kevin Korous and Karlyn Wegmann for their editorial assistance in the production of this Special Issue. We also thank Kim Updegraff and Rebecca White for their support in the manuscript review process.

References

Adams, G., & Markus, H. R. (2004). Toward a conception of culture suitable for a social psychology of culture. In Schaller, M. & Crandall, C. S. (Eds.), Psychological foundations of culture (pp. 335360). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Betancourt, H., & López, S. R. (1993). The study of culture, ethnicity, and race in American psychology. American Psychologist, 48, 629637. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.48.6.629Google Scholar
Bonilla-Silva, E. (2017). Racism without racists: Color-blind racism and the persistence of racial inequality in America. New York: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M. (2013). A roadmap for the integration of culture into developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 13751398. doi:10.1017/s0954579413000679Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., & Korous, K. M. (2018). How are genes related to culture? Introduction to the field of cultural genomics. In Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (Eds.), The handbook of culture and biology (pp. 153177). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Korous, K. M., & Cahill, K. M. (2018). Are Whites and minorities more similar than different? Testing the cultural similarities hypothesis on psychopathology with a second order meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 20092027.Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (2018). Introduction to culture and biology interplay. In Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (Eds.), The handbook of culture and biology (pp. 329). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Lee, R. M. (2017). Culture and biology interplay: An introduction. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23, 14. doi:10.1037/cdp0000121Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Vitriol, J. A., & Atkin, A. L. (2018a). Do we overemphasize the role of culture in the behavior of racial/ethnic minorities? Evidence of a cultural (mis)attribution bias in American Psychology. American Psychologist, 73, 243255. doi:10.1037/amp0000099Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Vitriol, J. A., & Atkin, A. L. (2018b). The cultural (mis)attribution bias in developmental psychology in the United States. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2018.01.003Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2014). Aspects of the theory of syntax (Vol. 11). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17. doi:10.2307/1129830Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1993). Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, reflections, projections. Developmental Review, 13, 471502.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Dawson, G. (2002). Multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 417666.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Richters, J. E. (1997). Examining the conceptual and scientific underpinnings of research in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 189191.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597600. doi:10.1017/s0954579400007318Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Building bridges and crossing them: Translational research in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 619622.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1625.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2015). Multilevel developmental perspectives on child maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4, Pt. 2), 13851386.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. B. (2009). Many forms of culture. American Psychologist, 64, 194204. doi:10.1037/a0015308Google Scholar
Constantine, M. G., Donnelly, P. C., & Myers, L. J. (2002). Collective self-esteem and Africultural coping styles in African American adolescents. Journal of Black Studies, 32, 698710. doi:10.1177/00234702032006004Google Scholar
Cooper, S. M., Brown, C., Metzger, I., Clinton, Y., & Guthrie, B. (2013). Racial discrimination and African American adolescents’ adjustment: Gender variation in family and community social support, promotive and protective factors. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 1529.Google Scholar
Cross, F. L., Hoffman, A. J., Constante, K., & Rivas-Drake, D. (2018). Ethnic–racial identity content and the development of depressive symptoms among Latino adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 15571569.Google Scholar
Deer, L. K., Shields, G. S., Ivory, S. L., Hostinar, C. E., & Telzer, E. H. (2018). Racial/ethnic disparities in cortisol diurnal patterns and affect in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 19771993.Google Scholar
Dismukes, A., Shirtcliff, E., Jones, C. W., Zeanah, C., Theall, K., & Drury, S. (2018). The development of the cortisol response to dyadic stressors in Black and White infants. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 19952008.Google Scholar
Doane, L. D., Sladek, M. R., & Adam, E. K. (2018a). An introduction to cultural neurobiology: Evidence from physiological stress systems. In Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (Eds.), The handbook of culture and biology (pp. 227254). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Doane, L. D., Sladek, M. R., Breitenstein, R. S., Park, H., Castro, S. A., & Kennedy, J. L. (2018). Cultural neurobiology and the family: Evidence from the daily lives of Latino adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 17791796.Google Scholar
Elam, K. K., Chassin, L., & Pandika, D. (2018). Polygenic risk, family cohesion, and adolescent aggression in Mexican American and European American families: Developmental pathways to alcohol use. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 17151728.Google Scholar
Fuller, B., & García Coll, C. (2010). Learning from Latinos: Contexts, families, and child development in motion. Developmental Psychology, 46, 559565.Google Scholar
García Coll, C., Akerman, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). Cultural influences on developmental processes and outcomes: Implications for the study of development and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 333374.Google Scholar
García Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., Pipes McAdoo, H., Crnic, K., Wasik, B. H., & Vázquez García, H. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67, 18911914. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01834.xGoogle Scholar
Gaylord-Harden, N. K., Burrow, A. L., & Cunningham, J. A. (2012). A cultural-asset framework for investigating successful adaptation to stress in African American youth. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 264271. doi:10. 1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00236.xGoogle Scholar
Gibbons, F. X., Fleischli, M. E., Gerrard, M., & Simons, R. L. (2018). Reports of perceived racial discrimination among African American children predict negative affect and smoking behavior in adulthood: A sensitive period hypothesis. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 16291647.Google Scholar
Gonzales, N. A., Johnson, M., Shirtcliff, E., Tein, J.-Y., Eskenazi, B., & Deardorff, J. (2018). The role of bicultural adaptation, familism, and family conflict in Mexican American adolescents’ cortisol reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 15711587.Google Scholar
Gonzales, N. A., Knight, G. P., Gunn, H. J., Tein, J.-Y., Tanaka, R., & White, R. M. B. (2018). Intergenerational gaps in Mexican American values trajectories: Associations with parent–adolescent conflict and adolescent psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 16111627.Google Scholar
Hill, L. K., & Hoggard, L. S. (2018). Active coping moderates associations among race-related stress, rumination and depressive symptoms in emerging adult African American women. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 18171835.Google Scholar
Jensen, L. A. (2012). Bridging universal and cultural perspectives: A vision for developmental psychology in a global world. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 98104.Google Scholar
Juang, L. P., Shen, Y., Costigan, C. L., & Hou, Y. (2018). Time-varying associations of racial discrimination and adjustment among Chinese-heritage adolescents in the US and Canada. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 16611678.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 419449. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357Google Scholar
Knight, G. P., Safa, M. D., & White, R. M. B. (2018). Advancing the assessment of cultural orientation: A developmental and contextual framework of multiple psychological dimensions and social identities. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 18671888.Google Scholar
Korous, K. M., Causadias, J. M., Bradley, R. H., & Luthar, S. S. (2018). Unpacking the link between socioeconomic status and behavior problems: A second-order meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 18891906.Google Scholar
Lam, P. H., Miller, G. E., Chiang, J. J., Levine, C. S., Le, V., Shalowitz, M. U., … Chen, E. (2018). One size does not fit all: Links between shift-and-persist and asthma in youth are moderated by perceived social status and experience of unfair treatment. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 16991714.Google Scholar
Lansford, J. E., Godwin, J., Bornstein, M. H., Chang, L., Deater-Deckard, K., Di Giunta, L., … Bacchini, D. (2018). Parenting, culture, and the development of externalizing behaviors from age 7 to 14 in nine countries. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 19371958.Google Scholar
Lehrner, A., & Yehuda, R. (2018). Cultural trauma and epigenetic inheritance. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 17631777.Google Scholar
Lei, M.-K., Beach, S. R. H., & Simons, R. L. (2018). Biological embedding of neighborhood disadvantage and collective efficacy: Influences on chronic illness via accelerated cardiometabolic age. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 17971815.Google Scholar
Lemery-Chalfant, K., Clifford, S., Dishion, T., Shaw, D., & Wilson, M. (2018). Genetic moderation of the effects of the Family Check-Up intervention on children's internalizing symptoms: A longitudinal study with a racial/ethnically diverse sample. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 17291747.Google Scholar
Lin, L. C., & Telzer, E. H. (2018). An introduction to cultural neuroscience. In Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (Eds.), The handbook of culture and biology (pp. 399420). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Meca, A., Schwartz, S. J., Martinez, C. R. Jr., & McClure, H. H. (2018). Longitudinal effects of acculturation and enculturation on mental health: Does the measure of matter? Development and Psychopathology, 30, 18491866.Google Scholar
Moya, C., & Henrich, J. (2016). Culture–gene coevolutionary psychology: Cultural learning, language, and ethnic psychology. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 112118. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.001Google Scholar
Muscatell, K. A., McCormick, E., & Telzer, E. H. (2018). Subjective social status and neural processing of race in Mexican American adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 18371848.Google Scholar
Navarrete, M. A., Silva, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Cárcamo, R. A. (2018). Physical and psychosocial development of Mapuche and nonindigenous Chilean toddlers: A modest role of ethnicity. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 19591976.Google Scholar
Ong, A. D., & Burrow, A. L. (2018). Affective reactivity to daily racial discrimination as a prospective predictor of depressive symptoms in African American graduate and postgraduate students. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 16491659.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Serafica, F. C., & Vargas, L. A. (2006). Cultural diversity in the development of child psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and method (2nd ed., pp. 588626). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Goodnow, J., Hatano, G., LeVine, R., Markus, H., & Miller, P. (2006). The cultural psychology of development: One mind, many mentalities. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child development (pp. 716792). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1990). Considering normal and abnormal together: The essence of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 335347.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 251268.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (2007). The place of development in developmental psychopathology. In Masten, A. (Ed.), Multilevel dynamics in developmental psychopathology pathways to the future: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology Vol. 34 (pp. 285299). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1729.Google Scholar
Su, J., Kuo, S. I., Meyers, J. L., Guy, M. C., & Dick, D. M. (2018). Examining interactions between genetic risk for alcohol problems, peer deviance, and interpersonal traumatic events on trajectories of alcohol use disorder symptoms among African American college students. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 17491761.Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2018). Intervening in cultural development: The case of ethnic–racial identity. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 19071922.Google Scholar
Updegraff, K. A., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Zeiders, K. H., Bravo, D. Y., & Jahromi, L. B. (2018). Familism values across the transition to adolescent motherhood: Links to family functioning and Mexican-origin adolescent mothers’ adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 15891609.Google Scholar
White, R. M. B., Zeiders, K. H., & Safa, M. D. (2018). Neighborhood structural characteristics and Mexican-origin adolescents’ development. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 16791698.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Laland, K. N., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Culture evolves. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 366, 938948. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0372Google Scholar
Yaylaci, F. T. (2018). Trauma and resilient functioning among Syrian refugee children. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 19231936.Google Scholar