The identification of factors that contribute to the development of psychological disorders across behavioral, environmental, and biological levels of analysis is of central importance to the field of developmental psychopathology. One example that holds promise is neurobehavioral disinhibition (ND), a complex condition of emotional and behavioral dysregulation reflecting a wide range of problems including oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), conduct disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), major depressive disorder, and the initiation of substance use (Habeych, Charles, Sclabassi, Kirisci, & Tarter, Reference Habeych, Charles, Sclabassi, Kirisci and Tarter2005; Lester et al., Reference Lester, Lin, Degarmo, Fisher, Lagasse and Levine2012; McNamee et al., Reference McNamee, Dunfee, Luna, Clark, Eddy and Tarter2008; Tarter et al., Reference Tarter, Kirisci, Mezzich, Cornelius, Pajer and Vanyukov2003). Children with ND appear to follow fairly consistent negative longitudinal trajectories that include peer rejection, academic difficulties, delinquency, and high rates of mental health, special education, residential care, and juvenile justice system involvement (Clausen, Landsverk, Ganger, Chadwick, & Litrownik, Reference Clausen, Landsverk, Ganger, Chadwick and Litrownik1998; Harden, Reference Harden2004). Key features of ND include difficulties with emotion regulation, impulse control (Iacono, Carlson, Taylor, Elkins, & McGue, Reference Iacono, Carlson, Taylor, Elkins and McGue1999), and executive functioning (Tarter et al., Reference Tarter, Kirisci, Mezzich, Cornelius, Pajer and Vanyukov2003). Emotion regulation is critical for adaptive responding in social situations and social affiliation, key constructs that are impaired among youths with externalizing behavior (Hill, Degnan, Calkins, & Keane, Reference Hill, Degnan, Calkins and Keane2006; Vasilev, Crowell, Beauchaine, Mead, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Vasilev, Crowell, Beauchaine, Mead and Gatzke-Kopp2009), conduct disorder and ODD (Beauchaine, Gartner, & Hagen, Reference Beauchaine, Gardner and Hagen2000; Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, Reference Beauchaine, Gatzeke-Kapp and Mead2007; Beauchaine, Hinshaw, & Pang, Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2010; Beauchaine, Hong, & Marsh, Reference Beauchaine, Hong and Marsh2008), major depressive disorder (Rottenberg, Gross, & Gotlib, Reference Rottenberg, Gross and Gotlib2005), and ADHD (Musser et al., Reference Musser, Backs, Schmitt, Ablow, Measelle and Nigg2011), most of which are included on the spectrum of ND (Iacono et al., Reference Iacono, Carlson, Taylor, Elkins and McGue1999).
Children exposed to substances prenatally and who have faced significant early life adversity may be particularly likely to develop ND (Chapman, Tarter, Kirisci, & Cornelius, Reference Chapman, Tarter, Kirisci and Cornelius2007; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011), perhaps in part because such early life stress is known to affect neural systems associated with behavior regulation and modulation (Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry, & Knight, Reference Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry and Knight2009). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) is a physiological correlate of emotion regulation that has been studied in a variety of typically developing and at-risk populations; however, there are no studies of RSA in children with ND. RSA is a phenomenon whereby heart rate shows high frequency variability in time with respiration (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Porges, Reference Porges2007). There is likely considerable equifinality, or the process by which different etiological pathways lead to the same disorder, to the development of ND that includes transactions across genetic, physiologic, and contextual levels (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011). Incorporating measures of RSA into longitudinal studies of ND may help us to identify children who are most susceptible to the development of ND by virtue of their profile of RSA reactivity across early childhood and their exposure to adverse events early in life.
RSA and ND
RSA is typically measured in two ways: during baseline and in response to task demands, termed RSA reactivity. High baseline RSA reflects the functional relationship between the central nervous system and the heart as mediated by the vagus (Berntson et al., Reference Berntson, Bigger, Eckberg, Grossman, Kaufmann and Malik1997; Porges, Reference Porges2007). High baseline RSA is thought to reflect a baseline of neural integrity and a readiness to respond to environmental stressors (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001). Change in RSA in response to acute stress is termed RSA reactivity. We use the term RSA reactivity to reflect decreases in RSA in response to task demands. In typically developing children, RSA reliably decreases in response to challenge and increases once the challenge has resolved (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001). Moderate RSA withdrawal in response to attention-demanding tasks is functional and may reflect “optimal” engagement with the environment (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Marcovitch et al., Reference Marcovitch, Leigh, Calkins, Leerks, O'Brien and Blankson2010). In contrast, greater RSA withdrawal may instead be a form of emotional lability suggestive of fight/flight responding (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001).
It may be particularly important to study changes in RSA during early childhood. Alterations in RSA associated with later externalizing psychopathology, a key component of the ND construct, appear to develop sometime between preschool and middle childhood (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzeke-Kapp and Mead2007). Given the presumed plasticity of the parasympathetic nervous system during early childhood, changes in RSA across this time period because of early adverse experiences and/or prenatal substance exposure may be prognostic of later externalizing behavior and behavior dysregulation (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001).
Previous work has focused on the parasympathetic nervous system and the behavior dysregulation component of ND. Although there are exceptions (Musser et al., Reference Musser, Backs, Schmitt, Ablow, Measelle and Nigg2011), youths prone to ND, including preschoolers with ADHD and ODD (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Sylvers, Mead and Chipman-Chacon2006) and adolescents (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gardner and Hagen2000), and boys with conduct problems (Beauchaine, Hong, et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hong and Marsh2008) exhibit lower baseline RSA. Children (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gatzeke-Kapp and Mead2007) and adolescents (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gardner and Hagen2000) with clinical levels of externalizing behavior, and boys with conduct problems (Beauchaine, Hong, et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Pang2008) also exhibit attenuated baseline RSA. In addition, boys (but not girls) with lower initial levels of baseline RSA had externalizing behavior that increased across adolescence (El-Sheikh & Hinnant, Reference El-Sheikh and Hinnant2011).
Some studies have found no significant association between RSA reactivity and externalizing behavior in clinical samples (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Gardner and Hagen2000, Reference Beauchaine, Gatzeke-Kapp and Mead2007), bit others found significant relations in community samples (Keller & El-Sheikh, Reference Keller and El-Sheikh2009) and in children with ADHD (Musser et al., Reference Musser, Backs, Schmitt, Ablow, Measelle and Nigg2011). For instance, children with reduced autonomic functioning in the form of lower RSA reactivity (Keller & El-Sheikh, Reference Keller and El-Sheikh2009) and the combination of both low baseline RSA and lower RSA reactivity (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, Reference Hinnant and El-Sheikh2009) exhibited the greatest levels of externalizing behavior. In contrast, others (Calkins, Graziano, & Keane, Reference Calkins, Graziano and Keane2007) found no differences in baseline RSA among 5-year-old children at risk for behavior problems. However, children at risk for both externalizing and internalizing problems exhibited the greatest RSA reactivity compared to children with low behavior problems and children at risk for externalizing problems only.
Although youths at risk for the development of externalizing disorder may exhibit a profile of reduced autonomic functioning, a second plausible pathway to the development of ND exists. The theory of diathesis stress suggests that a “risk” factor in combination with an adverse or stressful environment leads to greater risk for the development of problem behavior (Monroe & Simons, Reference Monroe and Simons1991). Children who are more physiologically reactive may be more likely to exhibit ND if they are also raised in adverse environments. For instance, in a community sample of children, Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler, and Boyce (Reference Obradovic, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler and Boyce2010) found that children with greater RSA reactivity exhibited less prosocial behavior and school engagement, but only if they were exposed to greater environmental adversity (operationalized as a cumulative index of six measures of family stress). Thus, RSA reactivity may moderate the effect of early adversity and/or prenatal substance exposure such that children who are more reactive will be more likely to exhibit growth in ND if exposed to more substances in utero or if reared in particularly adverse environments.
There is comparatively less information about RSA and executive dysfunction, a second component of ND. Children with higher baseline RSA perform better on tasks of attention and working memory (Hansen, Johnsen, & Thayer, Reference Hansen, Johnsen and Thayer2003; Marcovitch et al., Reference Marcovitch, Leigh, Calkins, Leerks, O'Brien and Blankson2010; Staton, El-Sheikh, & Buckhalt, Reference Staton, El-Sheikh and Buckhalt2009), perhaps because these children are better able to engage with environmental demands and also self-regulate during both cognitive and emotional challenges. Moderate decreases in RSA appear to be related to improved executive functioning, including greater on-task behavior, behavioral inhibition, and set shifting (Blair, Reference Blair2003; Marcovitch et al., Reference Marcovitch, Leigh, Calkins, Leerks, O'Brien and Blankson2010), though null findings with RSA reactivity and executive functions have also been found (Staton et al., Reference Staton, El-Sheikh and Buckhalt2009). Thus, high baseline RSA and moderate decreases in RSA in response to challenge may be prognostic of better executive functioning compared to low baseline RSA and an absence of RSA reactivity.
Prenatal Substance Exposure, Early Adversity, and RSA
The purpose of many prenatal drug exposure studies is to examine the effects of a single substance (such as cocaine), while controlling for the effects of all other substances (such as alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana). A complementary approach is to take into account recent work suggesting that prenatal substance exposure is a stressor (Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009), and as such, cumulative prenatal stress models may provide an alternative way of modeling the contribution of prenatal substance exposure to relevant developmental processes. Specifically, in addition to its teratogenic properties, prenatal substance exposure can alter the infant's physiological response to the postnatal environment (Lester, Marsit, Conradt, Bromer, & Padbury, in press; Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009). That is, exposure to substances prenatally acts as a stressor that may alter the expression of specific genes important to placental function in late gestation (Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009), which may lead to fetal exposure of high levels of maternal cortisol (Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009). The effects of prenatal substance exposure as an intrauterine stressor that alters physiological response systems (Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009) could result in disrupted parasympathetic responding, leading to the development of ND. A similar process is thought to occur in response to prenatal alcohol (Hellemans, Sliwowska, Verma, & Weinberg, Reference Hellemans, Sliwowska, Verma and Weinberg2010) and tobacco (Heim, Plotsky, & Nemeroff, Reference Heim, Plotsky and Nemeroff2004) exposure. Furthermore, women who use substances prenatally typically use other substances; therefore, polydrug use could be conceptualized as a cumulative prenatal stressor (Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009). Support for this view comes from the work of Fisher et al. (Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011), who found that a summative index of prenatal substance exposure predicted blunted hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis reactivity among children in foster care. The literature linking prenatal substance exposure to RSA is small, and no study that we know of has examined the effects of polysubstance exposure. Thus, we focused on polysubstance exposure, with the caveat that different substances may impact RSA via different mechanisms that are unknown thus far.
Studies examining the effects of prenatal substance exposure on RSA are mixed, and they are largely limited to infants. Infants with prenatal substance exposure have lower levels of baseline RSA at 1 and 7 months than do nonexposed infants (Schuetze & Eiden, Reference Schuetze and Eiden2006; Schuetze, Eiden, & Edwards, Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Edwards2009), and an absence of RSA reactivity at 7 months (Schuetze, Eiden, & Coles, Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Coles2007). Other groups found no significant associations with either baseline RSA (DiPietro, Suess, Wheeler, Smouse, & Newlin, Reference DiPietro, Suess, Wheeler, Smouse and Newlin1995) or RSA reactivity (DiPietro et al., Reference DiPietro, Suess, Wheeler, Smouse and Newlin1995; Sheinkopf et al., Reference Sheinkopf, Lagasse, Lester, Liu, Seifer and Bauer2007) compared to nonexposed children. Increased baseline RSA (Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, Finkelhor, Anderson, Harcar-Sevcik, Wasser and Bahler1993) has also been reported in infants with prenatal cocaine exposure, which may be due to the need to utilize greater neurobiological resources in order to meet environmental demands.
The development of children with prenatal substance exposure is also complicated by a host of additional risk factors. Early adverse experiences among children with prenatal substance exposure tend to co-occur and include exposure to parental psychopathology, neighborhood violence, chaotic home environments, and lack of monetary resources (Dong et al., Reference Dong, Anda, Felitti, Dube, Williamson and Thompson2004; Propper, Reference Propper2012), making it extremely difficult to separate the effect of one adverse experience from the other. Many researchers therefore take a cumulative risk approach to studying the impact of co-occurring risk factors on physiological functioning (Eiden, Granger, Schuetze, & Veira, Reference Eiden, Granger, Schuetze and Veira2011; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011; Sameroff, Seifer, Baldwin, & Baldwin, Reference Sameroff, Seifer, Baldwin and Baldwin1993). Cumulative risk models have also been shown to be a more powerful predictor of externalizing and internalizing behavior among children with prenatal substance exposure than risks examined in isolation (Yumoto, Jacobson, & Jacobson, Reference Yumoto, Jacobson and Jacobson2008).
Early adverse experiences have been related to the sympathetic and hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal systems (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Granger, Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Cox and Greenberg2011; Gunnar & Fisher, Reference Gunnar and Fisher2006; Heim et al., Reference Heim, Plotsky and Nemeroff2004; Lupien, King, Meaney, & McEwen, Reference Lupien, King, Meaney and McEwen2001; Saridjan et al., Reference Saridjan, Huizink, Koetsier, Jaddoe, Mackenbach and Hofman2010), though evidence of how these adverse experiences impact the parasympathetic system is scarce (Propper, Reference Propper2012), despite known associations between early adversity and emotion regulation (Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011; Tottenham et al., Reference Tottenham, Hare, Millner, Gilhooly, Zevin and Casey2011). Though there are exceptions, low income (Propper, Reference Propper2012), exposure to domestic violence (Rigterink, Fainsilber Katz, & Hessler, Reference Rigterink, Fainsilber Katz and Hessler2010), and marital conflict (El-Sheikh & Hinnant, Reference El-Sheikh and Hinnant2011) have been associated with altered RSA either in the form of lower baseline RSA (Field, Pickens, Fox, Nawrocki, & Gonzalez, Reference Field, Pickens, Fox, Nawrocki and Gonzalez1995; Pickens & Field, Reference Pickens and Field1995; Schuetze & Eiden, Reference Schuetze and Eiden2006; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Edwards2009) or less growth in baseline RSA (Rigterink et al., Reference Rigterink, Fainsilber Katz and Hessler2010). El-Sheikh and Hinnant (Reference El-Sheikh and Hinnant2011) found that boys with greater RSA reactivity who experienced higher marital conflict at age 8 or increasing marital conflict from ages 8 to 11 exhibited decreases in baseline RSA across early adolescence, suggesting “wear and tear” on the parasympathetic system. Exposure to adverse conditions among children with prenatal substance exposure could exacerbate an already sensitive physiological stress system (Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009), contributing to difficulties with emotion regulation and risk for the development of ND. In other words, children with prenatal substance exposure may be at more of a disadvantage by virtue of the increased likelihood of exposure to adverse conditions postnatally.
Study Aims and Hypotheses
Data from this study come from a prospective longitudinal investigation of children with prenatal substance exposure and from whom extensive measures of early adversity and ND were assessed. Across early childhood, assessments of RSA at baseline and in response to challenge were conducted. We thus have the unique opportunity to examine the effects of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on growth in baseline RSA and RSA reactivity when these physiological systems are in the process of maturing and are arguably quite vulnerable to environmental influence (Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Brenner, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Brenner and Gatzke-Kopp2008). We focused on RSA as a possible physiological correlate of ND owing to its association with emotion regulation.
There are inconsistencies regarding which disorders are included as components of ND. Tarter et al. (Reference Tarter, Kirisci, Mezzich, Cornelius, Pajer and Vanyukov2003) include both internalizing and externalizing problems because both are related to problems with emotional and behavioral dysregulation and frequently co-occur, whereas others (Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Hicks, Patrick, Carlson, Iacono and McGue2002; Krueger & South, Reference Krueger and South2009) include only externalizing symptoms. Tarter et al. also include executive function deficits in their model of ND while others do not (Iacono, Malone, & McGue, Reference Iacono, Malone and McGue2008; Krueger & South, Reference Krueger and South2009). Furthermore, adolescents whose parents have substance use disorders are frequently included in studies by Tarter. Based on careful consideration of these inconsistencies in the literature, and because of the similarities in our two samples, we decided to follow Tarter et al.'s approach to modeling ND, which includes both internalizing and externalizing symptoms as well as executive dysfunction. We focused on the construct of ND, rather than its component parts, based on observations that psychiatric disorders rarely occur in isolation but often occur in clusters (Lilienfeld, Reference Lilienfeld2003).
We examined growth in ND using a measure of ND validated with the current sample from middle childhood to adolescence (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011; Lester et al., Reference Lester, Lin, Degarmo, Fisher, Lagasse and Levine2012). ND was assessed during the transition to adolescence because it is during this time that extensive development in prefrontal areas are associated with the self-regulatory and executive function components of ND. We employed longitudinal growth modeling to better understand both normative and problematic growth trajectories of ND, as well as the impact of prenatal substance exposure, early adversity, RSA, and their interaction on this growth.
Our first goal was to describe the unconditional growth trajectories of baseline RSA and RSA reactivity at ages 3, 4, 5, and 6 years. On the basis of prior evidence, we expected that baseline RSA would increase with age (Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Edwards2009), whereas the magnitude of RSA reactivity would decrease (Blandon, Calkins, Keane, & O'Brien, Reference Blandon, Calkins, Keane and O'Brien2010; Propper et al., Reference Propper, Moore, Mills-Koonce, Halpern, Hill-Soderlund and Calkins2008). Our second goal was to examine the main effects of prenatal substance exposure, early adversity, and RSA on the development of the behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction components of ND. We hypothesized that decreases in baseline RSA and increases in RSA reactivity would be associated with increases in behavior dysregulation. Following prior research, we expected that greater increases in baseline RSA and moderate decreases in RSA reactivity over time would be associated with lower levels of executive dysfunction. Based on prior evidence with this sample, we expected that greater levels of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011) would be related to increases in both behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction over time, even when including measures of RSA in our models. Motivated by theories suggesting that interactions across multiple levels of analysis contribute to the development of psychopathology, our third goal was to test interactions between our stress variables (prenatal substance exposure and early adversity) and RSA on growth in ND. Based on the theory of diathesis stress, we expected that greater early adversity and prenatal substance exposure, combined with increased RSA reactivity or decreased baseline RSA (in separate models), would be related to increases in ND (both the behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction constructs) in adolescence.
Method
Participants
We used data collected from 1,387 participants in the Maternal Lifestyle Study (MLS), a multisite (Detroit, MI; Memphis, TN; Miami, FL; and Providence, RI) investigation of the effects of prenatal substance exposure in a longitudinal follow-up from ages 1 month to 16 years. Details on the enrollment and exclusion criteria are described elsewhere (Lester et al., Reference Lester, Tronick, LaGasse, Seifer, Bauer and Shankaran2002). In brief, families were selected to be in the exposed group (i.e., maternal report of cocaine or opiate use during pregnancy and gas chromatography–mass spectrometry confirmation of presumptive positive meconium screens for cocaine or opiate metabolites) or the comparison group (i.e., maternal denial of cocaine or opiate use during the pregnancy and a negative enzyme multiplied immunoassay meconium screen for cocaine and opiate metabolites). The exposed and comparison youths were group matched on race, sex, and gestational age within each study site. Other substances associated with cocaine use (e.g., alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana) were present in both groups so that group differences could be attributed to cocaine. Thus, most participants were polysubstance exposed.
The study was approved by the institutional review board at each study site, and written informed consent (caregivers) was obtained from all participants. Each site had a certificate of confidentiality from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. For this study, we used data from visits to the hospital clinic at ages 3, 4, 5, 6, 9, 11, and 13 years. The MLS sample includes children in the following racial and ethnic categories: African American (77%), Caucasian (16%), Hispanic (6%), and children whose parents identified other racial or ethnic backgrounds (1%).
Measures
RSA
Baseline RSA and RSA reactivity were measured at 3, 4, 5, and 6 years. RSA was derived from the R-R time series collected from digitized electrocardiogram (ECG) recordings using the Porges algorithm from MXEdit (Delta-Biometrics Inc., 1988–1993). ECGs were recorded via three electrodes placed on the child's chest and abdomen. The ECG signal was sampled at 1 kHz and stored on a computer for later scoring. Interbeat intervals were defined by detection of R-waves to the nearest millisecond.
Postprocessing of the data took place off-line by using a series of automated algorithms. R-R intervals outside of expected values were identified. Missed or spurious R-waves were flagged and corrected by linear interpolation. A 21-point moving polynomial was then applied to remove low frequency trends in the heart rate signal. Next, a bandpass filter extracted the variance in heart period within the frequency band of spontaneous respiration in young children (0.24–1.04 Hz). This process removes periodicities in the ECG signal that are outside the frequency range of the respiratory cycle. The resulting measure of RSA is in the frequency range of respiration. RSA was computed as the natural logarithm of heart period variance in milliseconds squared [ln(ms)2]. This method was described initially by Porges (Reference Porges1985) and is one of several acceptable approaches for calculating RSA (Berntson et al., Reference Berntson, Bigger, Eckberg, Grossman, Kaufmann and Malik1997). RSA data were calculated in 30-s overlapping windows and then averaged within each episode (baseline and attention task). The RSA data for an individual was used as long as there was a 30-s segment, with less than 20% of segments identified with artifact (Jennings et al., Reference Jennings, Berg, Hutcheson, Obrist, Porges and Turpin1981), which is the shortest duration recommended when calculating RSA with children. Thirty-second epochs are frequently sampled in studies of young children (Blandon et al., Reference Blandon, Calkins, Keane and O'Brien2010; Buss, Goldsmith, & Davidson, Reference Buss, Goldsmith and Davidson2005; Huffman, Reference Huffman1998; Moore & Calkins, Reference Moore and Calkins2004). In addition, small amounts of artifact can be expected to have a minimal effect on measures of heart rate variability such as RSA (Berntson, Cacioppo, & Quigley, Reference Berntson, Cacioppo and Quigley1993).
Baseline RSA was assessed while the children were waiting for a mastery motivation procedure (described below) to begin. Because RSA can be activated by psychomotor activity (Bazhenova & Porges, Reference Bazhenova and Porges1997), like others (Obradovic et al., Reference Obradovic, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler and Boyce2010), we assessed baseline RSA during a nonchallenge “control” task that paralleled the motor demands of our challenge task. Specifically, children played with a toy alone or with an experimenter for an average of 164.8 s (range = 135.6–199.3 s) while sitting at a table. Although this baseline required some attention, which may evoke vagal withdrawal (Suess, Porges, & Plude, Reference Suess, Porges and Plude1994), the toy with which the child played was simple, and no task demands were placed on the child during this time. Our estimates of baseline RSA are similar (within 1 SD) to other studies of young children of similar ages (Alkon et al., Reference Alkon, Goldstein, Smider, Essex, Kupfer and Boyce2003; Buss et al., Reference Buss, Goldsmith and Davidson2005; Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Graziano and Keane2007; Calkins & Keane, Reference Calkins and Keane2004; Doussard-Roosevelt, Montgomery, & Porges, Reference Doussard-Roosevelt, Montgomery and Porges2003), who have also calculated RSA based on the Porges technique. Furthermore, the sample as a whole had RSA levels that decreased from this baseline to the attention demanding task at each age assessed (Table 1), indicating that our baseline measure was perceived as less stressful than the attention-demanding task.
Note: RSA, respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Neurobehavioral disinhibition indicators (behavioral dysregulation and executive function difficulties) were rescaled on a continuous 0–1 metric and averaged to compute the behavioral dysregulation and executive dysfunction constructs.
RSA reactivity was assessed in response to a mastery motivation procedure (Dwek, Reference Dwek and Dienstbeir1991). The attention-demanding task comprised the first 3 min of the procedure. Children transitioned from supportive play with an experimenter to play with a moderately challenging toy just above the developmental level of the child without the aid of the experimenter or caregiver, though the experimenter was present during the task. Toys included a pathfinder (3 years), tinker toys (4 and 5 years), and a puzzle (6 years). Performance during mastery motivation tasks is positively related to emotion regulation (Heckhausen & Dweck, Reference Heckhausen and Dweck1998); therefore, it was expected that this challenge should also tax emotion systems. RSA reactivity was calculated using a difference score (RSAbaseline – RSAtask). Positive values reflect greater RSA reactivity (i.e. greater decrease of RSA). Our estimates of RSA reactivity are similar (within 1 SD) to other studies of young children of similar ages using attention-demanding tasks and the Porges technique (Alkon et al., Reference Alkon, Goldstein, Smider, Essex, Kupfer and Boyce2003; Buss et al., Reference Buss, Goldsmith and Davidson2005; Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Graziano and Keane2007; Calkins & Keane, Reference Calkins and Keane2004).
ND
Two longitudinal construct scores were employed to assess growth in ND over three assessment periods during early adolescence using a strategy previously employed in studies involving the same sample (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011; Lester et al., Reference Lester, Lin, Degarmo, Fisher, Lagasse and Levine2012). The construct scores for behavioral dysregulation and executive dysfunction were computed from multimethod, multiagent, validated, well-established measures. The ND construct has good predictive validity, particularly its ability to predict substance-use onset, and good construct, discriminative, and concurrent validity, as well as good test–retest and internal reliability (Mezzich et al., Reference Mezzich, Tarter, Feske, Kirisci, McNamee and Day2007).
The behavioral dysregulation construct was a composite score of six measures of internalizing and externalizing problems: (a) problem behaviors reported by caregivers on the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1992) using the standardized T scores for total problem behaviors at ages 9, 11, and 13 (we used T scores rather than raw scores because we were interested in comparing children's behavior relative to same-aged peers and normed by sex; in contrast, raw scores cannot be used to compare children's behavior against population norms of other children of the same age and sex, and the use of Child Behavior Checklist T scores is therefore common in longitudinal research in which trajectories of symptoms are assessed; e.g., see Milan & Pinderhughes, Reference Milan and Pinderhughes2006); (b) ODD, (c) conduct disorder, (d) ADHD, (e) and major depression symptom counts reported by the parent from the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children—IV (Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, Reference Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan and Schwab-Stone2000) at ages 8, 11, and 14; and (f) delinquency summary scores for the number of self-reported crimes against people and acts of general delinquency and school delinquency from the Things That You Have Done measure (Elliott, Huizinga, & Ageton, Reference Elliott, Huizinga and Ageton1985; Ingoldsby, Kohl, McMahon, Lengua, & Group, Reference Ingoldsby, Kohl, McMahon, Lengua and Group2006) at ages 9, 11, and 13. This measure is commonly used as an outcome variable in studies examining predictors of delinquent behavior (Bierman et al., Reference Bierman, Coie, Dodge, Foster, Greenberg and Lochman2004; Kaplow, Curran, & Dodge, Reference Kaplow, Curran and Dodge2002; Lamis, Malone, Lansford, & Lochman, Reference Lamis, Malone, Lansford and Lochman2012). Because the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children—IV was administered at years 8 and 14, we computed construct scores and have labeled the assessment periods as years 8/9, 11, and 13/14 throughout the text.
To evaluate comparable means over time, the composite growth score necessitates rescaling the original range of indicators to a common metric ranging from 0 to 1 as a continuous level scale. The composite is computed as the average of the three indicators. Cronbach α values were 0.73, 0.80, and 0.80, respectively, over time for the composite score of behavioral dysregulation. Using principal components factor analyses, we obtained a single-factor solution at each time point: eigenvalues of 2.98, 3.27, and 3.25, respectively, explained over 50% of the variance.
Executive dysfunction was measured with two computerized tests from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Batteries (Luciana, Reference Luciana2003) at ages 9, 11, and 13. There is variability in the indicators researchers use to measure executive dysfunction as a component of ND. We chose tasks that assess spatial working memory, thinking time, and planning, all of which have been shown to be related to brain areas related to deficiencies among children exposed prenatally to drugs (Sheinkopf et al., Reference Sheinkopf, Lester, Sanes, Eliassen, Hutchison and Seifer2009). The spatial working-memory task is self-ordered and requires the respondent to find a “token” in an array of colored boxes; we measured the total number of errors on the task. The Stockings of Cambridge task involves spatial planning, and the respondents must use “balls” in one display to copy the pattern shown in another display. Thinking time includes two trials measuring the time to select the initial stimulus. Shorter times have been related to impulsivity. We measured the total number of correct solutions in the minimal number of moves (reverse scored to indicate executive dysfunction). The composite growth score was the average of the two indicators rescaled to 0–1.
Key predictors
Prenatal substance exposure was measured as a summative index ranging from 0 to 5 for mother-reported (or gas chromatography–mass spectrometry confirmation of presumptive positive meconium screens for cocaine or opiate metabolites) use of cocaine, opiates, marijuana, alcohol, and tobacco during pregnancy. Maternal report/meconium screen of drug use (1 = yes, 0 = no) prenatally was computed. One point was assigned for each substance used. Dichotomous measures are frequently used in testing prenatal substance exposure effects (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011; Smith, Johnson, Pears, Fisher, & DeGarmo, Reference Smith, Johnson, Pears, Fisher and DeGarmo2007).
Early adversity was a summative risk index from birth to the age 3 assessments and included nine risk factors. Each risk factor was either a continuous scale or a count score and was dichotomized to create an overall risk index (0 = no/none, 1 = yes/one or more): (a) annual assessments of postnatal substance use of cocaine, opiates, tobacco, alcohol, or marijuana up to the Year 3 assessment; (b) chronic poverty status calculated as income below $10,000 over annual visits; (c) low social status scored from the Hollingshead Index of Social Position (Hollingshead, Reference Hollingshead1975) using education and occupation averaged over annual visits and scored as 1 SD or more below the mean; (d) primary caretaker changes; (e) sexual or physical abuse of the child as reported by caregivers; (f) caregiver depression of 1 SD or greater above the mean for averaged depressive symptoms on the caregiver reported Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Steer, & Brown, Reference Beck, Steer and Brown1996); (g) caretaker psychological distress of 1 SD or greater above the mean for averaged psychological symptoms above clinical cutoff on the Brief Symptom Inventory (Derogatis & Coons, Reference Derogatis and Coons1993); (h) poor quality home environment of 1 SD or more below the mean on the Home Observation Measurement of the Environment (Caldwell & Bradley, Reference Caldwell and Bradley1984); and (i) any history of Child Protective Services Involvement assessed annually until the Year 3 assessment.
Missing data
Participants from the current study come from the sample used in Fisher et al. (Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011), which included 1,073 MLS families in which the mothers had prenatal substance exposure scores and the youths had outcome data by the adolescent follow-up period. The MLS study originally enrolled 1,388 mothers with prenatal substance exposure scores. By the age 8/9 assessment, 81% of mothers had data on the youths enrolled in the study. Attrition analyses are described in detail in Fisher et al (Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011). In brief, there were no differences on prenatal substance exposure when comparing mothers with youth data at follow-up and those without. Significant differences were observed for early adversity, with mothers and youths in follow-up exhibiting more risk compared to mothers whose children were lost to follow-up (M = 1.80, SD = 1.14, and M = 1.56, SD = 1.07, respectively, p < .01). There were no significant differences in prenatal substance exposure or early adversity between children with and without baseline RSA and RSA reactivity data within each age (all ps > .14). Children with missing RSA data at 3 and 4 years had significantly higher behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction scores at age 8/9 compared to children without missing RSA data at 3 and 4 years (ps < .02). Children with missing RSA data at 4 years also had less growth in behavior dysregulation (b = –0.24, p = .02). There were no other differences in ND (assessed via the behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction growth factors) between children with and without baseline RSA and RSA reactivity data within each age (all ps > .19).
Analytic strategy
We employed structural equation modeling (SEM) to understand whether prenatal substance exposure and early adversity were related to the level and change over time in RSA and ND. Within this SEM framework, we used latent variable growth modeling to test the hypothesis that change in RSA is related to change in ND. Change in RSA and change in ND were characterized by scale-specific intercepts and slopes that were allowed to randomly vary across people.
Within-person variation in RSA and ND over time was modeled with individual-specific growth parameters (i.e., intercept, linear slope, and, when appropriate, quadratic slope). The two components that comprise ND, behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction, were modeled separately. We tested two overarching models. In the first, latent growth curves were used to test a series of models examining the level and shape of children's baseline RSA from 3 to 6 years of age and ND (behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction modeled separately) from ages 8/9 to 13/14. The four chronometric time weights for baseline RSA were specified at 0, 1, 2, and 3. In the second, we examined the level and shape of children's RSA reactivity from 3 to 6 years and ND (behavior dysregulation and executive dysfunction modeled separately) from ages 8/9 to 13/14. RSA reactivity was therefore modeled as a developmental process using a flexible approach to the structure of the SEM model. We specified the four chronometric time weights for the RSA reactivity linear slope factor at 0, 1, 2, and 3, and the quadratic slope as 0, 1, 4, and 9. We specified three chronometric time weights for the ND slope factor at 0, 1, and 2. This model allowed us to test the hypothesis that growth in RSA predicts growth in ND.
We used an SEM approach to test the interaction between growth in RSA and prenatal substance exposure/early adversity on growth in ND (Klein & Moosbrugger, Reference Klein and Moosbrugger2000). SEM uses maximum likelihood estimation of model parameters, and when latent variable interactions are included in the model, it does not yield chi-square and related model fit statistics because means, variances, and covariances are not sufficient for model estimation. Instead, nested models were compared using the sample-size adjusted Bayesian information criterion (Sclove, Reference Sclove1987) and the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, Reference Akaike1987) to determine whether the interaction model resulted in better model fit compared to the main effects model. Lower sample-size adjusted Bayesian information criterion and AIC values indicated better model fit. All analyses were conducted using Mplus 5.1 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2007).
Results
Preliminary analyses
The mean of the prenatal substance exposure index was 1.89 substances (SD = 1.38, range = 0–5). Forty-three percent of the sample was cocaine exposed, 8.3% opiate exposed, 58.5% alcohol exposed, 53.0% tobacco exposed, and 23.0% marijuana exposed. The mean number of early adversity factors was 2.35 (SD = 1.43, range = 0–8). As expected, there was a steady increase in baseline RSA from 3 to 6 years (see Table 1). RSA reactivity showed a curvilinear trend, with greater RSA reactivity at 3 years, decreasing RSA reactivity at 4 and 5 years, and increasing RSA reactivity again at 6 years. The mean composite growth construct score for behavioral dysregulation was largely stable over time (see Table 1). The mean composite score for executive function difficulties decreased over time.
Correlations
Correlations between baseline RSA, RSA reactivity, prenatal substance exposure, early adversity, behavioral dysregulation, and executive dysfunction are presented in Table 2. The intercorrelations between baseline RSA at 3, 4, 5, and 6 years were positively and strongly correlated. Intercorrelations among RSA reactivity measures were less strong, though also positive. Greater exposure to early adversity, and less exposure to substances prenatally, was related to less RSA reactivity at 3 years, but greater exposure to early adversity and drugs prenatally was associated with greater RSA reactivity at 6 years. Prenatal substance exposure was also significantly and positively associated with early adversity. Greater levels of behavioral dysregulation at all ages were associated with greater levels of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity, whereas greater levels of executive function difficulties were only significantly associated with greater levels of early adversity. Behavioral dysregulation and executive function difficulties were positively and moderately correlated (rs = .11–.26, ps < .001). In addition, behavioral dysregulation constructs at all ages were positively and strongly correlated (rs =.49–.62), as were the executive function difficulties constructs (rs = .44–.59).
Note: RSA, baseline respiratory sinus arrhythmia; RSA-R, RSA reactivity; BD, behavioral dysregulation; EF, executive dysfunction. Higher levels of RSA-R reflect greater RSA reactivity.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Unconditional growth models
Baseline RSA
Models were tested examining the level and shape of children's RSA trajectories during the baseline episode. Individual, within-person variation in RSA over time was allowed to vary across persons. On average, children showed a significant linear increase in baseline RSA over time. Children's baseline RSA at 3 years was 5.63 ln(ms)2, and the average rate of change in baseline RSA was 0.19 ln(ms)2. Significant variability was found for children's intercepts (σ2 = 0.79, p < .001), but not slopes (σ2 = 0.003, p = .86). This suggests that the initial level for baseline RSA varied across children, but the rate of change was relatively constant. The model fit the data well, χ2 (4) = 4.40, p = .35; comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, Reference Bentler1990) = 1.00, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck, Reference Browne and Cudeck1993) = 0.01.
RSA reactivity
Identical models also tested RSA reactivity in response to the attention-demanding task. A model that included a curvilinear growth term (quadratic) provided excellent fit to the data, χ2 (4) = 4.45, p = .35; CFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.01 (Bentler, Reference Bentler1990; Browne & Cudeck, Reference Browne and Cudeck1993), with reactivity highest at 3 years, decreasing at 4 and 5 years, and then increasing again at 6 years. Children's average linear slope was decreasing, suggesting that children were becoming less reactive over time (M linear slope = –1.24, p < .001), from an intercept of 2.12 ln(ms)2 at age 3 years. Significant variability in these parameters demonstrated that these patterns differed across children (σ2 intercept = 0.08, p < .001; σ2 linear slope = 0.14, p = .03). Children, on average, also exhibited a significant quadratic trend representing the decreases at ages 4 and 5 and the increase at age 6 (M quadratic = 0.15, p < .001) with significant variability suggesting differences across children in this pattern over time (σ2 quadratic = .001, p = .05).
ND
Details regarding the unconditional model of ND can be found in Fisher et al (Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011). In brief, there were significant individual differences in the trajectories of adolescent behavioral dysregulation and executive function difficulties. There was nonsignificant mean growth in behavioral dysregulation, whereas executive function difficulties decreased over time.
Effects of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on growth in baseline RSA and ND
We next entered the main effects of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on growth in baseline RSA on the behavioral dysregulation and executive dysfunction components of ND. Child sex was entered as a covariate in all models. The factor variance for the baseline RSA slope was fixed at 0 owing to its very low and nonsignificant value. We therefore did not include any predictors of growth in baseline RSA nor was growth in baseline RSA included as a predictor of behavioral dysregulation or executive dysfunction. This constraint did not affect model fit and improved the estimation of the overall model.
The main effect model for behavioral dysregulation showed acceptable fit (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.03), χ2 (33) = 70.98, p < .001. The findings indicate that greater prenatal substance exposure was associated with greater baseline RSA at age 3 (b = 0.14, p < .001) and greater increases in behavioral dysregulation over time (b = 0.15, p = .01). Greater levels of early adversity were related to higher initial levels of behavioral dyregulation at age 8/9 (b = 0.27, p < .001). Males had greater levels of behavioral dysregulation at age 8/9 than did females (b = –0.15, p < .001). There were no significant effects of RSA on initial status or growth in behavioral dysregulation.
The main effect model for the executive dysfunction outcome also demonstrated acceptable fit (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.02), χ2 (33) = 54.37, p = .01. Greater levels of prenatal substance exposure were related to greater baseline RSA at 3 years (b = 0.14, p < .001). There was only one significant association between the substantive covariates and the executive function difficulties construct. Greater levels of early adversity were associated with greater executive function difficulties at age 8/9 (b = 0.18, p < .001). There were no significant effects of RSA on initial levels or growth in executive dysfunction.
We then examined interactions between cumulative substance and baseline RSA at 3 years on initial status and growth in behavioral dysregulation, and the interaction between early adversity and baseline RSA at 3 years on initial status and growth in behavioral dysregulation (in separate models). No significant interaction effects emerged. We also examined these interactions on initial status and growth in executive dysfunction. As with behavioral dysregulation, there were no significant interaction effects.
Effects of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on growth in RSA reactivity and ND
We repeated the above analyses with RSA reactivity, prenatal substance exposure, and early adversity as predictors of growth in the behavioral dysregulation and executive dysfunction components of ND. The main effect model for behavioral dysregulation showed acceptable fit (CFI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.03), χ2 (20) = 40.28, p = .05. Greater exposure to early adversity was related to less RSA reactivity at 3 years (b = –0.32, p < .001), increased linear growth in RSA reactivity from 3 to 6 years (b = 0.29, p = .01), but not quadratic growth (b = –18, p = .10). In contrast, greater prenatal substance exposure was associated with greater RSA reactivity at 3 years (b = 0.30, p < .001), less linear growth in RSA reactivity (b = –0.27, p = .01), and greater quadratic growth (e.g., greater accelerations) of RSA reactivity from 3 to 6 years (b = 0.25, p = .03). Higher drug exposure was associated with less RSA reactivity at 4 and 5 years, followed by a steeper increase in RSA reactivity at age 6. Males exhibited more behavioral dysregulation at age 8/9 (b = –0.16, p < .001). Higher levels of early adversity were associated with greater behavioral dysregulation at age 8/9 (b = 0.24, p = .002). There were no main effects of RSA reactivity on initial levels or growth in behavioral dysregulation.
The main effect model for executive dysfunction showed good fit (CFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.02), χ2 (20) = 27.22, p > .05. Greater exposure to early adversity was related to less RSA reactivity at 3 years (b = –0.32, p < .001), increased linear growth in RSA reactivity from 3 to 6 years (b = 0.28, p = .01), but not quadratic growth (b = –0.18, p = .09). In contrast, greater prenatal substance exposure was associated with greater RSA reactivity at 3 years (b = 0.30, p < .001), less linear growth in RSA reactivity (b = –0.26, p = .01), and greater quadratic growth (e.g., greater accelerations) of RSA reactivity from 3 to 6 years, b = 0.24, p = .03. Higher drug exposure was associated with less RSA reactivity at 4 and 5 years followed by a steeper increase in RSA reactivity at age 6. There was only one significant association between the substantive covariates and the executive function difficulties construct. Greater levels of early adversity were associated with greater executive function difficulties at age 8/9 (b = 0.18, p = .02).
In separate models, we then examined the interaction between growth in RSA reactivity and prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on initial status and growth in behavioral dysregulation. There were no significant interactions between prenatal substance exposure and linear growth, or prenatal substance exposure and quadratic growth in RSA reactivity, on behavioral dysregulation intercept or linear slope. We next examined interactions between early adversity and linear and quadratic growth in RSA reactivity on initial status and growth in behavioral dysregulation. There was a significant interaction between early adversity and quadratic growth in RSA reactivity (but not with initial status or linear growth; see Figure 1). The inclusion of this interaction resulted in a significantly better model fit (Table 3). We probed this interaction by plotting RSA reactivity scores one standard deviation above and below the mean (Figure 2). The effect of early adversity on growth in behavioral dysregulation depended on quadratic growth in RSA reactivity. Children with decelerations in RSA reactivity exhibited growth in behavioral dysregulation regardless of whether they were exposed to early adversity. Children who exhibited accelerations in RSA reactivity, however, showed increases in behavioral dysregulation, but only if they were also raised in environments of high early life adversity. Children who exhibited accelerations in RSA reactivity and who were raised in environments of less early adversity exhibited no significant growth in behavioral dysregulation. We calculated the effect size as the proportion of explained variance using the following formula (Feingold, Reference Feingold2009):
Note: Only the main and interaction effects as predictors of growth in behavioral dysregulation are reported. RSA-R, respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity; SSABIC, sample size adjusted Bayesian information criterion; AIC, Akaike information criterion; EA, early adversity.
The effect size (R 2) was .40, indicating that the interaction explained approximately 40% of the variance in growth in behavior dysregulation.
Next, we examined whether there were interaction effects on executive function difficulties. The first model included the interaction between prenatal substance exposure and change in RSA reactivity on initial status and change in executive function difficulties. The second included the interaction between early adversity and change in RSA reactivity on initial status and change in executive function difficulties. Neither of the interaction effects was significant.
Discussion
The overarching goal of this study was to examine whether a biological marker of emotion regulation would aid in the identification of children who are most susceptible to the development of ND. We also added to the current literature by examining how interactions across environmental and physiological levels of analysis contribute to the development of ND. RSA reactivity moderated the relation between early adversity and trajectories of behavior dysregulation, a component of ND, suggesting that one pathway to the development of behavior dysregulation involves exposure to early adversity and accelerated growth in RSA reactivity across early childhood. In addition, we added to the ND literature by assessing how RSA growth trajectories contribute to growth in ND components. In our main effects models, we found that increases in prenatal substance exposure were associated with greater baseline RSA and RSA reactivity at age 3, linear growth in behavioral dysregulation, and quadratic growth in RSA reactivity that reflected a more rapid increase in RSA reactivity at 5 and 6 years compared with children with lower levels of prenatal substance exposure. Greater exposure to early adversity was related to decreased RSA reactivity at age 3, linear increases in RSA reactivity from ages 3 to 6 years, and increased behavioral dysregulation and executive function difficulties at age 8/9. Below we discuss additional contributions of this study.
Greater prenatal substance exposure and less early adversity were related to greater RSA reactivity at 3 years and less linear growth in RSA reactivity from 3 to 6 years. In typically developing populations, moderate withdrawal of RSA is considered functional (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001). That children with prenatal substance exposure exhibited less linear growth in RSA reactivity suggests the possibility that these children may be developing a profile of reduced autonomic functioning that is typically seen among children with externalizing disorders (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Sylvers, Mead and Chipman-Chacon2006). Compared to children with less prenatal substance exposure, another possibility is that this pattern of less RSA reactivity over time reflects difficulties prenatally exposed children may have with exhibiting the expected withdrawal of RSA in response to an acute stressor, which may be a risk factor for the development of problem behavior (Calkins & Dedmon, Reference Calkins and Dedmon2000; Gentzler, Santucci, Kovacs, & Fox, Reference Gentzler, Santucci, Kovacs and Fox2009; Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, Portales, & Greenspan, Reference Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, Portales and Greenspan1996).
It was surprising to us that the effects of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on growth in RSA reactivity manifested in opposite ways. When examining correlations, these prenatal substance exposure and early adversity exhibit opposite effects on RSA reactivity at age 3, but by age 6 the direction of effect is the same. These findings highlight the importance of examining RSA across multiple developmental periods, as patterns change with development. With greater exposure to adverse experiences over time, there may be a reorganization that occurs in the parasympathetic system. These results support the hypothesis that the effect of early adversity may be latent owing to its impact on specific areas of the brain that are not online until childhood when the child must organize resources to respond to more complex demands (Lester et al., Reference Lester, Tronick, LaGasse, Seifer, Bauer and Shankaran2002; Shonkoff, Boyce, & McEwen, Reference Shonkoff, Boyce and McEwen2009). As children develop, they face more demands at home, school, and with peers, and it is important that they learn to organize neurobiological resources in order to achieve competence in multiple settings (Lester, LaGasse, & Seifer, Reference Lester, LaGasse and Seifer1998).
It is interesting that there were no main effects of RSA on the ND components, though RSA reactivity interacted with early adversity to predict individual differences in growth of behavior dysregulation. RSA reactivity may have stronger links with emotion regulation compared to baseline RSA, because RSA reactivity is related to the organism's ability to engage and disengage with the environment (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Doussard-Roosevelt et al., Reference Doussard-Roosevelt, Montgomery and Porges2003). Therefore, it is less surprising that RSA reactivity, compared to baseline RSA, would have a stronger relation with behavior dysregulation because emotion dysregulation is one component of ND. Furthermore, although there was significant variability in baseline RSA at age 3, the lack of variability of growth in baseline RSA precluded our ability to estimate whether it was a predictor of individual differences in the ND components.
It was only through examining growth in RSA reactivity that the relation between early adversity and behavior dysregulation became clear. Decelerated growth in RSA reactivity was related to increases in behavior dysregulation, regardless of the child's exposure to early adversity. This finding highlights the importance of understanding how rates of physiological maturation over time may be associated with increased risk for the development of behavioral dysregulation. Although a faster maturation of the stress response system is thought to be impacted by processes of allostatic load (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998), it appears as though decelerations could also be problematic. Decelerations in RSA reactivity manifest as a flat, or blunted, response from age 3 to age 6. The correlates of this “blunted” RSA response may be similar to that of the neuroendocrine system. A blunted cortisol response to stress is associated with greater exposure to early adversity (Fisher, Stoolmiller, Gunnar, & Burraston, Reference Fisher, Stoolmiller, Gunnar and Burraston2007) and prenatal substance exposure (Lester et al., Reference Lester, Lagasse, Shankaran, Bada, Bauer and Lin2010), presumably because of wear and tear on the neuroendocrine system. This is the first study that we know of suggesting that a blunted RSA response may also be problematic. In addition to the effects of early adversity and prenatal substance exposure on this pattern of responding, there may be as yet unmeasured environmental or epigenetic influences. By epigenetic we mean the inheritance of information based on gene expression control rather than gene sequence (Lester et al., Reference Lester, Lin, Degarmo, Fisher, Lagasse and Levine2012). For instance, the theory of biological sensitivity to context (Boyce & Ellis, Reference Boyce and Ellis2005) suggests that it is through prenatal programming that stress response systems are formed, with the fetus “learning” to upregulate a response to stress if the intrauterine environment forecasts a particularly positive or negative postnatal environment, as a way to adapt to this environment. Perhaps this blunted RSA response was adaptive earlier in life or even in utero. We may also have identified a group of children who are chronically physiologically underaroused and thus may be more sensation seeking or aggressive (Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, Lacasse, & Colletti, Reference Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, Lacasse and Colletti2000).
Children who exhibited accelerated growth in RSA reactivity over time and who had greater exposure to early adversity exhibited increases in behavioral dysregulation. Perhaps the combination of accelerated growth in RSA reactivity and experiencing adversity early in life reflects a “double jeopardy” that places the child at increased risk for the development of behavioral dysregulation. The theory of biological sensitivity to context suggests that greater reactivity may allow the child to be more hypervigilant and aware of his/her surroundings, which may be an adaptive response to a chaotic or unpredictable environment. Because we only measured a negative environment in this study, we cannot also test whether accelerations in RSA reactivity are related to positive outcomes in supportive environments.
However, there were no significant associations with RSA and executive dysfunction. Previous reports measured RSA and executive functions concurrently or RSA measured at one point in time. No studies that we know of examined developmental trajectories of RSA in early childhood and executive dysfunction in middle childhood. RSA may not be as accurate a predictor of executive dysfunction compared to behavior dysregulation. RSA may be more strongly tied to emotion systems and emotion regulation than it is to cognitive control systems per se.
The results from this study raise interesting questions regarding the bottom-up and/or top-down processes involved in the development of ND (Lester et al., Reference Lester, Lin, Degarmo, Fisher, Lagasse and Levine2012). Altered executive control systems, thought to reflect a higher order cognitive process, may result in problems with emotion regulation (i.e., problems with inhibitory control and impulsivity) and subsequently individual differences in RSA, reflecting a top-down process in the development of ND. Children who exhibit more symptoms of ND exhibit greater activation in these frontal regions (McNamee et al., Reference McNamee, Dunfee, Luna, Clark, Eddy and Tarter2008), suggesting that more activation of these executive control systems is needed to negotiate changing environmental demands. In contrast, children with less control over their emotions (i.e., those who exhibit excessive RSA reactivity), may, through interactions over time with exposure to environmental adversity, develop problems with executive control systems (i.e., poor inhibitory control and planning), thereby impacting the development of ND, suggesting a bottom-up process might also be involved. Although we cannot infer direction of effect with these data, results from this study provide preliminary support for the hypothesis that accelerated growth in RSA reactivity in combination with exposure to early adversity is involved in the development of ND. Early adversity may affect frontal regions associated with emotion regulation (Kishiyama et al., Reference Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry and Knight2009), thereby impacting individual differences in RSA reactivity and increasing the likelihood for the development of ND (e.g., a bottom-up process). These effects might also be bidirectional, with exposure to early adversity impacting frontal cortex inhibitory processes (Kishiyama et al., Reference Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry and Knight2009), thereby compromising behavioral control of emotion and subsequently physiological systems of regulation (a “top-down”) process. Although we did not measure neurological control in this study, future prospective studies are needed to elucidate these potential mechanisms to vulnerability of ND through neurological pathways.
These results also raise important questions regarding the context in which RSA is assessed and how this context may or may not relate to the separate ND components of behavioral dysregulation and executive dysfunction. In some studies, RSA reactivity to emotionally evocative tasks was related to aspects of behavior dysregulation involving externalizing behavior (El-Sheikh, Reference El-Sheikh2001; El-Sheikh, Harger, & Whitson, Reference El-Sheikh, Harger and Whitson2001), whereas in others only RSA reactivity to attention-demanding tasks were related to behavior dysregulation (Calkins & Keane, Reference Calkins and Keane2004; Hinnant & El-Sheikh, Reference Hinnant and El-Sheikh2009). It appears as though stressors perceived as attention demanding may also tax emotion systems in addition to cognitive or attention systems, perhaps even more so than tasks that researchers judge to be emotionally evocative. In contrast, behavior dysregulation can be an amorphous and at times all-encompassing construct, and without measures of this dysregulation from multiple informants, we will have difficulty finding relevant physiological predictors or correlates of dysregulation. Although at this point the literature is equivocal, it is important in future work to validate paradigms researchers hypothesize as stressful and to incorporate multiple stressors that tax emotion, cognitive, and attention systems (see Alkon et al., Reference Alkon, Goldstein, Smider, Essex, Kupfer and Boyce2003, as an example of such work).
An important limitation of the current study is our assessment of one measure of physiology. Multiple measures of biological processes should be used in future studies to test relevant developmental models (Granger & Kivlighan, Reference Granger and Kivlighan2003). In addition, although most of our physiological measures were evaluated before symptom assessments, we still cannot infer causality with these data. Future studies using a cross-lagged approach will be helpful in understanding whether a physiological process at one point in time influences behavior later in development. We also did not isolate the effects of a single substance, such as cocaine, on RSA, and instead used a summary score approach, which may be a more ecologically valid indicator of prenatal substance use (Mayet et al., Reference Mayet, Groshkova, Morgan, MacCormack and Strang2008). We acknowledge that the mechanism of action of these drugs is different, and we cannot study the relative impact of one drug over another. Future studies could also examine prenatal substance exposure using a latent factor approach as a way to reduce measurement error. However, our interest was in polydrug exposure as used previously (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin and Shankaran2011) as a marker for cumulative intrauterine stress (Lester & Padbury, Reference Lester and Padbury2009), which is why we used a summary score approach. Another limitation includes our measurement of baseline RSA. We did not assess RSA while the child was at “rest,” but we instead chose to compare RSA activity during an attention-demanding task with RSA activity when a child was playing with a simple toy and experimental demands were minimal. Although our estimates of baseline RSA converge with other studies using the Porges method, we nonetheless note that our baseline measurement did not measure parasympathetic activity at rest. We also did not include a measure of movement as a covariate in our physiological analyses, which may have impacted heart rate and in turn RSA. In addition, we chose measures of executive dysfunction that have been shown to be related to brain areas related to deficiencies among children exposed prenatally to drugs (Sheinkopf et al., Reference Sheinkopf, Lester, Sanes, Eliassen, Hutchison and Seifer2009); however, there may have been others (such as inhibitory control) that could have been assessed. Furthermore, although we have added breadth to our understanding of the development of ND, it came at the expense of depth. We are unable to ascertain, for instance, whether growth in RSA reactivity at earlier ages is a stronger contributor to ND than growth at later ages. Likewise, we do not know whether growth in RSA reactivity more strongly influences ND at a particular time point (for instance, during preschool) more so than at later ages. Microlevel approaches are needed if we are to understand when in development a child may be most at risk for a dysregualted physiological profile (Beauchaine, Neuhaus, et al., Reference Beauchaine, Neuhaus, Brenner and Gatzke-Kopp2008). Nevertheless, this study has added specificity to our understanding of how Biology × Environment interactions contribute to disease risk.
In sum, we extended our understanding of how transactions between physiology and environmental context contribute to the development of ND. We used a multifaceted model taking into account the dynamic interactions that occur among physiological functioning, early adverse experiences, and prenatal substance exposure to predict the development of ND across preschool, childhood, and adolescence. This study builds on previous work by incorporating how RSA, early adversity, prenatal substance exposure, and their interactions predict the development of ND. The results from this study have illuminated some interesting pathways to the development of ND among a sample of children heterogeneous for exposure to substances prenatally and early adverse experiences. Longitudinal studies such as the one presented in this investigation are needed to elucidate transactional pathways leading to adverse outcomes. This Biology × Environment interaction is of critical importance to early interventionists because it suggests that the child most in need of intervention is one with greater RSA reactivity who is reared in a risky early environment. We hope this work has opened new avenues of research as well as clarified who might be most at risk for the development of a disinhibited profile. This information may then be used to add specificity and therefore cost-effectiveness to sorely needed preventative interventions for this group of underserved children.