Although there have been a number of different conceptual and empirical approaches to the study of the effects of parenting on child adjustment, such work is often conducted from within a developmental psychopathology framework (e.g., Cummings, Davies, & Campbell, Reference Cummings, Davies and Campbell2000; Rutter & Sroufe, Reference Rutter and Sroufe2000). This perspective suggests that there are multiple contributors to maladaptive and adaptive outcomes, that these contributors may interact in various ways within different individuals, and that the consequences for development are multiple pathways to disordered behavior and/or multiple variants of outcome from individual causative factors (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Reference Cicchetti1993; Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984). Cicchetti and others (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Richters, Reference Richters1997) have described these perspectives as multifinality and equifinality. This perspective also highlights the need for complex, process-oriented models of development and emphasizes the importance of conducting longitudinal investigations of the factors that may both influence and be influenced by early contextual, familial, or individual difference factors. Finally, a developmental psychopathology perspective advocates an organizational view of development; multiple factors, or levels of a given factor, are considered in the context of one another rather than in isolation (Cicchetti & Dawson, Reference Cicchetti and Dawson2002).
Current research on child psychopathology conducted within a developmental psychopathology framework has been informed by a large literature on the family factors that influence child adjustment versus maladjustment (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Davies and Campbell2000). Much of this extant research has focused specifically on parenting behavior as a causal factor in the emergence of problematic child behavior. The general but consistent finding that emerges from this literature is that poor quality parenting is a significant risk factor for childhood psychopathology (Rothbaum & Weisz, Reference Rothbaum and Weisz1994). Much of this work has addressed the role of harsh, controlling parenting and the emergence of childhood externalizing problems, including aggression, oppositional behavior, and conduct problems (Gershoff, Reference Gershoff2002; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, Reference Patterson, Reid and Dishion1992). Other work has examined the role of factors that moderate parenting, including characteristics of the child, such as temperament (Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge, Reference Bates, Petit, Dodge and Ridge1998) and biological vulnerability (Erath, El-Sheik, Hinnant, & Cummings, Reference Erath, El-Sheikh, Hinnant and Cummings2011).
As the literature examining parenting and child psychopathology has grown, there has been a shift in its emphasis and in several different directions. Early work focused on parenting and parent functioning as risk factors, with descriptive empirical work largely emphasizing main effects. Speculation on the developmental processes and specific mechanisms involved was often absent. Recent work has been more process-oriented, acknowledging that the relations between caregiving and child functioning were dynamic and transactional and often resulted in multiple pathways and different outcomes (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Davies and Campbell2000). Greater emphasis has also been placed on the actual mechanisms that might be involved, observing that, although parenting does affect child functioning, greater attention needed to be paid to how parenting affects child adjustment outcomes (Davies & Cummings, Reference Davies and Cummings1998). More attention has focused on the specific developmental pathways that are observed with respect to different, individual dimensions of both parenting and child outcomes (Meunier, Bisceglia & Jenkins, Reference Meunier, Bisceglia and Jenkins2012). Finally, as the developmental psychopathology framework became instantiated in research, it was clear that the biological level of analysis needed to be incorporated into empirical work and in a much more nuanced way than using traditional correlational and extreme group approaches (Hill-Soderlund et al., Reference Hill-Soderlund, Mills-Koonce, Propper, Calkins, Granger and Moore2008).
Thus, although historically the emphasis on developmental pathways to child psychopathology has been on the role of family processes, growing interest has focused on the biological processes implicated in emerging child behavior more generally and in competencies and maladjustment in particular. For example, behavior genetics research has clearly stimulated growing appreciation for the role of genetic processes in emerging child psychopathology (cf. Horwitz & Neiderhiser, Reference Horwitz and Neiderhiser2011). This appreciation has already led many developmental scientists to reconfigure their models of development to incorporate both genetic and other biological processes as factors in the emergence of complex behavior (Sameroff, Reference Sameroff2010; Shonkoff, Reference Shonkoff2010). Much of this theory building in developmental psychopathology has also been informed by existing conceptual and empirical work in the discipline of developmental psychobiology, an approach that is interdisciplinary and integrative, combining perspectives from evolutionary and developmental biology and physiology with subdisciplines of psychology to address questions about behavior and its development in humans and nonhuman animals (Michel & Moore, Reference Michel and Moore1995). It is this perspective that has had a major influence on subsequent empirical investigations of family processes that have adopted the tenets of developmental psychopathology (cf. Propper et al., Reference Propper, Moore, Mills-Koonce, Halpern, Hill and Calkins2008). This conceptual integration has led to greater appreciation of the complex role of biological processes in transactional models of development that seek to account for the emergence of patterns of adjustment and maladjustment across multiple levels of child functioning.
Psychobiological approaches to the study of early behavioral development, whether animal or human, remind us that a range of biological mechanisms are implicated in functioning at all levels of analysis. Gottlieb's depiction of probabilistic epigenesis (cf. Gottlieb, Reference Gottlieb1991, Reference Gottlieb2007) exemplifies the approach; it describes the changes that occur across developmental time in genetic, neural, behavioral, and environmental levels of analysis and depicts the bidirectional effects across time from one level to another. Gottlieb's model has stimulated both conceptual and empirical work aimed at providing greater specificity with regard to how the broad model may be instantiated to explain human developmental phenomena. Some of this work has attempted to “unpack” the action within and across levels to describe, for example, how gene action at the level of cellular activity occurs and influences numerous types of neural processes, some of which may ultimately be involved in producing complex behavior (Johnston & Edwards, Reference Johnston and Edwards2002) of the sort that is often referred to as child adjustment outcomes. Recent empirical work has also adopted Gottlieb's approach and begun to measure transactions across two or more of these biological levels, for example, from the gene (through molecular genetic analyses that specify candidate genes) to specific physiological processes (like parasympathetic regulation of cardiac output under stress) and how these relations may be moderated by dimensions of the environment, such as parenting across time (cf. Propper et al., Reference Propper, Moore, Mills-Koonce, Halpern, Hill and Calkins2008). Regardless of the focus of the research or the levels described or assessed, the underlying assumption in this work is the same: Genes initiate developmental pathways that are receptive to influences that occur from the cellular to the social contextual level, and all are candidates for developmental interactions that may influence pathways to child outcomes (cf. Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Thibodeau, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Thibodeau2012). Thus, coaction, transaction, and interaction across levels characterize development in a biologically informed model of child, and thus family, functioning (Sameroff, Reference Sameroff2010).
A biopsychosocial model motivates multilevel research that incorporates biological and behavioral indicators of the factors that may be implicated in complex developmental pathways to outcomes such as child psychopathology. Here, we review some of the research that informs such an approach. We focus on two pathways to child psychopathology: those in which genetic processes are moderated by the caregiving environment and those that examine physiological processes as a mediator of caregiver behavior and child outcomes.
Genes, Environments, and Their Interaction
It is well established that parental psychopathology and impaired parent–child relationships are among the primary risk factors for compromised child emotional and behavioral health (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Davies and Campbell2000). However, there is variation in the extant literature regarding the nature and strength of this link between family processes and specific mental health outcomes. Although some children who are exposed to poor-quality parenting early in life develop symptoms of psychopathology over time, many others do not. One reason for this discrepancy may be the influence of child genotype on outcomes; specific genetic effects on phenotypes may be conditional on particular environments and experiences and be unobserved under other conditions. Alternatively, exposure to certain experiences and environments may be influenced by the child's genotype (e.g., the way others behave toward them; activities they choose or that caregivers choose for them). It has become increasingly clear that child development and adjustment cannot be understood without taking into account both genes and environment as well as the relationship between them. Although there are several ways in which genotypes (measured at the molecular level) and environments may work together (Moffitt, Caspi, & Rutter, Reference Moffitt, Caspi and Rutter2006) to influence outcomes, here we focus on examples of just one, Gene × Environment interactions (G × E).
Although behavioral genetics studies laid the groundwork for what we know about the unique influence of genes and the environment on childhood behavior (see review by Horwitz & Neiderhiser, Reference Horwitz and Neiderhiser2011), it is only relatively recently that we have been able to examine the influence of specific genetic alleles on behavior. This molecular genetics approach, which attempts to make associations between specific genes that regulate neurotransmitter systems and complex behavioral traits, provides more precise information about which genes may affect the development of both normal and pathological behaviors and possibly by what mechanism such effects occur. However, progress in this area has been hampered by the large number of mixed findings and failed replications. One reason for the difficulty in replicating genetic effects on complex behavioral outcomes is the lack of attention paid to environmental or experiential factors. Genes and environmental factors, or life experiences, must be considered from very early in development, because the bidirectional relationship between them may place individuals on distinct trajectories that will affect long-term development of mental health and disorders.
Over the past decade, researchers have embraced the importance of measuring both genes and environments and their interplay in order to predict both normative and pathological outcomes. Although some of these have focused on more distal factors (e.g., family social support, socioeconomic; Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Javdani, Jackson, Reynolds, Potenza and Gelernter2010; Tucker-Drob & Harden, Reference Tucker-Drob and Harden2012), there have been a number of studies that have focused on the critical environmental context of the caregiving experience. The first such investigations were done with nonhuman animals and retrospective studies of human adults. Several pioneering animal studies focused on the serotonin transporter gene, due to diminished serotonin transporter gene (5-HTT) transcription, lower transporter levels, and reduced serotonin uptake, with functional effects on neural circuits regulated by serotonin for those animals possessing the short allele (Hariri et al., Reference Hariri, Mattay, Tessitore, Kolachana, Fera and Goldman2002). Several findings revealed that rhesus monkeys who were raised by peers, rather than mothers, exhibited more behavioral and physiological problems (i.e., alcohol consumption, stress reactivity) and deficits in self-regulation (e.g., impulsivity, inappropriate aggression, orienting problems, risk taking) if they possessed the risk allele of the 5-HTT linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR; short/short or short/long) instead of the nonrisk allele (long/long). These findings suggest that poor quality early experience was only predictive of maladaptive outcomes for monkeys at genetic risk. For those monkeys who experienced a natural and supportive mother–infant relationship there was no effect of genotype (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Newman, Shannon, Parker, Dvoskin and Becker2004; Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Lesch, Heils, Long, Lorenz and Shoaf2002; Champoux et al., Reference Champoux, Bennett, Shannon, Higley, Lesch and Suomi2002; Suomi, Reference Suomi2004, Reference Suomi, Tremblay, Hartup and Archer2005, Reference Suomi2006).
Similarly, in two landmark studies of G × E in human adults, Caspi et al. (Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin and Craig2002) found that early adverse experience alone did not predict adult psychopathology. As with the animal studies described above, adults carrying the risk (short/short or short/long) allele of serotonin were more likely to be depressed when they experienced stressful life events than were adults without the short allele or those with the short allele who did not experience stressful life events (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Sugden, Moffitt, Taylor, Craig and Harrington2003). In addition, another study by this group found childhood maltreatment alone did not predict antisocial behavior in adulthood but that there was a significantly higher chance of developing later antisocial behavior for those individuals who possessed the risk allele of the functional polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene and also experienced maltreatment. Although these studies were the first to find such gene–environment interactions within a molecular genetics framework, a number of other studies followed attempting to replicate and extend these results to include other genes and environments across various developmental stages. In some cases findings were replicated (Foley et al., Reference Foley, Eaves, Wormley, Silberg, Maes and Kuhn2004; Frazzetto et al., Reference Frazzetto, Di Lorenzo, Carola, Proietti, Sokolowska and Siracusano2007; Kim-Cohen et al., Reference Kim-Cohen, Caspi, Taylor, Williams, Newcombe and Craig2006; Widom & Brzustowicz Reference Widom and Brzustowicz2006), and in others they failed to replicate (Haberstick et al., Reference Haberstick, Lessem, Hopfer, Smolen, Ehringer and Timberlake2005; Huizinga et al., Reference Huizinga, Haberstick, Smolen, Menard, Young and Corley2006; Young et al., Reference Young, Smolen, Hewitt, Haberstick, Stallings and Corley2006), again suggesting the need to take unmeasured environmental factors into account. Although animal and adult human studies laid the groundwork for this area of study, the way in which G × E processes predict childhood behavior and maladaptive outcomes is still in the early stages. Two ways in which the field has attempted to apply a G × E approach to understand the emergence of childhood psychopathology is via G × E studies that (a) predict infant, toddler, and early childhood characteristics and behaviors, often associated with temperament or self-regulation, that are known to be precursors of later psychopathology, or (b) directly predict pathological outcomes in middle to late childhood.
G × E influence on infant and toddler temperament and behavior
Behavioral processes that emerge and develop over the first years of life are critical for subsequent adaptive social behavior, and thus it is important to understand the development of these early child-level factors that may be precursors for later psychopathology (Calkins & Dedmon, Reference Calkins and Dedmon2000; Calkins & Fox, Reference Calkins and Fox2002). Many of these processes fall under the rubric of temperament, defined as individual differences in reactivity and regulation (Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, Reference Rothbart, Ahadi and Evans2000), a construct believed to be a proxy for genotype but which, for the most part, has been studied behaviorally or via behavior genetic designs, such as twin or adoption studies (Goldsmith & Hewitt, Reference Goldsmith and Hewitt2003; Goldsmith, Lemery, Buss, & Campos, Reference Goldsmith, Lemery, Buss and Campos1999). Recent advances in molecular genetics have led to the identification of alleles of specific genes that may underlie domains of infant and child temperament (e.g., effortful control; Posner, Rothbart, & Sheese, Reference Posner, Rothbart and Sheese2007), but only a few investigations have looked at parenting as a moderator of the relationship between genes and behavioral outcomes with success (e.g., self-control; Wright, Schnupp, Beaver, Delisi, & Vaughn, Reference Wright, Schnupp, Beaver, Delisi and Vaughn2012; noncompliance; Sulik et al., Reference Sulik, Eisenberg, Lemery-Chalfant, Spinrad, Silva and Eggum2012) and without success (e.g., effortful control; Sheese, Voelker, Rothbart, & Posner, Reference Sheese, Voelker, Rothbart and Posner2007).
For example, behavioral inhibition (a fearful temperament or style of reacting when confronted with novelty) has been identified as a precursor of psychopathology in young adulthood (Caspi, Moffitt, Newman, & Silva, Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Newman and Silva1996). The first study to predict behavioral inhibition using a G × E model found that children with the risk allele of the polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter 5-HTT gene and low levels of social support early in life were at increased risk for behavioral inhibition at 7 years of age (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Nichols, Henderson, Rubin, Schmidt and Hamer2005). However, although low levels of social support are theoretically associated with greater difficulties in parenting, this study did not measure the specific parenting behaviors that may have mediated the relationship between social support and behavioral inhibition.
More recently, a study linking another gene, the MAOA length polymorphic region (MAOA-LPR, a 30 base pair repeat in the promoter region of MAOA that affects transcriptional efficiency), to behavioral disinhibition in childhood aimed to identify specific experiences that may moderate the relationship and differentiate between the influence of a broad array of early family adverse experiences and stressful life events experienced in utero to 7 years of age (Enoch, Steer, Newman, Gibson, & Goldman; Reference Enoch, Steer, Newman, Gibson and Goldman2010). Although this study also did not directly examine parenting, it provides an example of the way in which experience (related to the family or caregiving environment) may interact with genotype. Stressful life events, measured here as the sum of impact scores on 15 to 18 different stressful life events ranging from low to extreme severity (e.g., pet died, new sibling, physical hurt, sexual abuse) and family adversity operationalized as the sum of such experiences as maternal psychopathology, interparental cruelty, parental substance misuse, and at risk/taken into care, were examined in conjunction with MAOA. Several complex interactions were revealed: the low activity allele of MAOA-LPR was associated with hyperactivity at ages 4 and 7 years for girls exposed to more stressful life events between 6 months and 3.5 years. Boys showed a slightly different timing pattern: Low activity MAOA-LPR interacted with stressful life events between ages 1.5 and 2.5 to predict hyperactivity at age 7. In contrast, behavioral disinhibition was not predicted by any interactions between early-life family adversity and MAOA-LPR (although family adversity over the first year of life did lead to more hyperactivity at ages 4 and 7). The authors conclude that it may be the first year of life when children are most susceptible to family adversity and in the first 3 years when stressful life events matter most. These findings emphasize the complexity of examining G × E interactions to predict behavioral outcomes from a developmental perspective. It appears that different types of difficult experiences (here divided into “stressful life events” and “family adversity”) may interact with genes at different time points during development differently for males and females to influence behavior. Understanding these timing effects has important implications for developing appropriate preventions and interventions.
Other domains of infant temperament and behavior have also been the focus of G × E analyses. In one study, sensation seeking over the first 2 years of life was studied in children 18 to 21 months of age possessing the risk polymorphism of the dopamine receptor D4 gene (L-DRD4) of the DRD4 exon III variable number tandem repeat gene (a 48 base pair repeat; Sheese et al., Reference Sheese, Voelker, Rothbart and Posner2007). Those children whose mothers were observed as less sensitive caregivers exhibited more sensation seeking behavior (as reported by the mother) than did children possessing the S-DRD4 allele or with the L-DRD4 allele who had more sensitive mothers. It is interesting that those children with the L-DRD4 polymorphism who received higher quality parenting were reported to have the least sensation-seeking behavior of all the groups. Furthermore, children who did not possess the L-DRD4 polymorphism were unaffected by parenting, which potentially provides support for the differential susceptibility hypothesis discussed above. Although the results of this study do not differentiate between the specific aspects of parenting that were most critical for this relationship, due to the broad measure of “parenting quality” and because the parenting rating scales were so highly correlated, it does provide further evidence that parenting matters. These results are also in line with previous findings linking dopamine genes to sensation-seeking or risk-taking behaviors.
Finally, in a study of effortful control as an indicator of self-regulation, Kochanska, Philibert, and Barry (Reference Kochanska, Philibert and Barry2009) found that children's attachment security moderated the effect of the risk allele of 5-HTTLPR (short/short or short/long) on a child's ability to self-regulate. Diminished self-regulatory capacity was observed from age 2 to 4.5 but only for children with the risk allele who were insecurely attached to their mothers at the end of the first year. This effect was absent for securely attached children; thus, attachment security appears to act as a buffer against risk conferred by genotype. Attachment security may enhance effective emotional arousal modulation in children who are genetically less well equipped to handle this task (Herrmann et al., Reference Herrmann, Huter, Muller, Muhlberger, Pauli and Reif2007; Hofer, Reference Hofer1987). Children with low-risk genotypes may have more effective physiological regulation, and caregiver support may be less important.
Support for the latter argument is found in a study of the dopamine receptor D2 gene (DRD2) on infant physiological responses to stress over the first year of life in relation to infants' experiences with caregivers (Propper et al., Reference Propper, Moore, Mills-Koonce, Halpern, Hill and Calkins2008). At 3 and 6 months of age, infants with the taq1 A1 polymorphism of the DRD2 gene (risk), associated with impulse control problems and sensation-seeking behaviors, did not exhibit expected physiological regulation during an age-appropriate stressful situation (i.e., separation from mother) as measured by vagal reactivity (i.e., decrease in respiratory sinus arrhythmia [RSA]). However, results revealed that maternal caregiving behavior moderated the change in RSA response to stress by 12 months of age. Those infants possessing the taq1 A1 polymorphism of DRD2 who were also exposed to sensitive maternal caregiving over the first year of life exhibited a more optimal and expected RSA response to stress at 12 months of age, comparable to the RSA reactivity of those infants possessing the nonrisk version of the gene. Infants without the risk allele displayed typical and effective RSA response to stress whether or not mothers were sensitive, suggesting that the caregiving environment may actually be less important for their regulatory outcomes.
Implications of G × E for developmental psychopathology
Over the past decade, there has been growing interest in predicting childhood psychopathology using G × E models. Although some studies have looked at genetic effects within the context of more extreme examples of poor parenting, such as maltreatment (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin and Craig2002) or physical discipline (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Dodge, Latendresse, Lansford, Bates and Pettit2010), as predictors of aggression, antisocial behavior, and delinquency, others have looked at family and caregiving factors within the normal range. Here we highlight some of the work focusing on predictors of externalizing symptoms and disorders. For example, Waldman (Reference Waldman2007) found that children without DRD2 risk (A1 allele) were no more likely to receive an attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis when mothers had never married or married more than once than were children whose mothers had married only once, but for children carrying the A1 allele (risk) the likelihood of ADHD increased significantly when mothers never married or married more than once. These familial factors presumably affect the caregiving environment, although they were not directly measured.
In contrast, recent research has concentrated on disentangling the specific effects of different types of parenting behaviors in conjunction with child genes. For example, Li and Lee (Reference Li and Lee2012) examined both negative and positive parenting behavior, which have been found to be distinct measures of parenting rather than opposite ends of a single continuum (Ellis & Nigg, Reference Ellis and Nigg2009; Pettit, Bates, & Dodge, Reference Pettit, Bates and Dodge1997) to predict ADHD. They found that the MAOA genotype interacted with negative parenting, but not positive parenting, to predict ADHD symptoms in 6- to 9-year-old boys. Boys homozygous for high activity MAOA demonstrated significantly higher parent- and teacher-rated symptoms of inattention as negative parenting behavior increased, relative to boys with the low-activity MAOA genotype. These results provide evidence for the specific influence of negative parenting on the development of ADHD, particularly for boys with high-activity MAOA. In similar findings, Propper, Willoughby, Halpern, Carbone, and Cox (Reference Propper, Willoughby, Halpern, Carbone and Cox2007) also examined the separate contribution of both sensitive and negative maternal behavior, and results confirmed that these two styles of parenting have differential effects on the development of externalizing behavior (as measured by maternal report) in 30-month-old children. High warm-responsive parenting was associated with decreased externalizing behavior for African American children possessing the short polymorphism of the DRD4 gene. No interactions were found with negative parenting. Another study found that harsh and intrusive maternal parenting, but not sensitive, interacted with the risk (methionine) allele of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene to predict oppositional defiant disorder and callous–unemotional behaviors in 3-year-olds (Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Propper, & Waschbusch, Reference Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Propper and Waschbusch2013). Moreover, harsh–intrusive behaviors in infancy predicted callous–unemotional behaviors, whereas oppositional defiant disorder behaviors were predicted by this behavior in infant and toddlerhood/preschool years. These studies emphasize the importance of carefully measuring caregiving behaviors and exploring distinct characteristics of parenting rather than one broad measure.
This body of work is continuously expanding, but several other investigations have found similar interactions between parenting and multiple candidate genes: the high activity MAOA genotype prospectively predicted conduct disorder, substance use disorder, and ADHD only in youth exposed to less positive parenting (Vanyukov et al., Reference Vanyukov, Maher, Devlin, Kirillova, Kirisci and Yu2007); the interaction of the L-DRD4 polymorphism and insensitive maternal caregiving during infancy predicted increased externalizing behaviors, including oppositional and aggressive behaviors, in preschool-age children (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2006); and negative parenting positively predicted conduct disorder in young children, but only among those with the 9/9 genotype of the dopamine active transporter 1 (DAT1) gene (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Lee, Chronis-Tuscano, Pelham and Waldman2011).
That genes initiate pathways to behavioral adaptation that may be altered by caregiving experiences is supported by a growing literature. Nevertheless, challenges remain in instantiating a model of the etiology of psychopathology that points to the biological processes that may be genetically influenced and that play a role in the emergence of patterns of behavior that constitute impaired mental health and functioning. In the next section, we review literature that addresses the underlying biological processes that are candidates for mediation in pathways to psychopathology.
Physiological Mediators of Parenting and Child Functioning
Clearly, it is not believed that parenting experiences and genes interact to directly alter children's development; rather, these coactions are mediated through numerous biological mechanisms that are implicated in behavioral adaptations. In human populations, much of this work has focused on regulatory functioning, with specific emphases on peripheral systems associated with arousal and regulation such as the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the autonomic nervous system (ANS). Animal models offer evidence for caregiver effects on the development and functioning of the central nervous system, and advances in human neuroscience research have provided opportunities to begin to examine associations between caregiving experiences and neuroanatomical structure and function in early development. Recent human research has begun to parallel an extant animal literature on the associations between early caregiving and the biobehavioral systems that support affiliation and bonding, including the roles of neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin in the development of early parent–child relationships. However, for our purposes we will focus on the functioning of the HPA axis and the ANS, given that these systems have received the most attention by human researchers with respect to their associations with both early caregiving environments and subsequent child adjustment and maladjustment.
Parenting behaviors and early biopsychosocial models of reactivity and regulation
Reactivity to environmental stimuli in young children has both genetic and developmental origins. For example, individual differences in early reactivity appear very early in infancy and are thought to be biologically based and have relative stability over time, although evidence suggests that they are modifiable by developmental processes and environmental experiences (Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Ahadi and Evans2000), particularly variations in caregiving experiences (Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Damon, Lerner and Eisenberg2006; see also Belsky, Fish, & Isabella, Reference Belsky, Fish and Isabella1991; Calkins & Fox, Reference Calkins and Fox1992; Engfer, Reference Engfer, Rubin and Asendorpf1993; Matheny, Reference Matheny1986). In contrast, the development of self-regulation is quite protracted, with foundations for adaptive self-regulation emerging in infancy and effortful and autonomous regulation emerging in later childhood (Beauregard, Levesque, & Paquette, Reference Beauregard, Levesque, Paquette and Beauregard2004; Calkins, Reference Calkins, Booth, McHale and Lansdale2011). During the early phases of development, however, there is almost an exclusive reliance on the caregiver by the child to provide emotional scaffolding and external sources of regulation and control needed to help the child modulate arousal. These processes, and their relevance for other normative and pathological child outcomes, have been evidenced by multiple studies of the precursors and sequelae of early psychophysiological reactivity and regulation. In integrating the findings on this topic, Boyce and Ellis (Reference Boyce and Ellis2005) proposed a theoretical framework suggesting that hyperreactivity in stress responsive systems may occur in response to highly stressful early environments, specifically those that are threatening, unpredictable, and lacking in caregiver support. When such conditions are persistent over time, the chronic and prolonged activation of these systems (sometimes referred to as allostatic load) results in an impeded ability of the child to flexibly respond to different levels and types of challenge (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998, Reference McEwen2000) that may have long-term consequences for both mental and physical health during later childhood and adulthood (Evans, Reference Evans2003, Reference Evans2004; Gordis, Granger, Susman, & Trickett, Reference Gordis, Granger, Susman and Trickett2008; McEwen & Seeman, Reference McEwen and Seeman1999). Here we will focus explicitly on parenting associations with the HPA axis and ANS, as well as the implications of variations in these systems for subsequent child development.
Parenting experiences and early HPA functioning
Research on the end product of HPA activity, the glucocorticoid hormone cortisol, suggests that atypical levels of resting cortisol or cortisol reactivity in response to threat or challenge is associated with deficits in effortful control (Lengua, Reference Lengua, Maholmes and King2012) and higher levels of externalizing and internalizing behavior problems (Granger, Stansbury, & Henker, Reference Granger, Stansbury and Henker1994; Granger, Weisz, McCracken, Ikeda, & Douglas, Reference Granger, Weisz, McCracken, Ikeda and Douglas1996). Whereas the ideal relation between cortisol response to moderate stress and child competence is often considered to be an inverted U shape with moderate levels of cortisol response associated with attention, effortful appraisal, and engagement with the environment (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Granger, Kivlighan, Mills-Koonce, Willoughby and Greenberg2008), very high or very low levels of cortisol reactivity have been associated with regulatory deficits (de Kloet & Oitzl, Reference de Kloet and Oitzl2003; de Kloet, Oitzl, & Joels, Reference de Kloet, Oitzl and Joels1999; Erickson, Drevets, & Schulkin, Reference Erickson, Drevets and Schulkin2003). These patterns of cortisol function appear to be consolidated early in development (although they remain highly plastic) and the effects of experience on the development of the HPA responses to stress are well established (Gunnar & Quevedo, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2008; Meaney & Szyf, Reference Meaney and Szyf2005). Animal models (as well as a growing literature based on human studies) clearly implicate early caregiving as a primary influence on the development of the HPA axis (Champagne et al., Reference Champagne, Bagot, van Hasselt, Ramakers, Meaney and de Kloet2008; Liu, Diorio, Day, Francis, & Meaney, Reference Liu, Diorio, Day, Francis and Meaney2000) through the provision of nurturing supportive environments that appropriately scaffold learning experiences to support well-regulated physiological responses to stress (Gunnar & Quevedo, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2007; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1996).
To date, several studies have documented correlations between early maternal (Albers, Riksen-Walraven, Sweep, & de Weerth, Reference Albers, Riksen-Walraven, Sweep and deWeerth2008; Blair et al., Reference Blair, Granger, Kivlighan, Mills-Koonce, Willoughby and Greenberg2008; Pendry & Adam, Reference Pendry and Adam2007) and paternal (Mills-Koonce et al., Reference Mills-Koonce, Garrett-Peters, Barnett, Granger, Blair and Cox2011) caregiving and HPA functioning in young children. Furthermore, Blair et al. (Reference Blair, Granger, Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Cox and Greenberg2011) provides evidence for the role of parenting behavior as a mediator of the potentially adverse effects of poverty on early HPA dysfunction based on data collected by the Family Life Project, a large epidemiological study of children and families living in poor, rural communities in the United States. In this recent research, sensitive parenting (defined as warm, engaged, and emotionally supportive caregiving) mediated the association between income-to-needs ratios and baseline measures of child cortisol at 7, 15, and 24 months of age. Perhaps the most convincing evidence, however, comes from experimental manipulation of parenting behaviors through a randomized clinical trial that resulted in both improved parenting and parent–child relationships as well as more optimal HPA functioning in young children (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman, & Juffer, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman and Juffer2008).
In addition to the potential direct effects of early parenting on children's HPA functioning, there is also evidence that the quality of the early parent–child attachment relationship is also associated with early HPA functioning. The attachment behavioral system includes those infant behaviors that are activated by stress with the goal of reinstating a sense of security, usually best achieved in infancy by close physical contact or proximity with a familiar caregiver (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1969). Under typical conditions, a well-functioning attachment relationship will serve to buffer the infant (and adult) against extreme levels of fearful arousal. When the infant's caregiving history precludes the ability to use the parent as a source of security and external regulation (as is often the case among children with insecure or disorganized attachments; Main, Reference Main1990), then the child's cortisol response to stress or challenge is likely to diverge from typical patterns of age- and experience-appropriate reactivity and regulation. To this effect, Spangler and Grossmann (Reference Spangler and Grossmann1993) reported that children with insecure–disorganized attachment relationships maintained significantly elevated cortisol levels 30 min after experiencing an emotional challenge as compared to infants with secure strategies; whereas, cortisol levels of infants with insecure–avoidant attachments were intermediate in value. Similar findings have been reported by Hertsgaard, Gunnar, Erickson, and Nachmias (Reference Hertsgaard, Gunnar, Erickson and Nachmias1995) and Bernard and Dozier (Reference Bernard and Dozier2010) and suggest that in infancy dysregulated cortisol activity can occur when adequate child-behavioral strategies cannot be applied, such as the inability to effectively utilize the parent as a source of security and external regulation.
On the extreme end of the parenting spectrum, research identifies early and prolonged experience of abuse, maltreatment, and neglect as highly significant risk factors for atypical behavioral and psychobiological outcomes (Hussey et al., Reference Hussey, Marshall, English, Knight, Lau and Dubowitz2005; Kohl, Jonson-Reid, & Drake, Reference Kohl, Jonson-Reid and Drake2009). Although there are exceptions (e.g., King, Mandansky, King, Fletcher, & Brewer, Reference King, Mandansky, King, Fletcher and Brewer2001), several studies have shown that children and adolescents exposed to severe child abuse and neglect or other types of severe trauma have higher levels of baseline cortisol than do comparison children (Carrion, Weems, Ray, & Reiss, Reference Carrion, Weems, Ray and Reiss2002; Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch2001; De Bellis et al., Reference De Bellis, Keshavan, Clark, Casey, Giedd and Boring1999; Delahanty, Nugent, Christopher, & Walsh, Reference Delahanty, Nugent, Christopher and Walsh2005; Pfeffer, Altemus, Heo, & Jiang, Reference Pfeffer, Altemus, Heo and Jiang2007) and hyperactivity of the HPA axis and ANS (Heim, Newport, Mletzko, Miller, & Nemeroff, Reference Heim, Newport, Mletzko, Miller and Nemeroff2008).
Although the association between caregiver treatment and child HPA axis hyperreactivity is consistent with Boyce and Ellis' (Reference Boyce and Ellis2005) theoretical framework linking highly stressful early environments to physiological hyperreactivity, emerging theory and research also posits an alternative “attenuation hypothesis” (Gunnar & Vazquez, Reference Gunnar and Vazquez2001; Susman, Reference Susman2006) whereby the HPA axis adapts to sustained periods of hyperactivity in response to chronic and extreme environments (such as abuse and maltreatment) by downregulating cortisol secretion following a stressor, resulting in hyposecretion. Although several mechanisms have been proposed, such as decreased biosynthesis of hormones in the HPA axis, downregulation of pituitary receptors, and increased negative feedback sensitivity (Fries, Hesse, Hellhammer, & Hellhammer, Reference Fries, Hesse, Hellhammer and Hellhammer2005; Heim et al., Reference Heim, Newport, Heit, Graham, Wilcox and Bonsall2000), hyposecretion is proposed to have an adaptive function, as prolonged exposure to cortisol has deleterious effects on brain structures, such as the hippocampus and frontal cortex, as well as cardiovascular and immunological functioning (Bremner & Vermetten, Reference Bremner and Vermetten2001; De Bellis & Kuchibhatla, Reference De Bellis and Kuchibhatla2006; McEwen & Wingfield, Reference McEwen and Wingfield2003; Raison & Miller, Reference Raison and Miller2003; Sapolsky, Romero, & Munck, Reference Sapolsky, Romero and Munck2000). Furthermore, studies of adults retrospectively reporting childhood abuse have reported lower levels of cortisol during the course of the day (Bremner et al., Reference Bremner, Vythilingam, Vermetten, Southwick, McGlashan and Nazeer2003) and in response to hormonal or physical challenge (Heim, Newport, Bonsall, Miller, & Nemeroff, Reference Heim, Newport, Bonsall, Miller and Nemeroff2001; Santa Ana et al., Reference Santa Ana, Saladin, Back, Waldrop, Spratt and McRae2006). These associations, however, do appear malleable to proper intervention following reports of abuse. For example, a recent study reported that family-based preventative intervention for children at high risk for abuse can alter HPA axis stress responses (Brotman, Goldern, & Wittstein, Reference Brotman, Goldern and Wittstein2007) and can alter the daily rhythms of cortisol (Fisher, Stoolmiller, Gunnar, & Burraston, Reference Fisher, Stoolmiller, Gunnar and Burraston2007), potentially reducing the risk for later regulatory and psychopathological disorders.
Parenting behavior and early autonomic functioning
The human nervous system is divided into the central and peripheral nervous systems, and the peripheral nervous system can itself be divided by volition into somatic and autonomic components. The ANS can in turn be further divided into two branches: the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) and the sympathetic nervous system (SNS). The activity of the PNS is most often indexed by vagal tone, or the neural control of the heart via the vagus nerve. Effective vagal functioning has been related to the ability to maintain homeostasis in the face of situational change by allowing attention to shift from internal processes to external demands, facilitating the use of coping strategies to regulate affective or behavioral arousal (Porges, Reference Porges, Campbell, Hayne and Richardson1992). Overall, findings support the hypothesis that higher levels of baseline vagal tone and greater vagal withdrawal during a challenge context reflect more effective regulation (Bazhenova, Plonskaia, & Porges, Reference Bazhenova, Plonskaia and Porges2001; Moore & Calkins, Reference Moore and Calkins2004), including higher soothability (Huffman et al., Reference Huffman, Bryan, del Carmen, Pederson, Doussard-Roosevelt and Porges1998), more attentional control (Huffman et al., Reference Huffman, Bryan, del Carmen, Pederson, Doussard-Roosevelt and Porges1998; Suess, Porges, & Plude, Reference Suess, Porges and Plude1994), and better emotion regulation (Calkins, Reference Calkins1997; Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, Portales, & Greenspan, Reference Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, Portales and Greenspan1996). In contrast to the PNS, the SNS is associated with the “fight or flight” response to stress (Cannon, Reference Cannon1929) with sympathetic activation freeing and directing metabolic resources to support active defense behaviors. Early attempts to measure SNS activity focused largely on changes in heart rate, although more recently, Nater and Rohleder (Reference Nater and Rohleder2009) have advocated assaying saliva for the enzyme α-amylase (sAA), as well as measuring the heart's preejection period (Sherwood, Reference Sherwood, Blascovich and Katkin1995), as an index of sympathetic activity that strongly correlates with other measures of sympathetic activation (Bauer, Quas, & Boyce, Reference Bauer, Quas and Boyce2002).
As with the HPA axis, the quality of the early caregiving environment likely influences the trajectories of autonomic development from infancy onward (Calkins, Smith, Gill, & Johnson, Reference Calkins, Smith, Gill and Johnson1998; Calkins & Fox, Reference Calkins and Fox1992; Fox, Reference Fox1989), as regulatory strategies shift from caregiver–child coregulation to internalized and effortful regulation by the infant. Moore and Calkins (Reference Moore and Calkins2004) found that 3-month-old infants of dyads exhibiting lower synchrony (i.e., the degree to which parents and infants synchronously changed affective displays) showed an atypical response to a social challenge task, exhibiting greater levels of vagal withdrawal during an episode in which they played normally with their mothers and less vagal withdrawal during a situation meant to elicit distress. Working with 6-month-old infants, Porter (Reference Porter2003) demonstrated that children who spent more time in joint communicative sequences with their mothers had higher basal vagal tones than did children in dyads that spent less time engaged in this form of dyadic regulation. From 12 to 18 months, children classified as insecurely attached to their primary caregiver (specifically children with insecure–avoidant attachments) displayed higher heart rate, increased vagal withdrawal, and higher levels of sAA (a marker of SNS activity) throughout the entire Strange Situation Paradigm than did infants classified as securely attached (Frigerio et al., Reference Frigerio, Ceppi, Rusconi, Giorda, Raggi and Fearon2009; Hill-Soderlund et al., Reference Hill-Soderlund, Mills-Koonce, Propper, Calkins, Granger and Moore2008). Among toddlers in foster care, Oosterman, De Schipper, Fisher, Dozier, and Schuengel (Reference Oosterman, De Schipper, Fisher, Dozier and Schuengel2010) demonstrated that children in disordered attachment relationships (an aggregate category including children with disorganized–controlling and insecure–other classifications) exhibited increased sympathetic activity (as indexed by preejection period) over the course of a modified Strange Situation Paradigm.
The findings of various studies have consistently demonstrated that maternal sensitivity is associated with effective autonomic regulation in response to challenge and, conversely, that negative, controlling behaviors are related to poor ANS regulation (e.g., Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Smith, Gill and Johnson1998; Calkins, Graziano, Berdan, Keane, & Degnan, Reference Calkins, Graziano, Berdan, Keane and Degnan2008; Conradt & Ablow, Reference Conradt and Ablow2010; Ham & Tronick, Reference Ham and Tronick2006; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Hill-Soderlund, Propper, Calkins, Mills-Koonce and Cox2009; Propper & Moore, Reference Propper and Moore2006). As previously discussed, Propper et al. (Reference Propper, Moore, Mills-Koonce, Halpern, Hill and Calkins2008) found that the effects of maternal sensitivity on autonomic regulation can even overcome genetic risk factors, with infants carrying the “risk” allele of the DRD2 gene initially failing to show expected and normative vagal withdrawal during challenge but later demonstrating normative vagal functioning 6 months later when raised by sensitive and supportive mothers. At the opposite end of the maternal behavior spectrum, Kivlighan and Granger (Reference Kivlighan and Granger2006) reported that maternal detachment was associated with higher levels of baseline sAA during infancy, suggesting less flexibility in the ANS to respond adequately to challenge.
Just as with the HPA axis, the findings that exposure to family risk and nonoptimal parenting is associated with alterations of autonomic development are consistent with a biological embedding perspective (Hertzman & Boyce, Reference Hertzman and Boyce2010). In the short term, maintaining a low level of parasympathetic reactivity, possibly by establishing lower basal levels of vagal tone, may be adaptive. According to Boyce and Ellis (Reference Boyce and Ellis2005), high levels of parasympathetic reactivity would be ill suited to an environment in which challenges were frequent and severe. However, what is adaptive in the short term may not remain so. A young child born into an environment rife with sources of proximal risk is likely to continue to be exposed to those risks. If the resources afforded by the PNS are unavailable, then those offered by the SNS will be drawn upon, perhaps more heavily than in a child born to a less stressful environment (as the small number of studies above that assessed sympathetic response to challenge would suggest). Again, as with the HPA system, conventional theories of reactivity (e.g., Nesse & Young, Reference Nesse, Young and Fink2000) assert that such frequent, large, and prolonged sympathetic activation would impose an allostatic load with deleterious effects on the long-term physical and mental health of the child.
Implications for developmental psychopathology
Across multiple longitudinal studies there is accumulating evidence that moderate levels of psychophysiological reactivity to common stressors are associated with more adaptive short- and long-term outcomes and that both patterns of hyperresponsivity and hyporesponsivity in response to early adversity are associated with less adaptive and more problematic developmental outcomes (Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović and Boyce2009). For example, internalizing behaviors have been associated with higher levels of SNS reactivity (Fowles, Kochanska, & Murray, Reference Fowles, Kochanska and Murray2000; Weems, Zakem, Costa, Cannon, & Watts, Reference Weems, Zakem, Costa, Cannon and Watts2005) and higher levels of HPA reactivity (Adam & Kumari, Reference Adam and Kumari2009; Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long, & Grigs, Reference Gunnar, Wewerka, Frenn, Long and Griggs2009; Halligan, Herbert, Goodyer, & Murray, Reference Halligan, Herbert, Goodyer and Murray2007). Meanwhile, externalizing behaviors have been associated with lower levels of PNS reactivity (Calkins & Dedmon, Reference Calkins and Dedmon2000; Calkins, Graziano, & Keane, Reference Calkins, Graziano and Keane2007; Calkins & Keane, Reference Calkins and Keane2004), SNS reactivity (Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, Reference Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp and Mead2007; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Sylvers, Mead and Chipman-Chacon2006; Gatzke-Kopp, Raine, Loeber, Stouthamer-Loeber, & Steinhauer, Reference Gatzke-Kopp, Raine, Loeber, Stouthamer-Loeber and Steinhauer2002) and HPA reactivity (O'Neal et al., Reference O'Neal, Brotman, Huang, Gouley, Kamboukos and Calzada2010, Oosterlaan, Geurts, Knol, & Sergeant, Reference Oosterlaan, Geurts, Knol and Sergeant2005; Shirtcliff, Granger, Booth, & Johnson, Reference Shirtcliff, Granger, Booth and Johnson2005). These patterns of reactivity have implications for how children appraise and respond to their environments as well as how their environments continue to influence their ongoing development. For example, it is possible that early insensitive and punitive caregiving environments may result in hyporesponsive stress systems over time (Obradović, Reference Obradović2012); compromise the emotional experiences of fear, guilt, and empathy; increase children's insensitivity to punishment; and ultimately increase their propensities for aggressive and antisocial behaviors (Dadds & Salmon, Reference Dadds and Salmon2003). Such a model suggests that early family experiences shape how children react physiologically, cognitively, and behaviorally to salient emotional information about themselves and others and that this multilevel cascade may have profound implications for developmental psychopathology well beyond early childhood (Cox, Mills-Koonce, Propper, & Gariepy, Reference Cox, Mills-Koonce, Propper and Gariepy2010).
In addition, the effects of early parenting behaviors on the development of child psychophysiological reactivity and regulation may alter the child's sensitivity to future environmental experiences (including ongoing experiences with the caregivers). For example, children who are biologically more reactive have been identified as being more susceptible to the negative effects of environmental adversity as well as the positive effects of environmental support as compared to less-reactive children (Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011). Similar to Belsky's differential susceptibility hypotheses, which posits that temperamentally more-reactive children are more susceptible to both positive and negative environmental conditions (Belsky, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005; Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007), Boyce & Ellis (Reference Boyce and Ellis2005) propose that physiological reactivity can also be conceptualized as a biological indicator of sensitivity to context. Recent research on individual differences in both ANS and HPA axis functioning provides evidence that heightened physiological reactivity is associated with stronger correlations between environmental experiences and children's behavioral development (Boyce, Torshein, Currie, & Zambon, Reference Boyce, Torsheim, Currie and Zambon2006; Cummings, El-Sheikh, Kouros, & Keller, Reference Cummings, El-Sheikh, Kouros and Keller2007; Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler, & Boyce, Reference Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler and Boyce2010). The developmental implications of this conceptualization of physiological reactivity are quite profound. Given that the possible implications for atypical physiological reactivity are not mutually exclusive, we can view the ANS and HPA systems as providing both (a) a biological means of transmitting early family experience into a developmental endophenotype underlying typical or atypical developmental outcomes and (b) a biological mechanism that constrains or augments the influence of ongoing environmental experiences on the developing child. In other words, psychophysiological reactivity may serve as both a critical mediator and moderator of early experience on later developmental psychopathology.
Challenges and New Directions
The genetic and psychophysiological work reviewed here makes clear that direct-effects analyses of the role of either genes/biology or environment in emerging psychopathology are neither accurate nor very revealing of developmental process, largely because they do not reflect the complexity of the developmental system. Instead, acknowledging that family and child factors are embedded in a dynamic biopsychosocial context is key to understanding developmental trajectories of child psychopathology. Adoption of a biopsychosocial framework that emphasizes the dynamic and transactional nature of the developmental system is key to generating empirical work that may contribute substantially to our understanding of development across multiple levels of functioning and across the life span. We would argue that the integration of G × E studies with those that focus on underlying physiological processes that may be affected by caregiving and that support adaptive behavior may be the most fruitful approach to articulating the pathways to psychopathology in its various forms. Such investigations are still relatively rare and beset by numerous challenges, which are evident at the level of conceptualization, investigation, and translation. Here we articulate these challenges and suggest new directions for research that may push the field of developmental psychopathology to greater understanding of the role of biology in the multiple pathways to child adjustment.
Identifying the gene(s), defining the environment, and refining the phenotype
One important reason that a biopsychosocial perspective is a valuable tool in the empirical study of child psychopathology is that such inquiries force scientists to better articulate the elements of the multiple developmental pathways that exist. Much of the research on G × E interactions focuses on broad indicators of both inherited traits and child outcomes and somewhat global indicators of family process. However, in tandem with specifying the mechanisms of genetic transmissions, identifying and defining the behavioral, and observable, elements of a developmental trajectory is critical. This articulation not only is required of the outcome of interest, such as “child adjustment,” but also must be done at every level of the analysis.
For example, an important new and critical direction for G × E studies is the need to move beyond the focus on one genetic allele in prediction of complex outcomes and instead to consider multiple genes, as well as their interaction (Gene × Gene). The failure to do so may account for some of the mixed results regarding the effects of one specific gene on behavior; examining multiple polymorphisms in combination as haplotypes may ameliorate this issue. Haplotypes refer to a group of alleles on a single chromosome that are usually inherited together. It is likely that these polymorphisms interact, and it is necessary to be aware of these allelic combinations when making inferences about functional and phenotypic associations. For example, Sulik et al. (Reference Sulik, Eisenberg, Lemery-Chalfant, Spinrad, Silva and Eggum2012) found that certain outcomes are revealed only when specific allelic combinations as haplotypes are examined. In this work, the interaction between three solute carrier family C6, member 4 gene haplotypes and parenting behavior was a significant predictor of noncompliance from 18 to 54 months age, such that at younger ages one group of children (L10–L12 haplotypes) were more susceptible to parenting than were the others (S10 and S12 haplotypes). However, over time the S10 haplotype group became increasingly affected by parenting, while the S12 group remained relatively unaffected by parenting across the entire study. Not only do these findings highlight the importance of taking into account haplotypes rather than individual genes, but the work's longitudinal design also provides the chance to examine change over time. Many of these findings would not have been clear if they were assessed cross-sectionally, as the effect of parenting for each group was most salient at various points over early development.
Another challenge to specificity in developmental pathways is the articulation and measurement of environment. For example, although the environment in G × E may be obvious to some researchers, if one wishes to trace developmental pathways from genes to behavioral outcomes with any degree of emphasis on the actual process, whether biological or behavioral, better articulation of the relevant features of environment are necessary. Often, researchers assume that environment must mean parenting behavior or other dimensions of the family environment. Such an assumption may be intuitively appealing, and in some cases correct, but it should not preclude looking at other elements of the environment. This focus on family functioning and parenting behavior requires elaboration. Although considerable research has sought to understand the relations between parental behavior and a range of child developmental outcomes, much of this work has been conducted at a very broad level of analysis, using global constructs like attachment, warmth, and monitoring. Analyses of G × E interactions reveal the need for models of family functioning that offer greater specificity regarding proximal processes that may be implicated in the effects of these relationships on children's biological and behavioral functioning (Corter & Fleming, Reference Corter, Fleming and Bornstein1995).
Phenotypic specificity at the level of outcome is needed as well. Much of the work examining child adjustment has focused on broad indicators of the child's functioning, such as externalizing and internalizing symptoms. Predicting to such indicators is less than satisfying because they are not very revealing of the specific ways in which children's behavior or biology is being influenced or altered by the G × E interplay. Moreover, many of the measures of child outcome that are used would best be described as general indicators of child functioning, given that individual scales reflecting particular clusters of symptoms are often correlated (e.g., internalizing and externalizing) and given that parents are typically the reporters of the child's functioning. To the extent that investigations can specify and observe particular behaviors or traits in a more nuanced way, identifying linkages to neural or physiological processes that are genetically influenced will be made easier, as will hypothesizing how environmental factors might alter the pathway to the behavioral phenotype.
Situating parenting behaviors and biopsychosocial development in context and culture
When examining the role of parenting behaviors in the development of early biopsychosocial processes that may increase or decrease the likelihood of later psychopathology in children, it is necessary to consider the contexts and cultures surrounding these family dynamics and developmental processes. Unlike the highly controlled laboratory conditions present for most animal research models, human development does not occur in such a vacuum, and as a result, we must be sensitive to ecological and sociocultural influences on both the parent and child. For example, despite multiple observational studies reporting higher levels of controlling maternal behaviors among African American parents as compared to European American parents (Ispa et al., Reference Ispa, Fine, Halgunseth, Harper, Robinson and Boyce2004; Mills-Koonce et al., Reference Mills-Koonce, Garrett-Peters, Barnett, Granger, Blair and Cox2011), the positive associations between controlling parenting behaviors and maladaptive behavioral outcomes tend to be smaller among African American families as compared to European American families (Ispa et al., Reference Ispa, Fine, Halgunseth, Harper, Robinson and Boyce2004). Some social scientists have interpreted these findings as reflective of cultural differences in how parents view the roles of parenting in children's development. In this example, it is possible that African American parents endorse greater authoritarian parenting beliefs and practices (characterized by higher levels of parental control) as a best practice for child rearing due to a more non-Western, collectivistic cultural heritage as compared to European American parents (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, Reference Deater-Deckard and Dodge1997; Ispa et al., Reference Ispa, Fine, Halgunseth, Harper, Robinson and Boyce2004). There may also be a decoupling of parental control and parental warmth among African American parents (in which the negative correlation between control and warmth is weaker) as compared to European American parents, which may allow for a greater probability of parental warmth to attenuate negative associations between parental control and child outcomes in African American families (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, Reference Deater-Deckard and Dodge1997).
However, note that these variations in family processes and child outcomes as a function of culture might also be constrained by context or by developmental timing. For example, most of the weak associations between parental control and negative child outcomes for African American families are based on samples of preadolescent and older children. Within the infancy and toddler periods, Clincy, Mills-Koonce, and the Family Life Project Key Investigators (in press) report that increasing levels of intrusiveness and control among African American mothers during the first three years of life are associated with lower levels of language ability, cognitive development, and effortful control and higher levels of peer problems in toddlerhood. Blair et al. (Reference Blair, Granger, Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Cox and Greenberg2011) found higher levels of resting cortisol among African American children at 6, 15, and 24 months of age, and this group difference was mediated by observed parenting behaviors. The implications of these findings for subsequent variation in child maladaptation are as of yet unknown. Whereas the basic processes linking parenting behaviors and early biopsychosocial development may be more etic and common across cultures, how these associations ultimately coalesce into child adaptation or maladaptation over time may be more relativistic depending on (a) how the child interprets and internalizes those family processes over time and (b) how developmental variation in biopsychosocial functioning translates to context-specific functioning for that individual.
Using longitudinal models of biopsychosocial development to understand consolidation and maintenance
It is likely that characteristics of children that are labeled as “stable” or “unstable” at any given point in time can also, and perhaps more appropriately, be described as under developmental consolidation or maintenance over time (Cairns & Rodkin, Reference Cairns, Rodkin, Cairns, Bergman and Kagan1998). Often, constructs measured in older children that are particularly stable over time are considered trait or traitlike and possibly genetically mediated and resistant to developmental change or modification through intervention. However, examining these phenomena solely during later periods of development may obscure the effects of early developmental processes that contribute to the consolidation of such stable phenotypes over time and, in so doing, both limit our abilities to (a) observe developmentally sensitive critical periods for early intervention to offset later maladaptation and (b) better identify environmental factors contributing to the maintenance of child maladaptation over time.
For example, punishment insensitivity is a characteristic often reported for children with particularly high levels of conduct problems, callous–unemotional traits, and early antisocial behaviors (Dadds & Salmon, Reference Dadds and Salmon2003). For children who are less sensitive to punishment, traditional methods of behavior modification to limit aggressive behaviors have proven to have minimal effects and real-world experiences of punishment (such as being hurt during a fight) do not alter patterns of future behavior. Whereas some researchers would characterize punishment insensitivity as a genetically mediated endophenotype that underlies aggressive and antisocial behavior in children with high levels of conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits, Dadds suggests that this characteristic of older children has developmental roots in inconsistent and coercive parenting practices experienced at younger ages, which may alter the neurologically mediated reward-processing and fear-processing systems in children. Recent research has identified parenting experiences in the first year of life as particularly relevant for children genetically susceptible to early-onset conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits (Willoughby et al., Reference Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Propper and Waschbusch2013). These findings collectively suggest that biopsychosocial processes associated with later pathology may not only begin to consolidate in infancy but may also have highly sensitive, if not critical, developmental periods during the first years of life in some cases. These findings highlight the importance of a biopsychosocial model of development for identifying the predictors, mechanisms, and timing of processes related to the consolidation and maintenance endophenotypic characteristics of children across development and their roles in developmental psychopathology.
Developing genetically informed prevention/intervention
A clearer understanding of the relations between genes and environments, and identification of the specific child genotypes and caregiving behaviors most important for developing behaviors and pathology, may lead to important improvements in psychosocial interventions and preventions aimed at enriching the child's environment by shaping parenting behavior. Identification of the biological or physiological profiles that may be most receptive to specific behavioral therapies and treatments will allow for more specificity and focused efforts. There are a number of correlational findings in the literature (many of which are described above) that implicate specific candidate genes or moderating pathways that could be relevant for this work. Examining these associations within an experimental framework may allow for a more precise understanding of the effects of interventions that perhaps appeared to be unsuccessful in the past.
Several studies have begun to incorporate molecular genetics into psychosocial intervention/prevention work and provide evidence that this movement away from correlational studies and toward experimental designs may be an exciting and critical next direction in the field. For example, in the first study of its kind, Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. (Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman and Juffer2008) found that a video feedback intervention promoting positive parenting and sensitive discipline was most effective at decreasing externalizing behavior (at 1-year follow-up) in 1- to 3-year-old children who possessed the risk allele of DRD4 (L-DRD4) and that the effects were largest for those whose parents showed the most significant increase in use of positive discipline. A recent pilot study (van den Hoofdakker et al., Reference van den Hoofdakker, Nauta, Dijck-Brouwer, van der Veen-Mulders, Sytema and Emmelkamp2012) used this approach to investigate a behavioral parent training aimed to improve ADHD outcomes (which is crucial due to the high variability found in success of such trainings). Findings revealed that children's DAT1 genotype moderated their response to behavioral parent training such that children with two DAT1 10-repeat alleles did not benefit from behavioral parent training beyond their routine clinical care; however, those children with zero or one DAT1 10-repeat allele exhibited improvements from behavioral parent training above and beyond their routine clinical care alone.
The implications of this work are far reaching. It may be that specific genes found to be stronger in the presence of environmental risk may guide research on their expression, perhaps leading to genetic diagnoses and improved pharmacological interventions (Evans & Relling, Reference Evans and Relling1999). Alternatively, if specific environmental effects are found to be stronger in the presence of genetic risk, it could lead to new directions for environmental prevention efforts and help to identify which children will benefit from specific prevention programs and treatments the most.
Conclusions
Most recent models of genetic influences on both parenting and child behavioral outcomes have discarded any notion that such influences are ever independent of the environment (Meaney, Reference Meaney2010). Behavioral researchers are also beginning to appreciate the converse: Any examination of the family's influence on child functioning must, of necessity, acknowledge that such effects occur in the context of a host of biological processes, whether measured or not (cf. Booth, McHale, & Lansdale, Reference Booth, McHale and Lansdale2011). In this paper, we have argued that adopting a biopsychosocial approach will enhance an understanding of the pathways between parenting and child psychopathology. We review research that addresses the way in which genes and parenting interact to produce differential outcomes, and we address the underlying physiological processes of child behavior that may be altered by different patterns of parenting behavior. We argue in favor of a more integrated perspective of parenting effects that acknowledges the genetic component to complex behavior but note that we must address the critical biological mediators that are at the heart of developmental process in G × E research. Finally, we highlight areas of challenge and opportunity for this kind of research that will yield greater specificity and offer points of entry for intervention and prevention.