Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T20:36:25.989Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond risk, resilience, and dysregulation: Phenotypic plasticity and human development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2013

Jay Belsky*
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis King Abdulaziz University Birkbeck University of London
Michael Pluess
Affiliation:
Queen Mary University of London
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Jay Belsky, Human and Community Development, University of California, Davis, One Shields Avenue, Hart Hall, Davis, CA 95616; E-mail: jbelsky@ucdavis.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We provide a theoretical and empirical basis for the claim that individual differences exist in developmental plasticity and that phenotypic plasticity should be a subject of study in its own right. To advance this argument, we begin by highlighting challenges that evolutionary thinking poses for a science of development and psychopathology, including for the diathesis–stress framework that has (fruitfully) guided so much empirical inquiry on developmental risk, resilience, and dysregulation. With this foundation laid, we raise a series of issues that the differential-susceptibility hypothesis calls attention to, while highlighting findings that have emerged over just the past several years and are pertinent to some of the questions posed. Even though it is clear that this new perspective on Person × Environment interaction is stimulating research and influencing how hypotheses are framed and data interpreted, a great many topics remain that need empirical attention. Our intention is to encourage students of development and psychopathology to treat phenotypic plasticity as an individual-difference construct while exploring unknowns in the differential-susceptibility equation.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Empirical research on risk and resilience first emerged in the 1970s (e.g., Garmezy, Reference Garmezy, Anthony and Koupernik1974; Rutter, Reference Rutter, Kent and Rolf1979; Werner & Smith, Reference Werner and Smith1977) and has come to exert a major influence on the study of development and psychopathology (for reviews, see Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, Reference Luthar, Cicchetti and Becker2000; Masten, Reference Masten2007; Rutter, Reference Rutter2012). One of the most important and valuable insights that emerged over the past decades has been that individuals differ in the degree to which they are affected by contextual adversity. Whereas some “vulnerable” children and adults are likely to develop problematically in response to negative contextual conditions by becoming emotionally and/or behaviorally dysregulated, others are not and are thus regarded as “resilient.”

Empirically observed variability in vulnerability to contextual adversity accords nicely with the widely embraced, if not always labeled, diathesis–stress framework (Gottesman & Shields, Reference Gottesman and Shields1967; Monroe & Simons, Reference Monroe and Simons1991; Zuckerman, Reference Zuckerman1999). This perspective stipulates that some individuals are disproportionately, if not exclusively, likely to succumb to the negative effects of some contextual stressors (e.g., childhood maltreatment, insensitive parenting, poverty) and become psychologically and/or behaviorally disturbed and dysregulated, if not disordered. This heightened vulnerability to adversity may be due to organismic characteristics of the individual, such as genetic makeup, personality, or temperament, or even family factors and processes or extrafamilial conditions. Perhaps nowhere else has diathesis–stress thinking explicitly or implicitly guided recent empirical inquiry more than in the study of Gene × Environment (G × E) interaction (e.g., Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin and Craig2002).

One of the challenging issues that arises with respect to diathesis–stress motivated G × E research has to do with understanding why natural selection would craft an organism to respond to adversity by becoming disordered or dysregulated. That is, what could be the pay-off of developmental dysfunction? As we clarify in the next section, the answer to this question emerges upon challenging another prevailing notion that is commonly encountered in the literature on development and psychopathology, that of “optimal development.” Thus, the first major section of this paper raises some evolutionary-inspired challenges to diathesis–stress thinking and, more generally, the science of development and psychopathology. Although evolutionary theory has made inroads recently into these arenas of inquiry (see the 2011 Special Section of Development and Psychopathology edited by Ellis and Boyce and 2012 Developmental Psychology edited by Ellis and Bjorklund), it would still be fair to say that developmentally oriented scholars have been among the slowest to embrace an evolutionary perspective. We seek to change that and join the limited company of others, some of whom have encouraged such an orientation relatively unsuccessfully for quite some time (Belsky, Steinberg, & Draper, Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991; Bjorklund & Pellegrini, Reference Bjorklund and Pellegrini2002; Daly & Wilson, Reference Daly and Wilson1980, Reference Daly and Wilson1981).

The evolutionary-inspired challenges to prevailing thinking about development and psychopathology that we highlight in the next section, including the diathesis–stress framework, lead to the conclusion that developmental plasticity should be regarded as a phenotype in its own right, that is, as an individual-difference construct, something students of animal behavior are becoming ever more appreciative of (Dingemanse, Kazem, Reale, & Wright, Reference Dingemanse, Kazem, Reale and Wright2010; Mathot et al., Reference Mathot, van den Hout, Piersma, Kempenaers, Reale and Dingemanse2011). This claim calls attention to a new and evolutionary-inspired way of thinking about Person × Environment interaction: the differential-susceptibility framework (Belsky, Reference Belsky1997, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005; Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a; Boyce & Ellis, Reference Boyce and Ellis2005; Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011). In an attempt to further inquiry in this area, we highlight future directions for research and theory by highlighting some new empirical findings in the second major section of this paper. Then, we draw some general conclusions in the final section.

Evolutionary Challenges to a Science of Development and Psychopathology

Optimal versus adaptive development

Much theory, research, and writing about development and psychopathology today is based on what has been described recently as the “mental health model” (Belsky, Reference Belsky2008) or “developmental psychopathology model” (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Del Giudice, Dishion, Figueredo, Gray and Griskevicius2012; Frankenhuis & Del Giudice, Reference Frankenhuis and Del Giudice2012). Central to this theoretical and empirical framework is the implicit if not explicit assumption that there is something ubiquitously referred to in the developmental literature as “optimal” development. The notion of optimal development more or less implies that it is the natural condition of humans from youth to later in life to become secure, autonomous, self-controlling, prosocial, achievement striving, intimate in the context of pair bonds, hardworking, sensitively responsive in parenting, and generative in the next generation, as well as physically healthy, happy, and long lived. Development goes awry (i.e., becomes dysregulated) when forces direct it from its natural or otherwise anticipated course. Thus, being insecure, dependent, impulsive, antisocial, a risk taker, an insensitive parent, and/or depressed are implicitly if not explicitly regarded as developmental outcomes that nature did not intend.

However, an evolutionary perspective challenges this prevailing view of so-called dysfunctional, dysregulated, or maladaptive development, especially when, consistent with diathesis–stress thinking, it arises within settings of adversity (Belsky, Reference Belsky2008; Daly & Wilson, Reference Daly and Wilson2005; Ellis, Reference Ellis2004; Ellis, Boyce, et al., Reference Ellis and Boyce2011; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Del Giudice, Dishion, Figueredo, Gray and Griskevicius2012; Hinde & Stevenson-Hinde, Reference Hinde and Stevenson-Hinde1990). Because stressful and supportive environments have been part of human experience throughout our evolutionary history, evolutionists conceptualize developmental systems as shaped by natural selection to respond adaptively to both kinds of contexts. From this perspective, the experience of encountering stressful environments does not so much disturb, or dysregulate, development as direct, or regulate, it toward strategies that are or at least once were biologically adaptive under conditions of adversity. Moreover, by adaptive we mean functional and perhaps even optimal, given the circumstances in which the individual finds him/herself, in that they directly or indirectly promoted the individual's reproductive fitness. (See below for further discussion of this fundamental goal of all living things.)

Some of the most compelling evidence highlighting the validity of the adaptational perspective just outlined can be found in the much-heralded rodent work showing that what is regarded by many as “low-quality” maternal care in the rat, as reflected in limited licking and grooming of the newborn pup, modifies stress physiology and brain morphology, but in functional and strategic rather than maladaptive ways. Even though evidence of higher corticosterone levels, shorter dendritic branch lengths, and lower spine density in hippocampal neurons would appear to provide indisputable evidence of negative effects of early experience, such presumptively nonoptimal developments appear strikingly functional, given the conditions that gave rise to them. This is because the developmental experiences in question enhance learning and memory processes under stressful conditions (Champagne et al., Reference Champagne, Bagot, van Hasselt, Ramakers, Meaney and de Kloet2008). Moreover, such putatively disturbed or dysregulated physiological and morphological changes promote central features of defensive and reproductive strategies (behavior under threat, open-field exploration, pubertal development, sexual behavior, and parenting; Cameron et al., Reference Cameron, Champagne, Parent, Fish, Ozaki-Kuroda and Meaney2005) in ways consistent with evolutionary models of adaptive reproductive strategies (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991; Chisholm, Reference Chisholm1993), a subject we consider in greater detail below.

Ultimately, developmental adaptations to high-stress environments enable individuals to make the best of a bad situation (i.e., mitigate inevitable fitness costs), even though “the best” may still constitute a high-risk strategy that jeopardizes the person's health and survival (e.g., Mulvihill, Reference Mulvihill2005; Shonkoff, Boyce, & McEwen, Reference Shonkoff, Boyce and McEwen2009). It is certainly possible to view many of the developmental sequelae of child maltreatment in this manner. After all, it is not difficult to appreciate the developmental “wisdom” in being especially vigilant to threat, engaging in biased attributions or behaving aggressively if the lessons of a painful life have taught one to “act first and ask questions later” and “presume the worst rather than the best of people.” By the same token, the indiscriminant friendliness of institutionally reared children (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Steele, Zeanah, Muhamedrahimov, Vorria and Dobrova-Krol2011) could be regarded as a highly functional strategy for seeking help and support in a world that has offered little, even if it looks disturbed or disordered from the perspective of family-reared children.

Reflection on these observations should clarify why natural selection may have crafted human development to respond to certain conditions by making individuals behave in ways we now regard as disturbed, dysfunctional, dysregulated, psychopathological, and nonoptimal. As such, this analysis perhaps strengthens the theoretical foundations of diathesis–stress thinking that we questioned earlier. That is, one can now understand why nature may have crafted at least some individuals to respond to adversity by developing in putatively disturbed and dysregulated ways. Nevertheless, it remains our view that the potential costs of not responding to adversity in a putatively dysfunctional, dysregulated, and disordered manner merits more consideration by students of development and psychopathology than it has been given. Thus, in addition to considering the possible benefits of behavior regarded by many as dysregulated and problematic, students of development and psychopathology would be well advised to consider what the costs may be of behaving, given the circumstances some live in, in those ways that mainstream thinking labels as optimal.

The argument advanced here ultimately calls into question the use of the term optimal when discussing development and suggests that there could be substantial merit in thinking more in terms of why it may actually make good sense for children, adolescents, and adults to behave in the dysregulated and troubled ways in which they do. However, such a perspective does not mean abandoning efforts to reduce psychological pain and suffering. What it suggests is that to do so most effectively may require consideration of the payoffs and trade-offs that may have resulted in natural selection crafting an organism to develop and behave in the way we observe them to, especially under conditions of adversity. We suspect that intervention efficacy could be enhanced if interventionists seriously considered “biological gravity” (natural selection has crafted phenotypes that serve to enhance fitness) when designing programs rather than think only in traditional mental health terms about what behavior is optimal and what is not. Much like the efforts of would-be aviators who predated the modern ones who eventually came to grips with physical gravity, thereby recognizing the need for lift and propulsion before they could fly, some intervention efforts reflect little more than attaching wings to the arms and jumping off cliffs. Without understanding the nature, origin, and function of disordered and dysregulated behavior, including its evolutionary basis (i.e., biological gravity), the likelihood of preventing or remediating it may be seriously compromised. Ellis and associates (Reference Ellis and Bjorklund2012) develop this point further in the case of adolescent risk taking, highlighting existing interventions that appear consistent or inconsistent with evolutionary thinking as well as offering novel, evolutionary-inspired ones.

Probabilism versus determinism

It is widely appreciated by developmentalists today that factors that shape human development typically do so in a probabilistic rather than deterministic fashion. That is, forces that we expect to enhance or undermine human functioning, in the traditional mental health sense, will not always do so, even if they sometimes, or even often, do so. This probabilistic nature of development also needs to be appreciated in its evolutionary basis, something that seems forgotten when it is argued that disturbances in development could not reflect evolutionary adaptations because they so often lead to failure, including perhaps failure to disburse genes in future generations in the modern western world. Two points merit consideration in response to this seemingly reasonable observation.

The first is that natural selection is backward not forward looking, selecting phenotypes that have fostered fitness in the past. This means that what was once evolutionarily adaptive may no longer be so today. However, even if that is true, which should not necessarily be the default assumption, it would not “biologically” follow that the neurobiological and psychological processes that gave rise to them in the first place would have been short circuited in order to no longer engender the behavioral functioning that is typically regarded as problematic today. If that were the case, it would be easy to override the human proclivity to consume sugary foods. After all, this penchant for sweets evolved at a time when such substances were rarely or intermittently available, yet in the modern world today they are continuously available and abundant. Thus, even if a developmental process no longer achieves the goal it was selected to realize but instead engenders developmental problems in the modern world (e.g., obesity), the process itself likely remains operative because of the time it takes for evolution to engineer genetic change.

The second point regarding probabilism and nonoptimal development is that, for selection to take place, a phenotype does not have to always or often succeed in enhancing fitness; it only needs to have done so more often than alternative phenotypes once did (Frankenhuis & Del Giudice, Reference Frankenhuis and Del Giudice2012). Moreover, the costs of failure need to be weighed against the benefits of success (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Del Giudice, Dishion, Figueredo, Gray and Griskevicius2012; Figueredo & Jacobs, Reference Figueredo, Jacobs, Frias-Armenta and Corral-Verdugo2010; Frankenhuis & Del Giudice, Reference Frankenhuis and Del Giudice2012). Thus, even if antisocial behavior or depression or teenage pregnancy seem counterproductive from either a mental health or fitness standpoint and could have proven so in the past (failing more often than succeeding), such putative disturbances in development could still have been selected, especially when a low-frequency success engendered a substantial payoff. What this implies, of course, is that some putatively dysregulated behavior will be of the high-stakes’ variety: unlikely to succeed yet succeeding hugely when it does.

What is development trying to achieve?

The concept of optimal development is most certainly intended to capture phenomena like health, happiness, security, longevity, and other indisputably highly valued life conditions. On the basis of much developmental writing, it would not seem misguided to infer that nature has crafted human development to achieve such outcomes. From an evolutionary perspective, however, this is certainly not (exclusively) the case. Although security, good health, happiness, and a long life may be the means by which some or many individuals successfully pass genes on to future generations and promote their more general inclusive fitness, especially when growing up and living under benign or benevolent conditions, there is no reason to presume that these phenotypes achieved or can achieve these same ends under all conditions, especially contrasting conditions. This observation was central to Belsky's (in press; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991) effort to recast socialization theory in evolutionary perspective two decades ago, which was built on Draper and Harpending's (Reference Draper and Harpending1982) evolutionary insight about why father absence may be related to “promiscuous” sexual behavior on the part of females.

Rather than presuming that development was crafted by natural selection to yield phenotypes reflective of optimal development, Belsky et al.'s (Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991) psychosocial acceleration theory and its derivatives (Chisholm, Reference Chisholm1993; Del Giudice, Reference Del Giudice2009; Del Giudice, Angeleri, & Manera, Reference Del Giudice, Angeleri and Manera2009; Ellis, Reference Ellis2004; Ellis & Garber, Reference Ellis and Garber2000; Frankenhuis & Panchanathan, Reference Frankenhuis and Panchanathan2011; James, Ellis, Schlomer, & Garber, Reference James, Ellis, Schlomer and Garber2012) are founded on the premise that the ultimate goal of all living things, including humans, is to disperse their genes to future generations and that the means of achieving this fitness end is contextually contingent. Thus, what works under certain developmental conditions does not necessarily work under others. It is ultimately for this reason that the notion of optimal development is fundamentally problematic: what is optimal in one (modern and well resourced?) context may not be so in another.

We submit that students of development and psychopathology would do well to think in fitness terms when trying to understand phenotypes of concern. Upon doing so, what is regarded as dysregulated, dysfunctional, disturbed, or disordered may come to be seen in a new and informative light, a point recently made by Ellis and associates (Reference Ellis, Del Giudice, Dishion, Figueredo, Gray and Griskevicius2012) with respect to adolescent risk taking. Once again, however, an evolutionary perspective with its emphasis on selection of phenotypes that foster the dispersion of genes in future generations does not mean that nothing should be done. Such would reflect succumbing to the naturalistic fallacy, inferring what “ought to be” from what “is,” a long-derided perspective. However, we do suggest that seeing development the way natural selection views it may facilitate efforts to address contemporary concerns.

Fast versus slow development

Because of its grounding in evolutionary life-history theory (Kaplan & Gangestad, Reference Kaplan, Gangestad and Buss2005; Stearns, Reference Stearns1992), psychosocial acceleration theory appreciated that growing up fast and behaving in putatively nonoptimal ways was sometimes advantageous when it came to dispersing genes in future generations. Although maturing early is known to carry numerous risks for females, including early sexual debut, early first birth, sexually transmitted disease, and breast cancer (Ellis, Reference Ellis2004), such “ontogenic risk taking” was theorized to make biological sense under conditions of adversity. It was for this reason that Belsky et al. (Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991) proposed that rearing conditions that induced in the individual the sense that the future was risky and that supportive relationships were precarious would promote early maturation, early sexual debut, promiscuous sexual behavior, unstable pair bonds, limited parental investment, and the bearing of more rather than fewer offspring. After all, from the standpoint of reproductive fitness, it is better to “live fast and die young,” having offspring along the way, than to die (or become disabled) before getting the chance to reproduce (Chisholm, Reference Chisholm1993; Nettle, Reference Nettle2010). Thus, adolescents who, for example, respond to dangerous environments by developing insecure attachments; adopting opportunistic, advantage-taking interpersonal orientations; engaging in externalizing behavior; discounting the future; and experiencing early sexual debut are no less functional or even less regulated than are those responding to a well-resourced and supportive social environment by developing the opposing characteristics and orientations (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991; Ellis, Boyce, et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011).

What this analysis highlights is that development involves trade-offs, a concept central to an evolutionary, life-history perspective. As just noted, a primary risk associated with waiting to mature, find a mate, and breed is that one may not survive long enough to achieve any or all of these ends. Additional risks include limits to ones’ capabilities and competencies, as well as poor health, thereby making it difficult to attract mates and/or protect and facilitate the development of the offspring one eventually produces. Ellis and associates (in press) recently pointed out in this regard that the concept of allostatic load, which informs ever more developmental inquiry (e.g., Evans & Kim, Reference Evans and Kim2012; Fuller-Rowell, Evans, & Ong, Reference Fuller-Rowell, Evans and Ong2012), may need rethinking. Instead of conceptualizing this construct as part of a disease process, it could be better to think of it in terms of evolutionary and life-history trade-offs. Although there are indisputable costs associated with developing in certain ways under conditions of ecological stress, costs that seem to undermine child well-being, long-term health and even longevity, these costs may be compensated for by the increased likelihood that the individual incurring these costs would make it to maturity, find a mate, and produce offspring.

It is intriguing that the aforementioned rodent work once again proves consistent with the view just advanced: life conditions can regulate the rate of development in the service of fitness goals. Moreover, this is because it is not true that stressful rearing conditions that take the form of limited maternal licking of the rat pup increase fearful and defensive behaviors via the epigenetic regulation of the stress-response system. All too unappreciated by many developmentalists who (appropriately) herald this work for its documentation of such effects is that such putatively limited maternal care also accelerates sexual maturation, increases sexual behavior, and reduces parental investment. It would appear then that parental “neglect” in the form of limited licking and grooming serves as a means of regulating (not dysregulating) development in a biologically strategic manner. This is consistent with Belsky et al.'s (Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991) psychosocial acceleration theory, as noted by Cameron and associates (Reference Cameron, Champagne, Parent, Fish, Ozaki-Kuroda and Meaney2005). In this rodent context, neglect can be regarded as a mechanism through which rat mothers facilitate their offspring's development by accelerating it toward survival and reproductive strategies that are optimal under conditions of adversity. Therefore, it would seem mistaken to view diminished licking and grooming as “poor maternal care” or the development induced by such care as disturbed, dysregulated, or nonoptimal. From an evolutionary perspective, the care provided by the putatively neglectful parents may serve as appropriate preparation of their offspring for the ecological conditions into which they are likely to mature.

Costs and limits of plasticity

Like many theories and perspectives on human development, psychosocial acceleration theory presumes that development is contextually conditional and developmentally plastic. Developmental experiences and environmental exposures regulate growth and development such that how an individual turns out is at least partly based on environmental influences, shared or unshared, to use the terminology of behavior genetics (Plomin & Daniels, Reference Plomin and Daniels1987). Evolutionists use the terminology adaptive developmental plasticity to characterize this process (Schlichting & Pigliucci, Reference Schlichting and Pigliucci1998; West-Eberhard, Reference West-Eberhard2003), as do evolutionary-minded developmentalists (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991; Chisholm, Reference Chisholm1993; Ellis, Reference Ellis2004; Kuzawa, Reference Kuzawa2005; Nettle, Reference Nettle2011; Quinlan, Reference Quinlan2007).

However, what many developmentalists have failed to appreciate (and we include ourselves among this group, at least until recently) is that plasticity carries costs (DeWitt, Sih, & Wilson, Reference DeWitt, Sih and Wilson1998; Frankenhuis & Del Giudice, Reference Frankenhuis and Del Giudice2012; Schlichting & Pigliucci, Reference Schlichting and Pigliucci1998; Sih, Reference Sih2011). There is the cost associated with the increased complexity that may be required of organisms in order to be capable of developing in diverse ways depending on contextual conditions. The costs derive from the idea that, as complexity increases, so does the risk of something going wrong, in the same way that a machine with more moving parts has more ways to break down than one with fewer parts. Consider the following and related costs of plasticity enumerated by DeWitt and associates (Reference DeWitt, Sih and Wilson1998): energetic costs of the sensory and regulatory mechanisms of plasticity; the production cost of environmentally inducible structures and processes; the cost of acquiring information about the environment, including the energy expended and potential risks incurred; and the imprecision in “manufacturing” a phenotype based on environmental experience. Collectively, these would seem to imply that some individuals may manifest limited malleability, with their development highly canalized, trading off the benefits of being able to thrive in more than a single ecological niche against costs of a breakdown in the developmental machinery and processes needed to do so.

It is also important to appreciate with respect to the costs of plasticity that modifying development early in life in response to developmental experiences and environmental exposures may result in a “mismatch” between the developed organism and the environment in which it finds itself later in life (Nederhof & Schmidt, Reference Nederhof and Schmidt2012; Sih, Reference Sih2011). In this way, seemingly adaptive developmental plasticity, selected to allow the organism to be “programmed” on the basis of early experience so as to “fit” the later environment, can undermine developmental opportunity and eventual fitness rather than enhance it. Thus, here is another reason why there seem to be grounds for expecting individual differences in developmental plasticity. After all, those individuals whose development is more canalized and less programmable by environmental exposures would be less likely to experience a mismatch resulting from developmental plasticity. Of course, such highly canalized specialists with more or less fixed characteristics could end up mismatched to their environment, but for different reasons from that of the plastic, programmable generalist, who may develop in a manner guided by experiences earlier in development.

What these observations imply is that the being developmentally malleable should not be regarded, as it typically is, as an unmitigated good for the developing organism and, thus, as something that should be pronounced in all individuals. In some cases it would appear wiser not to be plastic or to be less malleable, manifesting a more canalized pattern of development, than others and thus less susceptible to environmental conditions. What this suggests, as noted repeatedly, is that we should expect individual differences in developmental plasticity. In other words, developmental plasticity should be conceptualized as a phenotype in its own right, something that will vary across individuals for a variety of reasons. For the most part, this possibility has not been appreciated by those studying development and psychopathology, at least not until recently. Where it has, questions can be raised about how those studying development and psychopathology have conceptualized the issue.

New Directions for Theory and Research

Recall that central to the prevailing diathesis–stress model of environmental action and psychopathology is the view that some individuals are more susceptible than are others to the negative developmental effects of contextual adversity; these are the so-called vulnerable ones. We have already questioned whether it would make evolutionary sense for nature to craft a species in which some individuals were more susceptible than others, especially for organismic reasons, to having their development compromised by becoming disordered or dysregulated. Recall, however, that we then delineated why this could nevertheless occur, especially once we eschewed concepts like “developmentally compromised” and optimal development, while appreciating that what is often regarded as nonoptimal and dysregulated could enhance fitness or could have in our ancestral past under certain conditions.

An alternative, evolutionary-inspired way of thinking about Person × Environment interaction has recently gained a foothold in the developmental literature and psychopathological literature, one that is based on the appreciation of the costs of plasticity. It differs from the traditional diathesis–stress model by treating developmental plasticity as a phenotype while proposing that what appears to be evidence of disproportionate vulnerability to adversity may actually reflect more general and often undetected negative and positive susceptibility to environmental influences in the mental-health sense of these terms (Belsky, Reference Belsky1997, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009b; Boyce & Ellis, Reference Boyce and Ellis2005; Ellis, Boyce et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011). The argument has been advanced that the former viewpoint has been more widely considered than the latter not only because of the absence of an evolutionary perspective when thinking about development and psychopathology but also because of the designs of many investigations (Belsky, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a; Ellis, Boyce et al., Reference Ellis and Boyce2011). Rather than studying the full range of environmental exposures and of development from negative to positive, there has been a disproportionate, even if understandable (from a humanitarian point of view), focus on contextual adversity and developmental dysfunction/dysregulation. This has resulted in individuals who may be disproportionately responsive to negative and positive developmental or other environmental experiences and exposures being regarded exclusively as disproportionately susceptible to the negative effects of adversity.

Fortunately, this situation is changing rapidly, but it remains that numerous issues related to developmental plasticity as a phenotype and differential susceptibility to environmental influences are in need of attention. In this section, we therefore highlight these as a way of outlining future directions for research. Along the way we also summarize some of the latest differential-susceptibility related findings, virtually all of which have appeared since our 2009 reviews of the literature (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a).

Resilience or reduced plasticity?

Implicit in the diathesis–stress model is the notion of resilience. After all, the resilient individual develops in a manner opposite that of the vulnerable one according to this theoretical perspective, because he or she is, by definition, “protected” from the anticipated negative effects of adversity (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch2012; Garmezy, Masten, & Tellegen, Reference Garmezy, Masten and Tellegen1984; Masten, Best, & Garmezy, Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990; Rutter, Reference Rutter1987, Reference Rutter2012; Werner & Smith, Reference Werner and Smith1982). Because diathesis–stress thinking focuses upon response to adversity, it makes no claims about differences in how resilient and vulnerable individuals develop under benign or supportive conditions. In this regard it differs substantially from differential-susceptibility thinking, because differential susceptibility presumes that the putatively vulnerable are generally plastic in a “for-better-and for-worse manner” (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007); whereas the putatively resilient will be relatively unresponsive to supportive conditions, just like they are to unsupportive ones, with their developmental pathways highly canalized. This view leads us to take issue with the use of the terms buffer and protection when discussing how more developmentally plastic individuals respond to positive circumstances; such individuals are not so much protected (against what?) when they experience good conditions, as some contend (e.g., Zimmermann et al., Reference Zimmermann, Brueckl, Nocon, Pfister, Binder and Uhr2011), but they experience enhanced functioning, something not expected of those resilient to adversity (for a review, see Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2013).

To the extent that this is the case, it would suggest that resilience, or at least that which derives from endogenous characteristics of the individual (e.g., temperament, genotype), is something of a misnomer, reflecting a highly canalized developmental process with limited developmental plasticity more than anything else (Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2013). Unfortunately, too many investigations of risk and protection informed by diathesis–stress thinking have not only failed to entertain this possibility for theoretical reasons but have also not been well positioned to evaluate it for reasons previously outlined: a supportive environment in much resilience research often reflects little more than the absence of adversity (e.g., not impoverished, not maltreated), and positive functioning often reflects nothing more than the absence of dysfunction (e.g., not depressed, not aggressive). In order to be able to determine whether those resilient to adversity are also relatively unresponsive to environmental support and enrichment, as the differential-susceptibility framework presupposes, more will be required than merely expanding the range of measurements of environments and development.

Vantage sensitivity: Response to positive environmental influences

According to differential susceptibility reasoning, diathesis–stress thinking only addresses the “dark side” of the Person × Environment equation, ignoring the “bright side.” Recently, we adopted and promoted the term vantage sensitivity, originally introduced by Manuck and associates (Manuck, Reference Manuck2011; Sweitzer et al., Reference Sweitzer, Halder, Flory, Craig, Gianaros and Ferrell2013), to convey the notion that some individuals are more sensitive and positively responsive to the environmental advantages to which they are exposed and vantage resistance to describe the absence of such responsivity in other individuals (Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2013). In that effort, we also reviewed emerging empirical evidence for vantage sensitivity as a function of behavioral, physiological, and genetic factors previously associated with differential susceptibility and diathesis–stress. Although the same factors seem to moderate effects of environmental influences, irrespective of whether they are exclusively negative (i.e., diathesis–stress), exclusively positive (i.e., vantage sensitivity), or range from the negative to the positive (i.e., differential susceptibility), it is important to caution against inferring that every risk factor will also, by default, function as a vantage-sensitivity and thus differential-susceptibility factor. Ultimately, this is an empirical question, as there may very well be specific factors that play a predominant role in diathesis–stress but not in vantage sensitivity and vice versa. Future research should address whether there are psychological, behavioral, or neurobiological factors and mechanisms that are specific to diathesis–stress or vantage sensitivity.

Competitive evaluation of models of Person × Environment interaction

Much of the evidence cited in our earlier reviews of differential-susceptibility related evidence (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a), as well as in subsections to follow, is based on “eye-ball” tests of graphed interactions between an environmental predictor and developmental outcome moderated by a plasticity factor. The limits of this approach to “testing” the differential-susceptibility hypothesis led Belsky et al. (Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007) to propose explicit empirical criteria. Subsequently, Kochanska, Kim, Barry and Philibert (Reference Kochanska, Kim, Barry and Philibert2011) extended this effort by using the regions of significance approach (Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, Reference Preacher, Curran and Bauer2006; Takane & Cramer, Reference Takane and Cramer1975) to separately evaluate the for-better and for-worse sides of the differential-susceptibility hypothesis upon documenting the required cross-over interaction.

Roisman and associates (Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012) recently offered an even more demanding approach for evaluating differential susceptibility by providing two new quantitative metrics in combination with the regions of significance analysis: the proportion of interaction index, which represents the proportion of the total area of an interaction plot that is uniquely attributable to differential susceptibility, and the proportion affected index which quantifies the proportion of people in the sample who fall above the cross-over point (i.e., proportion of sample in the for-better condition). In addition, Roisman et al. (Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012) recommend testing for nonlinear relationships between predictor and outcome and correction for multiple comparisons.

All of the approaches offered to date to evaluate differential susceptibility have involved conducting essentially exploratory tests of Organism × Environment interactions as a first step before further interrogating the data at hand. Widaman and associates (Belsky, Pluess, & Widaman, Reference Belsky, Pluess and Widaman2013; Widaman et al., Reference Widaman, Helm, Castro-Schilo, Pluess, Stallings and Belsky2012) recently advanced a strategy that sidesteps such approaches, moving directly to test competing theoretical models. In a first step, the cross-over point between high- and low-susceptible individuals is estimated with the help of a reparameterized regression equation (Widaman et al., in press). If the estimated cross-over point lies within the observed range of the predictor, four additional reparameterized regression models are applied: two constrained to reflect a weak and a strong version of diathesis–stress and two to reflect a weak and a strong version of differential susceptibility. Fit indices of the four models are then compared to evaluate whether the data fits better with a diathesis–stress or differential-susceptibility model (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Pluess and Widaman2013). The strong version of differential susceptibility presupposes that there are those who are susceptible to positive and negative contextual conditions and others who are not; the weak version presupposes that, although many may be susceptible to environmental influences to some extent, some are more affected than others.

The time has thus come to move beyond exploratory tests of Organism × Environment interaction in regression and analysis of variance designs and to more formally evaluate in a competitive manner the a priori hypotheses central to diathesis–stress and differential-susceptibility models of environmental action.

Temperament/negative emotionality as plasticity factor

The first evidence Belsky (Reference Belsky1997, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005) could point to consistent with his evolutionary-inspired, differential-susceptibility hypothesis surprisingly indicated that a difficult temperament or high levels of negative emotionality (e.g., fear, inhibition) early in life not only function as a risk factor that increases the likelihood of problematic functioning when coupled with contextual risks (e.g., poverty, maternal depression), as long presumed, but also seemed to predispose individuals to benefit from benign or supportive experiences. Since our 2009 review, even more evidence to this effect has emerged in the case of children, as revealed in research linking maternal empathy (Pitzer, Jennen-Steinmetz, Esser, Schmidt, & Laucht, Reference Pitzer, Jennen-Steinmetz, Esser, Schmidt and Laucht2011) and anger (Poehlmann et al., Reference Poehlmann, Hane, Burnson, Maleck, Hamburger and Shah2012) with externalizing problems; mutual responsiveness observed in the mother–child dyad with effortful control (Kim & Kochanska, Reference Kim and Kochanska2012); intrusive maternal behavior (Conway & Stifter, Reference Conway and Stifter2012) and poverty (Raver, Blair, & Willoughby, Reference Raver, Blair and Willoughby2012) with executive functioning; and sensitive parenting with social, emotional, and cognitive–academic development (Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012).

However, it is not just responsiveness to parenting that negativity seems to moderate in a for better and for worse manner, as documented in recent work examining the influence of teacher–child conflict on change in symptomology during the primary-school years (Essex, Armstrong, Burk, Goldsmith, & Boyce, Reference Essex, Armstrong, Burk, Goldsmith and Boyce2011) and of marital quality on sensitive mothering (Jessee et al., Reference Jessee, Mangelsdorf, Brown, Schoppe-Sullivan, Shigeto and Wong2010). The latter work also makes it clear that it is not just children who prove differentially susceptible to environmental effects. We would be remiss, however, if we failed to call attention to studies showing it was less rather than more negatively emotional or reactive children who proved highly responsive to environmental experiences in a manner consistent with differential susceptibility (Essex et al., Reference Essex, Armstrong, Burk, Goldsmith and Boyce2011, see their figure 2, bottom; Du Rocher Schudlich, White, Fleischhauer, & Fitzgerald, Reference Du Rocher Schudlich, White, Fleischhauer and Fitzgerald2011) and to other evidence showing that sometimes difficult temperament operates only in a manner consistent with the diathesis–stress rather than differential-susceptibility framework (Kochanska & Kim, Reference Kochanska and Kim2013; Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012; Yaman, Mesman, van IJzendoorn, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference Yaman, Mesman, van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2010).

The cited research reveals that diverse indices of negative emotionality function as plasticity factors (e.g., fear, inhibition, distress, difficult temperament, negative affect), which raises several issues that future research should address. For example, is there a particular component or feature of negativity that is principally responsible for the moderational effects detected (Buss, Reference Buss2011) or could more progress be made with regard to this issue by embracing the integrated, evolutionary, hawk–dove model of temperament (Korte, Koolhaas, Wingfield, & McEwen, Reference Korte, Koolhaas, Wingfield and McEwen2005) that Sturge-Apple, Davies, Martin, Cicchetti and Hentges (Reference Sturge-Apple, Davies, Martin, Cicchetti and Hentges2012) recently found to be empirically useful? Whereas hawklike strategies are characterized by approach, dominant–negative affect (e.g., anger), and activity, dovelike ones involving avoidance, inhibition, and vulnerable affect (e.g., fear) seem more consistent with heightened plasticity according to existing research.

We also wonder about the extent to which the moderational effect of negative emotionality depends on the origins of this plasticity factor, given evidence that negativity is heritable (e.g., Buss & Plomin, Reference Buss and Plomin1984; Rhee et al., Reference Rhee, Corley, Friedman, Hewitt, Hink and Johnson2012) but also shaped by prenatal and postnatal experience (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009b; Davis, Glynn, Waffarn, & Sandman, Reference Davis, Glynn, Waffarn and Sandman2011; Glynn & Sandman, Reference Glynn and Sandman2011; for a review, see Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2011), although sometimes more so in some genotypes than others (Ivorra et al., Reference Ivorra, Sanjuan, Jover, Carot, Frutos and Molto2010; Pluess et al., Reference Pluess, Velders, Belsky, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Jaddoe2011). Thus, what may be particularly important is distinguishing negative emotionality that reflects an experience-induced failure to self-regulate and that which reflects a general hypersensitivity to the environment.

Physiological reactivity as a plasticity factor

Belsky's (Reference Belsky1997, Reference Belsky, Ellis and Bjorklund2005) differential-susceptibility hypothesis was based entirely on evolutionary reasoning, but Boyce and Ellis’ (Reference Boyce and Ellis2005) evolutionary-oriented biological sensitivity to context (BSC) proposal emerged in response to an unanticipated finding that Boyce and associates (Reference Boyce, Chesney, Alkon, Tschann, Adams and Chesterman1995) generated a decade earlier. Central to the BSC framework is the claim that physiological reactivity is a plasticity factor regulated by environmental experience. The recently promulgated adaptive calibration model represents the further development of these ideas (Del Giudice, Ellis, & Shirtcliff, Reference Del Giudice, Ellis and Shirtcliff2011).

The evidence indicating that physiological reactivity operates as a plasticity factor is diverse, with findings showing that more physiologically reactive children are more susceptible, in a for better and for worse fashion, to the effects of actual marital conflict (Obradovic, Bush, & Boyce, Reference Obradovic, Bush and Boyce2011), as well as simulated interparental aggression (Davies, Sturge-Apple, & Cicchetti, Reference Davies, Sturge-Apple and Cicchetti2011), on externalizing problems; of family adversity on school achievement (Obradovic, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler, & Boyce, Reference Obradovic, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler and Boyce2010); and of teacher–child conflict on change in symptom severity (Essex et al., Reference Essex, Armstrong, Burk, Goldsmith and Boyce2011). Work showing similar moderational effects of physiological reactivity with respect to family influences on female pubertal development by Ellis and associates (Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011) is especially noteworthy, given the roots of this work in psychosocial acceleration theory (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991) and the way in which its findings proved consistent with the differential-susceptibility related revision of the theory (Belsky, Reference Belsky, Rodgers, Rowe and Miller2000). After all, Belsky (Reference Belsky, Rodgers, Rowe and Miller2000) postulated that some females would be “alternative strategists” whose pubertal development proved unrelated to their rearing experiences, just as Ellis and associates (Reference Ellis, Shirtcliff, Boyce, Deardorff and Essex2011) found for girls low in physiological reactivity, whereas others would be “conditional strategists” susceptible to the accelerating and delaying effects of unsupportive and supportive rearing, respectively, just as Ellis et al. (Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011) observed in the case of highly reactive girls. Once again, though, we would be remiss if we did not highlight some evidence inconsistent with the BSC thinking, such as that from the aforementioned Obradovic et al. (Reference Obradovic, Bush and Boyce2011) investigation showing that it was young children with low rather than high respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity who seemed, in some analyses, to benefit from growing up under supportive family conditions characterized by little marital strife.

One of the key questions that still remains to be answered regarding BSC concerns its life span implications. More specifically, do those individuals who are induced to be highly physiologically reactive early in life, and thus developmentally plastic, remain that way over time? It seems hard to imagine that a child induced by supportive rearing to be highly physiologically reactive, in accordance with the theory (Boyce & Ellis, Reference Boyce and Ellis2005), would remain highly reactive as the years go by. Of course, this is an empirical question, one that has implications for our understanding of the role physiological reactivity does or does not play as a plasticity factor as the individual develops.

Genetic polymorphisms as plasticity factors

Perhaps nowhere has the diathesis–stress framework informed Person × Environment interaction research in recent years more than in the study of G × E interaction, launched by Caspi and associates (Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin and Craig2002, Reference Caspi, Sugden, Moffitt, Taylor, Craig and Harrington2003) a decade ago. In the time since our 2009 reviews of differential-susceptibility related evidence (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Jonassaint, Pluess, Stanton, Brummett and Williams2009; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a), ever more G × E findings have appeared consistent with the notion that there are individual differences in developmental plasticity. Much of the evidence to this effect comes from studies involving the serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) and the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4).

Consider in this regard evidence showing for better and for worse results in the case of those carrying one or more 5-HTTLPR short alleles when the rearing predictor and child outcome were maternal responsiveness and moral internalization (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Kim, Barry and Philibert2011), child maltreatment and antisocial behavior (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Thibodeau, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Thibodeau2012), and supportive parenting and positive affect (Hankin et al., Reference Hankin, Nederhof, Oppenheimer, Jenness, Young and Abela2011), respectively. Differential-susceptibility related findings also emerged (among male, African American adolescents) when perceived racial discrimination was used to predict conduct problems (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Beach, Chen, Obasi, Philibert, Kogan and Simons2011), when life events were used to predict neuroticism (Pluess, Belsky, Way, & Taylor, Reference Pluess, Belsky, Way and Taylor2010) and life satisfaction of young adults (Kuepper et al., Reference Kuepper, Wielpuetz, Alexander, Mueller, Grant and Hennig2012), and when retrospectively reported childhood adversity was used to explain aspects of impulsivity among college students (e.g., pervasive influence of feelings, feelings trigger action; Carver, Johnson, Joormann, Kim, & Nam, Reference Carver, Johnson, Joormann, Kim and Nam2011).

To clarify, other 5-HTTLPR related findings prove more consistent with the diathesis–stress than the differential-susceptibility framework (e.g., Bakermans-Kranenburg, Dobrova-Krol, & van IJzendoorn, in press; Brody et al., Reference Brody, Yu, Chen, Kogan, Evans and Windle2012), sometimes even in G × E studies that also chronicle differential susceptibility (e.g., Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Kim, Barry and Philibert2011). Moreover, in apparently rare instances it is those with long rather than short alleles who prove most susceptible to environmental influences, even when differential-susceptibility related findings emerge (e.g., Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Oshri, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Oshri2011; Sulik et al., Reference Sulik, Eisenberg, Lemery-Chalfant, Spinrad, Silva and Eggum2012). Therefore, the results of a recent meta-analysis of G × E findings pertaining to children under 18 years of age are especially noteworthy; they show that short allele carriers are more susceptible to the effects of positive and negative developmental experiences and environmental exposures, at least in Caucasians (van IJzendoorn, Belsky, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference van IJzendoorn, Belsky and Bakermans-Kranenburg2012).

Turning to DRD4, it is worth highlighting that recent differential-susceptibility related evidence showing heightened or exclusive susceptibility of individuals carrying the seven-repeat (7R) allele when the environmental predictor and developmental outcome were maternal positivity and prosocial behavior (Knafo, Israel, & Ebstein, Reference Knafo, Israel and Ebstein2011), early nonfamilial childcare and social competence (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2013), contextual stress and support and adolescent negative arousal (Beach et al., Reference Beach, Lei, Brody, Simons, Cutrona and Philibert2012), childhood adversity and young-adult persistent alcohol dependence (Park, Sher, Todorov, & Heath, Reference Park, Sher, Todorov and Heath2011), and newborn risk status (i.e., gestational age, birth weight for gestational age, length of stay in neonatal intensive care unit) and observed maternal sensitivity (Fortuna et al., Reference Fortuna, van IJzendoorn, Mankuta, Kaitz, Avinun and Ebstein2011), respectively. However, note that at least one investigatory team finds that it is those without the 7R allele who prove most responsive to childhood adversities in a diathesis–stress manner (Das, Cherbuin, Tan, Anstey, & Easteal, Reference Das, Cherbuin, Tan, Anstey and Easteal2011). Nevertheless, a meta-analysis of G × E research involving dopamine-related genes revealed that children 8 years and younger respond to positive and negative developmental experiences and environmental exposures in a manner consistent with differential susceptibility (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011).

Emerging evidence has also begun to suggest that the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene (BDNF) may be a plasticity gene (e.g., Chen, Li, & McGue, Reference Chen, Li and McGue2012; Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, Wenner, Thomas, Glatt, McKenna and Clark2012; Juhasz et al., Reference Juhasz, Dunham, McKie, Thomas, Downey and Chase2011; Mata, Thompson, & Gotlib, Reference Mata, Thompson and Gotlib2010; Suzuki et al., Reference Suzuki, Matsumoto, Shibuya, Sadahiro, Kamata and Goto2011), as is the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR; Johansson et al., Reference Johansson, Bergman, Corander, Waldman, Karrani and Salo2012; Poulin, Holman, & Buffone, Reference Poulin, Holman and Buffone2012; Sturge-Apple, Cicchetti, Davies, & Suor, Reference Sturge-Apple, Cicchetti, Davies and Suor2012) and the FK506 binding protein 5 gene (Bevilacqua et al., Reference Bevilacqua, Carli, Sarchiapone, George, Goldman, Roy and Enoch2012; Xie et al., Reference Xie, Kranzler, Poling, Stein, Anton and Farrer2010; Zimmermann et al., Reference Zimmermann, Brueckl, Nocon, Pfister, Binder and Uhr2011). There are perhaps less frequent indications of this in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (Laucht et al., Reference Laucht, Blomeyer, Buchmann, Treutlein, Schmidt and Esser2012; Nijmeijer et al., Reference Nijmeijer, Hartman, Rommelse, Altink, Buschgens and Fliers2010) and, in the previously reviewed literature, the monoamine oxidase A gene (MAOA; Enoch, Steer, Newman, Gibson, & Goldman, Reference Enoch, Steer, Newman, Gibson and Goldman2010; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Kistner, Pine, Biesecker, Pickett and Skol2010). It will be worth monitoring these and other polymorphisms as more work emerges to see whether additional evidence proves consistent with differential susceptibility.

Given that virtually all of the polymorphisms just considered have been studied in G × E perspective as a result of the original interests of psychiatric geneticists in simple genotype–phenotype linkages, we cannot but wonder whether an entirely different set of polymorphisms reflecting sensitivity to the environment would have emerged had the phenotype of interest been not some psychiatric disorder but developmental plasticity itself. It therefore behooves investigators to expand the list of genetic “suspects” beyond those thought to be related to disturbances in functioning by thinking biologically about genes that could be related to physiological processes instantiating plasticity. An excellent recent example of this approach is found in Grazioplene, DeYoung, Rogosch, and Cicchetti's (Reference Grazioplene, DeYoung, Rogosch and Cicchetti2013) work showing that the neuronal acetylcholine receptor subunit α-4 genotype interacts with childhood maltreatment to predict features of personality in a differential-susceptibility related manner. This polymorphism was selected for investigation because it is involved in the production of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine, a component of the cholinergic system that is strongly involved in neural plasticity and learning.

Polygenetic plasticity

Most G × E research has focused on one or another polymorphism. In recent years, however, work that we believe should be encouraged has emerged focusing on multiple polymorphisms, thus reflecting the operation of epistatic (i.e., G × G) interactions (e.g., Beaver, Sak, Vaske, & Nilsson, Reference Beaver, Sak, Vaske and Nilsson2010; Conner, Hellemann, Ritchie, & Noble, Reference Conner, Hellemann, Ritchie and Noble2010) as well as G × G × E polymorphisms. One can distinguish polygenetic G × E research in terms of the basis used for creating multigene composites. One strategy that seems fundamentally problematic to us involves identifying genes that show main effects and then compositing only these to then test an interaction with some environmental parameter (e.g., Docherty, Kovas, & Plomin, Reference Docherty, Kovas and Plomin2011). What leads us to question the wisdom of this approach is that genes or any other factors that do not exert main effects could still be involved in interactions with the environment, just as some recent G × E research reveals (e.g., Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch2012; Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Oshri, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Oshri2011; Pluess et al., Reference Pluess, Velders, Belsky, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Jaddoe2011).

Another approach is to composite genes for a secondary, follow-up analysis that has been found in a first round of inquiry to generate significant G × E interactions (e.g., Brody, Chen, & Beach, Reference Brody, Chen and Beach2013; Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Oades, Psychogiou, Chen, Franke and Buitelaar2009). When Cicchetti and Rogosch (Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch2012, see their figure 2) applied this approach using four different polymorphisms, they found that as the number of sensitivity to the environment alleles increased (i.e., short–short allele of 5-HTTLPR, zero copies of the corticotropin releasing hormone receptor 1 gene TAT haplotype, the TT genotype of DRD4-521C/T, and A carrier of OXTR), so did the degree to which maltreated and nonmaltreated low-income children differed on a composite measure of resilient functioning in a for better and for worse manner.

A third approach that has now been used successfully a number of times to chronicle differential susceptibility involves compositing a set of genes selected on an a priori basis before evaluating G × E (e.g., Brody et al., Reference Brody, Chen and Beach2013). Consider in this regard evidence indicating that two-gene composites moderate links between sexual abuse and adolescent depression/anxiety and somatic symptoms (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Sturge-Apple, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Sturge-Apple2007), between perceived racial discrimination and risk-related cognitions reflecting a fast versus slow life-history strategy (Gibbons et al., Reference Gibbons, Roberts, Gerrard, Li, Beach and Simons2012, see their figure 2), between contextual stress/support and aggression in young adulthood (Simons et al., Reference Simons, Lei, Beach, Brody, Philibert and Gibbons2011), and between social class and postpartum depression (Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Notterman, Brooks-Gunn, Hobcraft, Garfinkel and Jaeger2011); that a three-gene composite moderates the relation between a hostile–demoralizing community and family environment and aggression in early adulthood (Simons et al., Reference Simons, Lei, Beach, Brody, Philibert and Gibbons2011); and that a five-gene composite moderates the relation between parenting and adolescent self-control (Belsky & Beaver, Reference Belsky and Beaver2011).

No matter how informative the approach to compositing polymorphisms on an a priori basis has proven to be when detecting differential susceptibility, we suspect that the wave of the future will be the “system-level genetic approach.” This involves combining genes considered to collectively influence a particular biological process or pathway, such as the dopaminergic or serotonergic system (e.g., Brody et al., Reference Brody, Chen and Beach2013; Conner et al., Reference Conner, Hellemann, Ritchie and Noble2010; Nikolova, Ferrell, Manuck, & Hariri, Reference Nikolova, Ferrell, Manuck and Hariri2011), or neurological endophenotypes for increased susceptibility, such as amygdala volume (Yap et al., Reference Yap, Whittle, Yucel, Sheeber, Pantelis and Simmons2008), hippocampus volume (Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Yap, Sheeber, Dudgeon, Yuecel and Pantelis2011), and ventral striatum reactivity (Nikolova, Bogdan, Brigidi, & Hariri, Reference Nikolova, Bogdan, Brigidi and Hariri2012), to list just a few possibilities. Further insight could be gained, conceivably, by even more refined analysis of such subsystems, including those reflecting synthesis, degradation/transport, receptor, and modulation (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Moyzis, Stern, He, Li and Li2011). To date, these latter approaches have not been used in studies of G × E to our knowledge, whether evaluating or not evaluating the differential-susceptibility hypothesis. Of course, they could be.

G × E mechanisms

There are likely multiple explanations as to why the mechanisms in the G × E findings prove consistent with differential susceptibility and thus our claim that developmental plasticity should be regarded as a phenotype in its own right. When considering mechanisms, however, we must remember that this term means quite different things to different scholars. For example, mechanism may refer to brain structure or function to a brain scientist; for an endocrinologically oriented investigator it may refer to a hormonal phenomenon, for a geneticist to an epigenetic cascade, and for a cognitive neuroscientist an attentional process. Thus, what may qualify for a cognitive scientist as a “proximate mechanism” (e.g., attentional bias) may well be regarded by a geneticist focused on the epigenome as a “distal predictor.” Developmental and behavioral scientists should thus not be arrogant when stipulating what does and does not qualify as a mechanism. It ultimately depends on the level or levels of analysis at which one is working.

Based on recent empirical evidence, in the following we call attention to some possible differential-susceptibility related mechanisms at different levels of analysis. Instead of emphasizing the findings directly linking particular genes to particular psychological or biological processes as most genotype–phenotype and even G × E reports do, we highlight research showing that such processes are responsive to environmental exposures in a differential-susceptibility related manner.

In regard to cognitive processes, the following have been found to be moderated by 5-HTTLPR and an environmental factor in a for better and for worse manner: positive and negative attentional biases (Fox, Zougkou, Ridgewell, & Garner, Reference Fox, Zougkou, Ridgewell and Garner2011; see also Pergamin-Hight, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & Bar-Haim, Reference Pergamin-Hight, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn and Bar-Haim2012), reward sensitivity (Roiser, Rogers, Cook, & Sahakian, Reference Roiser, Rogers, Cook and Sahakian2006), and accurate processing of emotional faces (Jacobs et al., Reference Jacobs, Pine, Schoeny, Henry, Gollan and Moy2011). The same is true of inattention in DRD4 (Berry, Deater-Deckard, McCartney, Wang, & Petrill, Reference Berry, Deater-Deckard, McCartney, Wang and Petrill2013), working-memory accuracy and reaction time in BDNF (Gatt et al., Reference Gatt, Nemeroff, Dobson-Stone, Paul, Bryant and Schofield2009), and rumination in a two-gene composite (5-HTTLPR, BDNF; Clasen, Wells, Knopik, McGeary, & Beevers, Reference Clasen, Wells, Knopik, McGeary and Beevers2011) and “street code,” reflecting high value placed on being tough and aggressive, standing up for one's rights, and being ready to fight in the case of a three-gene composite (5-HTTLPR, DRD4, MAOA; Simons et al., Reference Simons, Lei, Stewart, Beach, Brody and Philibert2012).

In regard to physiological processes, 5-HTTLPR and an environmental factor have been found to interact in a differential-susceptibility related manner in predicting cortisol reactivity (Way & Taylor, Reference Way and Taylor2010), with the same being true of BDNF when the dependent variable is heart rate (Gatt et al., Reference Gatt, Nemeroff, Dobson-Stone, Paul, Bryant and Schofield2009) or evening salivary cortisol (Vinberg et al., Reference Vinberg, Trajkovska, Bennike, Knorr, Knudsen and Kessing2009). For brain measurements, Alexander and colleagues (Reference Alexander, Klucken, Koppe, Osinsky, Walter and Vaitl2012) present evidence that 5-HTTLPR interacts with stressful life events in a for better and for worse manner to predict left amygdala reactivity to fearful faces, whereas Gatt et al. (Reference Gatt, Nemeroff, Dobson-Stone, Paul, Bryant and Schofield2009) document much the same when using BDNF to forecast grey matter volume in the hippocampus and the amygdala. In addition, Klucken and associates (Reference Klucken, Wehrum, Schweckendiek, Merz, Hennig and Vaitl2013) report that BDNF interacts with positive and negative feedback in a for better and for worse manner during appetitive conditioning when predicting hemodynamic response in the amygdala. Evidence also suggests that individuals homozygous for the isoallele of a polymorphism in the mineralocorticoid receptor gene (rs5522) manifest the most and least right amygdala reactivity relative to valine carriers when they (retrospectively) reported experiencing high and low levels, respectively, of emotional neglect in childhood (Bogdan, Williamson, & Hariri, Reference Bogdan, Williamson and Hariri2012).

However, it may not be only adversity to which the amygdala of certain individuals proves especially sensitive. After all, an extensive meta-analysis showed that the amygdala responds to negative stimuli and even more strongly to positive stimuli (Sergerie, Chochol, & Armony, Reference Sergerie, Chochol and Armony2008). This suggests to us, as does the work cited in the preceding paragraph, that amygdala reactivity could be one of several central nervous mechanism by which differential susceptibility operates. This would certainly be consistent with the hypothesis proposed by the differential-susceptibility framework (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a) and the concept of sensory-processing sensitivity (Aron & Aron, Reference Aron and Aron1997; Aron, Aron, & Jagiellowicz, Reference Aron, Aron and Jagiellowicz2012) that heightened susceptibility is the function of a more sensitive central nervous system on which experiences register more easily and deeply. According to this neurosensitivity hypothesis, specific gene variants (e.g., 5-HTTLPR short allele, DRD4 7R) contribute to the increased sensitivity and responsivity of specific brain regions that then manifests itself in increased negative emotionality and physiological reactivity (Pluess, Stevens, & Belsky, Reference Pluess, Stevens, Belsky, Legerstee, Haley and Bornstein2013), in part because highly sensitive individuals are more easily aroused.

What this brief summary of diverse findings related to putative plasticity genes should make clear is that a variety of plausible mechanisms at multiple levels of analysis may help to account for the G × E findings noted or referred to earlier. More such research is required, and the big challenge going forward will be to “put Humpty Dumpty together” (even if not “again”) so that a multilevel system of causation can be illuminated (Hyde, Bogdan, & Hariri, Reference Hyde, Bogdan and Hariri2011). This represents a huge agenda, perhaps the holy grail of inquiry.

Repeated measurements

All of the differential-susceptibility related work cited herein, as well as in our earlier reviews, essentially presumes that children and adults who share the same characteristics, be they temperamental, physiological or genetic plasticity factors, would function in a manner opposite to what was observed were they also observed under contrasting conditions. For example, we have presumed that children carrying DRD4 7R and showing high levels of externalizing problems in the face of insensitive parenting would score low on such problems if they were raised by skilled caregivers; however, there is almost no research validating this assumption, in part because it is unethical to provide a child growing up under for-better, supportive conditions with for-worse, adverse ones just to test the differential-susceptibility hypothesis. One of the few studies that systematically addressed the repeated-measurement issue under consideration, even if not in such differential-susceptibility related terms or with children, examined the probability of taking a risk in a gambling-decision-making game when the chances of winning were high (i.e., for-better condition) and when they were low (i.e., for-worse condition; Roiser et al., Reference Roiser, Rogers, Cook and Sahakian2006). The results revealed that the more short 5-HTTLPR alleles an individual carried, the more likely the person was to make a bet (i.e., take a risk) when the chance of winning was high, and the less likely the individual was to make such a bet when the probability of winning was low.

In addition to constructing repeated-measurement experiments, future researchers would be well advised to take advantage of experiments in nature that could serve this purpose. Consider in this regard the imaginative work of Verschoor and Markus (Reference Verschoor and Markus2011), who took advantage of days in which undergraduates did and did not have examinations in order to see how those students homozygous for long and short 5-HTTLPR alleles functioned. They found that the latter experienced the most tension, negative mood, and perceived stress on exam days, yet the least on nonexam days. Consider Schoebi, Way, Karney, and Bradbury's (Reference Schoebi, Way, Karney and Bradbury2012) marital research, which indicated that spouses homozygous for short alleles proved more sensitive to their partner's positive and negative emotion during marital interactions.

Experimental evaluation of plasticity

All of the research cited through this point has been observational, correlational, and often longitudinal in character. The common strategy employed in such work for discounting the possibility that organism–environment correlation is masquerading as Organism × Environment interaction is to insure that the temperamental, physiological, or genetic moderator under study is not correlated with the contextual predictor under consideration. However laudable such efforts are, they are not without limits. For example, just because the moderating polymorphism (e.g., DRD4) under investigation proves unrelated to the contextual predictor of interest (e.g., sensitive parenting) does not mean that this is true of all other unmeasured or unexamined organismic factors (e.g., BDNF). Ultimately, that environments have not been systematically manipulated in most plasticity-related research means that one cannot be certain, even in the case of the putatively malleable, that truly causal environmental effects are being detected (van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Reference van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2012).

As pioneered by Dutch investigators in genetic plasticity factors (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Mesman, Alink, & Juffer, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Mesman, Alink and Juffer2008; Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman, & Juffer, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman and Juffer2008), one way around this problem involves conducting intervention experiments in which individuals are randomly assigned to a treatment condition. Because it is unethical to assign individuals, especially children, to conditions of adversity, this approach has only been used to evaluate differential response, as a function of some hypothesized plasticity factor, to an experience presumed to promote positive functioning. Such a design is limited, because it can only evaluate the for-better side of developmental plasticity. Nevertheless, it represents an ideal way to determine whether those considered to be resilient to adversity on the basis of observational research also prove relatively unresponsive to supportive conditions and whether those considered vulnerable to adversity disproportionately benefit from environmental enrichment; there is ever-growing experimental evidence that this is the case.

With regard to the plasticity factor of negative emotionality, consider findings showing that it is infants who score relatively low on irritability as newborns who fail to benefit from an otherwise security-promoting intervention (Cassidy, Woodhouse, Sherman, Stupica, & Lejuez, Reference Cassidy, Woodhouse, Sherman, Stupica and Lejuez2011) and infants who show few, if any, mild perinatal adversities (known to be related to limited negative emotionality) who fail to benefit from computer-based instruction otherwise found to promote preschoolers’ phonemic awareness and early literacy (van der Kooy-Hofland, van der Kooy, Bus, van IJzendoorn, & Bonsel, Reference van der Kooy-Hofland, van der Kooy, Bus, van IJzendoorn and Bonsel2012). In other words, only the putatively “vulnerable.” those manifesting or likely to manifest high levels of negativity, experienced developmental enhancement as a function of the interventions cited. Similar results emerge among older children, as Scott and O'Connor's (Reference Scott and O'Connor2012) parenting intervention resulted in the most positive change in conduct among emotionally dysregulated children (i.e., loses temper, angry, touchy). Van de Wiel, van Goozen, Matthys, Snoek, and van Engeland (Reference van de Wiel, van Goozen, Matthys, Snoek and van Engeland2004) presented related results when the plasticity factor was physiological reactivity, finding that an intervention for children with disruptive behavior disorder proved effective but only for those displaying high cortisol related, stress reactivity.

Recent genetically informed intervention evaluations also indicate that alleles presumed to place individuals at risk in the face of adversity (5-HTTLPR short, DRD4 7R) or to promote resilience (i.e., not short/not 7R) are associated with them being respectively susceptible or not susceptible to the benefits of intervention (but, for counterevidence, see Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Toth2011). Consider in this regard Drury and associates’ (Reference Drury, Gleason, Theall, Smyke, Nelson and Fox2012) data showing that it was only children growing up in Romanian orphanages who carried 5-HTTLPR short alleles who benefited, in terms of reductions in the display of indiscriminant friendliness, from being randomly assigned to high-quality foster care. Eley and associates (Reference Eley, Hudson, Creswell, Tropeano, Lester and Cooper2012) also documented intervention benefits restricted to short allele carriers, but their design included only treated children (i.e., not a randomized clinical trial [RCT]). Turning to DRD4, Kegel, Bus, and van IJzendoorn (Reference Kegel, Bus and van IJzendoorn2011) tested and found support for the hypothesis that it would be DRD4 7R carriers who would benefit from specially designed computer games promoting phonemic awareness and, thereby, early literacy in their RCT. Other RCT results point in the same direction with regard to DRD4 7R, including research on African American teens in which substance use was the examined outcome (Beach, Brody, Lei, & Philibert, Reference Beach, Brody, Lei and Philibert2010; Brody et al., Reference Brody, Chen and Beach2013, in press).

Last to be considered is research examining genetically moderated intervention effects by considering multigene composites rather than singular candidate genes. The Drury et al. (Reference Drury, Gleason, Theall, Smyke, Nelson and Fox2012) findings show that even though BDNF did not operate as a plasticity factor when it came to distinguishing those who did and did not benefit from the foster-care intervention, the moderating effect of 5-HTTLPR was amplified if a child carried methionine alleles and short 5-HTTLPR alleles. In other words, the more plasticity alleles children carried, the more their indiscriminant friendliness declined over time when assigned to foster care and the more it increased if they remained institutionalized. Consider next Brody et al.'s (Reference Brody, Chen and Beach2013) confirmed prediction that the more GABAergic and dopaminergic genes African American teens carried, the more protected they were from increasing their alcohol use over time when enrolled in a whole-family prevention program. Such results once again call attention to the benefits of moving beyond single polymorphisms when operationalizing the plasticity phenotype. They also indicate that even if a single gene may not moderate an intervention (or other environmental) effect, in could still play a role in determining the degree to which an individual benefits. These are insights future investigators and interventionists should keep in mind when seeking to find “what works for whom?”

Domain specific versus domain general

Conceptualizing plasticity as a phenotype would seem to imply, particularly as we have discussed it, that some individual differences will be more and some less developmentally malleable. However, should this be broadly interpreted to mean that some individuals will be more and some less malleable to any and all environmental inputs and with respect to any and all developmental sequelae? Is it more likely that a narrow conceptualization holds, such that plasticity will prove domain specific rather than domain general? Thus, instead of some being more plastic in general, individuals will vary in terms of what they are susceptible to (e.g., parents, not peers; peers, not parents) and with regard to particular developmental outcomes but not others (e.g., social but not academic competence; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2007; Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009a, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009b)?

However more likely the latter prospect would seem, two new sets of evidence underscore the domain generality of plasticity. The first involves the nature and effects of two dramatically different Dutch interventions that used different indirect and direct approaches to influence children and that focused on different developmental outcomes yet revealed that children sharing the same plasticity factor were the primary beneficiaries of each. More specifically, even though one intervention promoted sensitive parenting via video feedback in order to reduce toddler's externalizing behavior (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman and Juffer2008) and cortisol-related stress reactivity (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Mesman, et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Mesman, Alink and Juffer2008) and the other relied on a computerized instructional program to promote preschooler's phonemic awareness and thereby early literacy (Kegel et al., Reference Kegel, Bus and van IJzendoorn2011), it was children carrying the DRD4 7R allele who disproportionately or exclusively benefited in both cases. There is also the longitudinal data evaluated by Roisman and associates (Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012) showing that infants with difficult temperaments measured in the first 6 months of life proved more susceptible to the respective positive and negative effects of sensitive and insensitive mothering experienced during their first 3 years of life when it came to predicting teacher-reported social competence, behavior problems, and academic skills across the primary school years; this was so even with different teachers rating the children each and every year. Moreover, even though a diathesis–stress rather than differential-susceptibility model fit the data best when it came to predicting performance on objective tests of academic performance through 15 years of age, it once again proved to be children who as infants had difficult temperaments who were disproportionately susceptible to the shorter and longer term effects of early mothering. Of interest may be recent work showing that even though DRD4 and negative emotionality early in life are related (Holmboe, Nemoda, Fearon, Sasvari-Szekely, & Johnson, Reference Holmboe, Nemoda, Fearon, Sasvari-Szekely and Johnson2010; Ivorra et al., Reference Ivorra, Sanjuan, Jover, Carot, Frutos and Molto2010), the moderating effect of DRD4 does not account for a parallel moderating effect of negative emotionality, at least in a study of effects of child care on children's social functioning (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Pluess, Stevens, Belsky, Legerstee, Haley and Bornstein2013).

Despite such evidence, it still seems premature to embrace the broad domain-general interpretation of developmental plasticity. Nevertheless, the results just summarized caution against its premature dismissal. Ultimately, further work is called for before any firm conclusions can be drawn. Our suspicion is that there will be variation across individuals in that some will prove highly susceptible to many developmental experiences and environmental exposures (i.e., extremely plastic) and that some will prove susceptible to virtually none (i.e., extremely fixed) but that most will prove to be intermediate between these extremes. One could even envision a bell curve with regard to the degree of plasticity that individuals manifest.

Environmental cue reliability

Whether children develop in a generally supportive or unsupportive environment, it is likely that the reliability of environmental cues indicative of the valence of the rearing milieu will vary. One can imagine in this regard two different hostile environments: one in which conflict is constant and thus highly predictable and another in which conflict is episodic and unpredictable. Frankenhuis and Panchanathan (Reference Frankenhuis and Panchanathan2011) have insightfully observed that highly plastic individuals growing up in these two environments may differ in terms of when they “commit” to a developmental strategy, with the former “deciding” to specialize early, having high confidence about the state of the environment, but the latter deferring commitment and choosing to keep sampling the environment until a better estimate of the environment can be established. Whether this is actually the case is an issue that should be pursued empirically in the future. Frankenhuis and Panchanathan's (Reference Frankenhuis and Panchanathan2011) intriguing ideas lead us to wonder whether it is when cue reliability is low that individuals “call upon” the epigenome to infer from markings passed down across multiple generations what past environments were like and thus what is the best developmental pathway to pursue. Such a process would be consistent with Kuzawa's (Reference Kuzawa2005) notion of “intergenerational phenotypic inertia,” which highlights that it is the experiences of not only the immediately preceding generation to which developing organisms are sensitive but also generations preceding that.

Parent–child conflict of interest

Like many developmental scientists lacking foundational knowledge about evolutionary theory, virtually all of our thinking and writing about differential susceptibility to date has presumed that malleable children will have their development regulated by their rearing experiences. However, as Trivers (Reference Trivers1974) pointed out more than four decades ago, that parents and children share an average of 50% of their genes means that their biological interests are not the same and thus that they often experience a conflict of interest (see also Schlomer, Del Giudice, & Ellis, Reference Schlomer, Del Giudice and Ellis2011). In discussing the influence of prenatal stress on development, Del Giudice (Reference Del Giudice2012) recently pointed out that it is because of this that children, even highly malleable ones, may not simply take instructions from their parents. What are the implications of this observation for understanding developmental plasticity?

Family dynamics

However intrigued we have become with theory and evidence pertaining to differential susceptibility and the notion that individual differences exist in developmental plasticity, one issue has always caused us concern. We have never heard a parent characterize one of their children as more susceptible to their influence than another, which we regard as distinct from saying that their children are temperamentally different, with perhaps one being more “sensitive” to challenging or stressful situations than others or even being more “difficult” to care for. Why is that? Evolutionary reasoning would seem to imply that parents should detect for whom their socialization efforts, implicit or explicit, pay off and for whom they do not and so adjust those efforts accordingly. However, we are aware of no evidence suggesting or indicating this to be the case. Should not more malleable siblings seek to alert their parents that they are more responsive to rearing inputs in order to obtain more attention and other resources? However, could it be that such children do engage in such efforts but that they do not prove effective because less malleable children employ tactics to camouflage this characteristic of theirs in some way? Perhaps it is such children who prove most likely to claim “it's not fair” within families when siblings are perceived as being treated better as a way of “throwing their parents off track.” At the very least, it would be interesting to study parental awareness or perception of variation in susceptibility across siblings within a family.

Timing of susceptibility

It is widely assumed by many developmentalists that it is during the early years of life that human development is most susceptible to environmental influences, for better and for worse (e.g., Blair & Raver, Reference Blair and Raver2012; Ganzel & Morris, Reference Ganzel and Morris2011), as plasticity is presumed to be greatest when biological systems are being laid down. Even if this is true in general, does it mean that it is true in the case of each and every individual? It certainly seems conceivable that, at times in the ancestral past, individuals who were especially susceptible to environmental influence at some point in their life span could have experienced an adaptive advantage, leading perhaps to the selection of genes for later rather than earlier plasticity. If this is so, it implies that variation should exist in terms of when children and even adults are especially developmentally plastic. If this is the case, does it not imply that those who appear not to be especially malleable may simply not be so at the developmental point in time when developmental experiences and environmental exposures are measured? Could it be that some are especially susceptible to contextual regulation early, others later, others at all times, and still others more or less never?

Conclusion

Our ultimate goal in this essay was to provide a theoretical and empirical basis for our claim that there exists individual differences in developmental plasticity and, thus, that phenotypic plasticity should be a subject of study for developmentalists and psychopathologists. To advance this argument, we began by highlighting challenges that evolutionary thinking poses for a science of development and psychopathology, including for the diathesis–stress framework that has (informatively) guided so much empirical inquiry implicitly or explicitly on risk, resilience, and dysregulation. With this foundation, we raised a series of issues that the differential-susceptibility hypothesis calls attention to, while highlighting findings that have emerged over just the past several years and that are pertinent to some of the questions posed. Even though it remains clear that this new perspective is stimulating research and influencing how hypotheses are framed and data interpreted, it should be clear that there are a great many topics in need of empirical attention. Our hope is that this essay will encourage students of development as well as psychopathology to treat phenotypic plasticity as an individual-difference construct while exploring unknowns in the differential-susceptibility equation.

References

Alexander, N., Klucken, T., Koppe, G., Osinsky, R., Walter, B., Vaitl, D., et al. (2012). Interaction of the serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region and environmental adversity: Increased amygdala-hypothalamus connectivity as a potential mechanism linking neural and endocrine hyperreactivity. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 4956. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1997). Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 345368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, E. N., Aron, A., & Jagiellowicz, J. (2012). Sensory-processing sensitivity: A review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 262282. doi:10.1177/1088868311434213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Dobrova-Krol, N. A., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (in press). Impact of institutional care on attachment disorganization and insecurity of Ukrainian pre-schoolers: Protective effect of the long variant of the serotonin transporter gene (5HTT). International Journal of Behavioral Development.Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Steele, H., Zeanah, C. H., Muhamedrahimov, R. J., Vorria, P., Dobrova-Krol, N. A., et al. (2011). Attachment and emotional development in institutional care: Characteristics and catch-up. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, 6291. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00628.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 3952. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000635CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Mesman, J., Alink, L. R., & Juffer, F. (2008). Effects of an attachment-based intervention on daily cortisol moderated by dopamine receptor D4: a randomized control trial on 1- to 3-year-olds screened for externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 805820.Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Pijlman, F. T., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. (2008). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Development and Psychopathology, 44, 293300.Google Scholar
Beach, S. R. H., Brody, G. H., Lei, M.-K., & Philibert, R. A. (2010). Differential susceptibility to parenting among African American youths: Testing the DRD4 hypothesis. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 513521. doi:10.1037/a0020835CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beach, S. R. H., Lei, M. K., Brody, G. H., Simons, R. L., Cutrona, C., & Philibert, R. A. (2012). genetic moderation of contextual effects on negative arousal and parenting in African-American parents. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 4655. doi:10.1037/a0026236Google Scholar
Beaver, K. M., Sak, A., Vaske, J., & Nilsson, J. (2010). Genetic risk, parent–child relations, and antisocial phenotypes in a sample of African-American males. Psychiatry Research, 175, 160164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to rearing influences: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (2000). Conditional and alternative reproductive strategies: Individual differences in susceptibility to rearing experience. In Rodgers, J., Rowe, D., & Miller, W. (Eds.), Genetic influences on human fertility and sexuality: Theoretical and empirical contributions from the biological and behavioral sciences (pp. 127146). Boston: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to rearing influences: An evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In Ellis, B. & Bjorklund, D. (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 139163). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Belsky, J. (2008). War, trauma and children's development: Observations from a modern evolutionary perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 260271. doi:10.1177/0165025408090969CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J. (in press). The development of human reproductive strategies: Progress and prospects. Current Directions in Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., & Beaver, K. M. (2011). Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting and adolescent self-control/regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 619626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., Stanton, M., Brummett, B., & Williams, R. (2009). Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 746754.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009a). Beyond diathesis–stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009b). The nature (and nurture?) of plasticity in early human development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 345351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2013). Genetic moderation of early child-care effects on social functioning across childhood: A developmental analysis. Child Development, 84, 12091225. doi:10.1111/cdev.12058Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Pluess, M., & Widaman, K. F. (2013). Confirmatory and competitive evaluation of alternative gene–environment interaction hypotheses. Manuscript submitted for publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, D., Deater-Deckard, K., McCartney, K., Wang, Z., & Petrill, S. A. (2013). Gene–environment interaction between dopamine receptor D4 7-repeat polymorphism and early maternal sensitivity predicts inattention trajectories across middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 291306.Google Scholar
Bevilacqua, L., Carli, V., Sarchiapone, M., George, D. K., Goldman, D., Roy, A., & Enoch, M.-A. (2012). Interaction between FKBP5 and childhood trauma and risk of aggressive behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 6270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D., & Pellegrini, A. (2002). The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Individual development and evolution: Experiential canalization of self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48, 647657. doi:10.1037/a0026472Google Scholar
Bogdan, R., Williamson, D. E., & Hariri, A. R. (2012). Mineralocorticoid receptor iso/val (rs5522) genotype moderates the association between previous childhood emotional neglect and amygdala reactivity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 515522. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11060855CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyce, W. T., Chesney, M., Alkon, A., Tschann, J. M., Adams, S., Chesterman, B., et al. (1995). Psychobiologic reactivity to stress and childhood respiratory illnesses: Results of two prospective studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 411422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271301.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Beach, S. R. H., Chen, Y.-F., Obasi, E., Philibert, R. A., Kogan, S. M., & Simons, R. L. (2011). Perceived discrimination, serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region status, and the development of conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 617627. doi:10.1017/s0954579411000046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Chen, Y., & Beach, S. R. H. (2013). Differential susceptibility to prevention: GABAergic, dopaminergic and multilocus effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 863871. doi:10.1111/cdev.12058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Chen, Y., Beach, S. R. H., Kogan, S. M., Yu, T., DiClemente, R. J., et al. (in press). Differential sensitivity to prevention programming: A dopaminergic polymorphism-enhanced prevention effect on protective parenting and adolescent substance use. Health Psychology.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., Yu, T., Chen, Y. F., Kogan, S. M., Evans, G. W., Windle, M., et al. (2012). Supportive family environments, genes that confer sensitivity, and allostatic load among rural African American emerging adults: A prospective analysis. Journal of Family Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0027829Google ScholarPubMed
Buss, A., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Buss, K. A. (2011). Which fearful toddlers should we worry about? Context, fear regulation, and anxiety risk. Developmental Psychology, 47, 804819. doi:10.1037/a0023227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, N. M., Champagne, F. A., Parent, C., Fish, E. W., Ozaki-Kuroda, K., & Meaney, M. J. (2005). The programming of individual differences in defensive responses and reproductive strategies in the rat through variations in maternal care. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 843865.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., Joormann, J., Kim, Y., & Nam, J. Y. (2011). Serotonin transporter polymorphism interacts with childhood adversity to predict aspects of impulsivity. Psychological Science, 22, 589595. doi:10.1177/0956797611404085CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851854.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J., Woodhouse, S. S., Sherman, L. J., Stupica, B., & Lejuez, C. W. (2011). Enhancing infant attachment security: An examination of treatment efficacy and differential susceptibility. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 131148. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000696CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champagne, D. L., Bagot, R. C., van Hasselt, F., Ramakers, G., Meaney, M. J., de Kloet, E. R., et al. (2008). Maternal care and hippocampal plasticity: Evidence for experience-dependent structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and differential responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 60376045. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0526-08.2008Google Scholar
Chen, C., Moyzis, R., Stern, H., He, Q., Li, H., Li, J., et al. (2011). Contributions of dopamine-related genes and environmental factors to highly sensitive personality: A multi-step neuronal system-level approach. PLoS ONE, 6(7), e21636. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021636Google Scholar
Chen, J., Li, X., & McGue, M. (2012). Interacting effect of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and stressful life events on adolescent depression. Genes, Brain and Behavior. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00843.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chisholm, J. S. (1993). Death, hope, and sex: Life-history theory and the development of reproductive strategies. Current Anthropology, 34, 124. doi:10.1086/204131Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2012). Gene × Environment interaction and resilience: Effects of child maltreatment and serotonin, corticotropin releasing hormone, dopamine, and oxytocin genes. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 411427. doi:10.1017/s0954579412000077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Oshri, A. (2011). Interactive effects of corticotropin releasing hormone receptor 1, serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region, and child maltreatment on diurnal cortisol regulation and internalizing symptomatology. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 11251138. doi:10.1017/s0954579411000599CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2007). Interactions of child maltreatment and serotonin transporter and monoamine oxidase A polymorphisms: Depressive symptomatology among adolescents from low socioeconomic status backgrounds. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 11611180.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Thibodeau, E. L. (2012). The effects of child maltreatment on early signs of antisocial behavior: Genetic moderation by tryptophan hydroxylase, serotonin transporter, and monoamine oxidase A genes. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 907928.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2011). The effects of child maltreatment and polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter and dopamine D4 receptor genes on infant attachment and intervention efficacy. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 357372. doi:10.1017/s0954579411000113Google Scholar
Clasen, P. C., Wells, T. T., Knopik, V. S., McGeary, J. E., & Beevers, C. G. (2011). 5-HTTLPR and BDNF Val66Met polymorphisms moderate effects of stress on rumination. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 740746. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00715.xGoogle Scholar
Conner, B. T., Hellemann, G. S., Ritchie, T. L., & Noble, E. P. (2010). Genetic, personality, and environmental predictors of drug use in adolescents. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 38, 178190. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2009.07.004Google Scholar
Conway, A., & Stifter, C. A. (2012). Longitudinal antecedents of executive function in preschoolers. Child Development, 83, 10221036. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01756.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. I. (1980). Discriminative parental solicitude: A biological perspective. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42, 277288. doi:10.2307/351225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. I. (1981). Child maltreatment from a sociobiological perspective. New Directions for Child Development, 198, 93112. doi:10.1002/cd.23219811107Google Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (2005). Carpe diem: Adaptation and devaluing the future. Quarterly Review of Biology, 80, 5560. doi:10.1086/431025Google Scholar
Das, D., Cherbuin, N., Tan, X., Anstey, K. J., & Easteal, S. (2011). DRD4-exonIII-VNTR moderates the effect of childhood adversities on emotional resilience in young-adults. PLoS ONE, 6. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020177Google Scholar
Davies, P. T., Sturge-Apple, M. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2011). Interparental aggression and children's adrenocortical reactivity: Testing an evolutionary model of allostatic load. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 801814. doi:10.1017/s0954579411000319Google Scholar
Davis, E. P., Glynn, L. M., Waffarn, F., & Sandman, C. A. (2011). Prenatal maternal stress programs infant stress regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 119129. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02314.xGoogle Scholar
Del Giudice, M. (2009). Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 121. doi:10.1017/s0140525x09000016Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M. (2012). Fetal programming by maternal stress: Insights from a conflict perspective. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 16141629. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.05.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Giudice, M., Angeleri, R., & Manera, V. (2009). The juvenile transition: A developmental switch point in human life history. Developmental Review, 29, 131. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2008.09.001Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). the adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 15621592. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeWitt, T. J., Sih, A., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, 7781. doi:10.1016/s0169-5347(97)01274–3Google Scholar
Dingemanse, N. J., Kazem, A. J., Reale, D., & Wright, J. (2010). Behavioural reaction norms: Animal personality meets individual plasticity. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 25, 8189. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.07.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Docherty, S. J., Kovas, Y., & Plomin, R. (2011). Gene–environment interaction in the etiology of mathematical ability using SNP sets. Behavior Genetics, 41, 141154. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9405-6Google Scholar
Draper, P., & Harpending, H. (1982). Father absence and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Anthropological Research, 38, 255273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drury, S. S., Gleason, M. M., Theall, K. P., Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., et al. (2012). Genetic sensitivity to the caregiving context: The influence of 5HTTLPR and BDNF Val66Met on indiscriminate social behavior. Physiology and Behavior, 106, 728735. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.11.014Google Scholar
Du Rocher Schudlich, T. D., White, C. R., Fleischhauer, E. A., & Fitzgerald, K. A. (2011). Observed infant reactions during live interparental conflict. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73, 221235. doi:10.1111/j.17413737.2010.00800.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eley, T. C., Hudson, J. L., Creswell, C., Tropeano, M., Lester, K. J., Cooper, P., et al. (2012). Therapygenetics: The 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 236241. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.132Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 920958.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2012). Beyond mental health: An evolutionary analysis of development under risky and supportive environmental conditions: An introduction to the special section. Developmental Psychology, 48, 591597. doi:10.1037/a0027651Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: toward an understanding of sensitivity to developmental experiences and context. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 15. doi:10.1017/s095457941000060xGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 728. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000611Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Del Giudice, M., Dishion, T. J., Figueredo, A. J., Gray, P., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2012). The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: implications for science, policy, and practice. Developmental Psychology, 48, 598623. doi:10.1037/a0026220CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Del Giudice, M., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (in press). Beyond allostatic load: The stress response system as a mechanism of conditional adaptation. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (2 ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., & Garber, J. (2000). Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls’ pubertal timing: Maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Development, 71, 485501. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00159Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Shirtcliff, E. A., Boyce, W. T., Deardorff, J., & Essex, M. J. (2011). Quality of early family relationships and the timing and tempo of puberty: Effects depend on biological sensitivity to context. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 8599. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000660CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enoch, M. A., Steer, C. D., Newman, T. K., Gibson, N., & Goldman, D. (2010). Early life stress, MAOA, and gene–environment interactions predict behavioral disinhibition in children. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 9, 6574. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00535.xGoogle Scholar
Essex, M. J., Armstrong, J. M., Burk, L. R., Goldsmith, H. H., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Biological sensitivity to context moderates the effects of the early teacher–child relationship on the development of mental health by adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 149161. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000702CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2012). Childhood poverty and young adults’ allostatic load: The mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychological Science. doi:10.1177/0956797612441218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figueredo, A. J., & Jacobs, W. J. (2010). Aggression, risk-taking, and alternative life history strategies. In Frias-Armenta, M. & Corral-Verdugo, V. (Eds.), Bio-psycho-social perspectives on interpersonal violence (pp. 328). Hauppauge, NY: NOVA Science.Google Scholar
Fortuna, K., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Mankuta, D., Kaitz, M., Avinun, R., Ebstein, R. P., et al. (2011). Differential genetic susceptibility to child risk at birth in predicting observed maternal behavior. PLoS ONE, 6. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019765Google Scholar
Fox, E., Zougkou, K., Ridgewell, A., & Garner, K. (2011). The serotonin transporter gene alters sensitivity to attention bias modification: Evidence for a plasticity gene. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 10491054. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Del Giudice, M. (2012). When do adaptive developmental mechanisms yield maladaptive outcomes? Developmental Psychology, 48, 628642. doi:10.1037/a0025629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Panchanathan, K. (2011). Individual differences in developmental plasticity may result from stochastic sampling. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 336347. doi:10.1177/1745691611412602Google Scholar
Fuller-Rowell, T. E., Evans, G. W., & Ong, A. D. (2012). Poverty and health: The mediating role of perceived discrimination. Psychological Science, 23, 734739. doi:10.1177/0956797612439720Google Scholar
Ganzel, B. L., & Morris, P. A. (2011). Allostasis and the developing human brain: Explicit consideration of implicit models. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 955974. doi:10.1017/s0954579411000447Google Scholar
Garmezy, N. (1974). The study of competence in children at risk for severe psychopathology. In Anthony, E. J. & Koupernik, C. (Eds.), The child in his family: Children at psychiatric risk (Vol. 3, pp. 7797). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 97111. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00276.xGoogle Scholar
Gatt, J. M., Nemeroff, C. B., Dobson-Stone, C., Paul, R. H., Bryant, R. A., Schofield, P. R., et al. (2009). Interactions between BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and early life stress predict brain and arousal pathways to syndromal depression and anxiety. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 681695.Google Scholar
Gibbons, F. X., Roberts, M. E., Gerrard, M., Li, Z., Beach, S. R. H., Simons, R. L., et al. (2012). The impact of stress on the life history strategies of African American adolescents: Cognitions, genetic moderation, and the role of discrimination. Developmental Psychology, 48, 722739. doi:10.1037/a0026599Google Scholar
Glynn, L. M., & Sandman, C. A. (2011). Prenatal origins of neurological development: A Critical period for fetus and mother. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 384389. doi:10.1177/0963721411422056Google Scholar
Gottesman, I., & Shields, J. (1967). A polygenic theory of schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 58, 199205.Google Scholar
Grazioplene, R. G., DeYoung, C. G., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2013). A novel differential susceptibility gene: CHRNA4 and moderation of the effect of maltreatment on child personality. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 872880. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Wenner, J. A., Thomas, K. M., Glatt, C. E., McKenna, M. C., & Clark, A. G. (2012). The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism moderates early deprivation effects on attention problems. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 12151223. doi:10.1017/s095457941200065xGoogle Scholar
Hankin, B. L., Nederhof, E., Oppenheimer, C. W., Jenness, J., Young, J. F., Abela, J. R. Z., et al. (2011). Differential susceptibility in youth: Evidence that 5-HTTLPR × Positive Parenting is associated with positive affect “for better and worse.” Translational Psychiatry, 1, e44. doi:10.1038/tp.2011.44Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1990). Attachment: Biological, cultural and individual desiderata. Human Development, 33, 6272.Google Scholar
Holmboe, K., Nemoda, Z., Fearon, R. M., Sasvari-Szekely, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Dopamine D4 receptor and serotonin transporter gene effects on the longitudinal development of infant temperament. Genes, Brain and Behavior. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00669.xGoogle Scholar
Hyde, L. W., Bogdan, R., & Hariri, A. R. (2011). Understanding risk for psychopathology through imaging gene–environment interactions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 417427. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.001Google Scholar
Ivorra, J. L., Sanjuan, J., Jover, M., Carot, J. M., Frutos, R., & Molto, M. D. (2010). Gene–environment interaction of child temperament. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 545554. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ee4072Google Scholar
Jacobs, R. H., Pine, D. S., Schoeny, M. E., Henry, D. B., Gollan, J. K., Moy, G., et al. (2011). Maternal depressive history, teen 5HTTLPR genotype, and the processing of emotional faces: Exploring mechanisms of risk. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 8084. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.10.004Google Scholar
James, J., Ellis, B. J., Schlomer, G. L., & Garber, J. (2012). Sex-specific pathways to early puberty, sexual debut, and sexual risk taking: Tests of an integrated evolutionary–developmental model. Developmental Psychology, 48, 687702. doi:10.1037/a0026427Google Scholar
Jessee, A., Mangelsdorf, S. C., Brown, G. L., Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., Shigeto, A., & Wong, M. S. (2010). Parents’ differential susceptibility to the effects of marital quality on sensitivity across the first year. Infant Behavior & Development, 33, 442452.Google Scholar
Johansson, A., Bergman, H., Corander, J., Waldman, I. D., Karrani, N., Salo, B., et al. (2012). Alcohol and aggressive behavior in men-moderating effects of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphisms. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 11, 214221. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00744.xGoogle Scholar
Juhasz, G., Dunham, J. S., McKie, S., Thomas, E., Downey, D., Chase, D., et al. (2011). The CREB1-BDNF-NTRK2 pathway in depression: Multiple gene–cognition–environment interactions. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 762771. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.019Google Scholar
Kaplan, H. S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2005). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 6895). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kegel, C. A. T., Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility in early literacy instruction through computer games: The role of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4). Mind, Brain, and Education, 5, 7178. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01112.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent–child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83, 12751289. doi:10.1111/j.1467–8624.2012.01778.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klucken, T., Wehrum, S., Schweckendiek, J., Merz, C. J., Hennig, J., Vaitl, D., et al. (2013). The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism is associated with altered hemodynamic responses during appetitive conditioning. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 25492560. doi:10.1002/hbm.22085Google Scholar
Knafo, A., Israel, S., & Ebstein, R. P. (2011). Heritability of children's prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 5367. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000647Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2013). Difficult temperament moderates links between maternal responsiveness and children's compliance and behavior problems in low-income families. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 323332. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12002Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., Barry, R. A., & Philibert, R. A. (2011). Children's genotypes interact with maternal responsive care in predicting children's competence: Diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility? Development and Psychopathology, 23, 605616.Google Scholar
Korte, S. M., Koolhaas, J. M., Wingfield, J. C., & McEwen, B. S. (2005). The Darwinian concept of stress: Benefits of allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 338. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.08.009Google Scholar
Kuepper, Y., Wielpuetz, C., Alexander, N., Mueller, E., Grant, P., & Hennig, J. (2012). 5-HTTLPR S-allele: A genetic plasticity factor regarding the effects of life events on personality? Genes, Brain and Behavior. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00783.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuzawa, C. W. (2005). Fetal origins of developmental plasticity: Are fetal cues reliable predictors of future nutritional environments? American Journal of Human Biology, 17, 521. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20091Google Scholar
Laucht, M., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A. F., Treutlein, J., Schmidt, M. H., Esser, G., et al. (2012). Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype, parenting practices and adolescent alcohol use: Testing the differential susceptibility hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 351359. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02408.xGoogle Scholar
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, 543562.Google Scholar
Manuck, S. B. (2011). Delay discounting covaries with childhood socioeconomic status as a function of genetic variation in the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4). Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2007). Resilience in developing systems: Progress and promise as the fourth wave rises. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 921930. doi:10.1017/s0954579407000442Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Best, K., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 425444.Google Scholar
Mata, J., Thompson, R. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). BDNF genotype moderates the relation between physical activity and depressive symptoms. Health Psychology, 29, 130133. doi:10.1037/a0017261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathot, K. J., van den Hout, P. J., Piersma, T., Kempenaers, B., Reale, D., & Dingemanse, N. J. (2011). Disentangling the roles of frequency- vs. state-dependence in generating individual differences in behavioural plasticity. Ecology Letters, 14, 12541262. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01698.xGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, C., Notterman, D., Brooks-Gunn, J., Hobcraft, J., Garfinkel, I., Jaeger, K., et al. (2011). Role of mother's genes and environment in postpartum depression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 81898193. doi:10.1073/pnas.1014129108Google Scholar
Monroe, S. M., & Simons, A. D. (1991). Diathesis–stress theories in the context of life stress research: Implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 406425.Google Scholar
Mulvihill, D. (2005). The health impact of childhood trauma: An interdisciplinary review, 1997–2003. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 28, 115136. doi:10.1080/01460860590950890Google Scholar
Nederhof, E., & Schmidt, M. V. (2012). Mismatch or cumulative stress: Toward an integrated hypothesis of programming effects. Physiology & Behavior, 106, 691700. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.008Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2010). Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 387395. doi:10.1093/beheco/arp202Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2011). Flexibility in reproductive timing in human females: integrating ultimate and proximate explanations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 366B, 357365. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0073Google Scholar
Nijmeijer, J. S., Hartman, C. A., Rommelse, N. N. J., Altink, M. E., Buschgens, C. J. M., Fliers, E. A., et al. (2010). Perinatal risk factors interacting with catechol-O-methyltransferase and the serotonin transporter gene predict ASD symptoms in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 12421250. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02277.xGoogle Scholar
Nikolova, Y. S., Bogdan, R., Brigidi, B. D., & Hariri, A. R. (2012). Ventral striatum reactivity to reward and recent life stress interact to predict positive affect. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 157163. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.014Google Scholar
Nikolova, Y. S., Ferrell, R. E., Manuck, S. B., & Hariri, A. R. (2011). Multilocus genetic profile for dopamine signaling predicts ventral striatum reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 19401947. doi:10.1038/npp.2011.82Google Scholar
Obradovic, J., Bush, N. R., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). The interactive effect of marital conflict and stress reactivity on externalizing and internalizing symptoms: The role of laboratory stressors. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 101114. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000672Google Scholar
Obradovic, J., Bush, N. R., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N. E., & Boyce, W. T. (2010). Biological sensitivity to context: The interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on socio-emotional behavior and school readiness. Child Development, 81, 270289.Google Scholar
Park, A., Sher, K. J., Todorov, A. A., & Heath, A. C. (2011). Interaction between the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism and proximal and distal environments in alcohol dependence during emerging and young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 585595. doi:10.1037/a0022648Google Scholar
Pergamin-Hight, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2012). Variations in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene and biased attention for emotional information: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 71, 373379. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitzer, M., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Esser, G., Schmidt, M. H., & Laucht, M. (2011). Differential susceptibility to environmental influences: The role of early temperament and parenting in the development of externalizing problems. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 52, 650658. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.10.017Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 116.Google Scholar
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2011). Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity? Development and Psychopathology, 23, 2938.Google Scholar
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2013). Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 901916. doi:10.1037/a0030196CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pluess, M., Belsky, J., Way, B. M., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). 5-HTTLPR moderates effects of life events on neuroticism: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 34, 10701074.Google Scholar
Pluess, M., Stevens, S., & Belsky, J. (2013). Differential susceptibility: Developmental and evolutionary mechanisms of gene and environment interactions. In Legerstee, M., Haley, D. W., & Bornstein, M. H. (Eds.), The infant mind: Origins of the social brain. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pluess, M., Velders, F. P., Belsky, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Jaddoe, V. W., et al. (2011). Serotonin transporter polymorphism moderates effects of prenatal maternal anxiety on infant negative emotionality. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 520525. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.006Google Scholar
Poehlmann, J., Hane, A., Burnson, C., Maleck, S., Hamburger, E., & Shah, P. E. (2012). Preterm infants who are prone to distress: Differential effects of parenting on 36-month behavioral and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02564.xGoogle Scholar
Poulin, M. J., Holman, E. A., & Buffone, A. (2012). The neurogenetics of nice: Receptor genes for oxytocin and vasopressin interact with threat to predict prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 23, 446452.Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regressions, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437448.Google Scholar
Quinlan, R. J. (2007). Human parental effort and environmental risk. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 274B, 121125. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3690Google Scholar
Raver, C. C., Blair, C., & Willoughby, M. (2012). Poverty as a predictor of 4-year-olds’ executive function: New perspectives on models of differential susceptibility. Developmental Psychology, 49, 292. doi:10.1037/a0028343Google Scholar
Rhee, S. H., Corley, R. P., Friedman, N. P., Hewitt, J. K., Hink, L. K., Johnson, D. P., et al. (2012). The etiology of observed negative emotionality from 14 to 24 months. Frontiers in Genetics, 3, 9. doi:10.3389/fgene.2012.00009Google Scholar
Roiser, J. P., Rogers, R. D., Cook, L. J., & Sahakian, B. J. (2006). The effect of polymorphism at the serotonin transporter gene on decision-making, memory and executive function in ecstasy users and controls. Psychopharmacology, 188, 213227. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0495-zGoogle Scholar
Roisman, G. I., Newman, D. A., Fraley, R. C., Haltigan, J. D., Groh, A. M., & Haydon, K. C. (2012). Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: Recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 389409. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000065Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children's responses to stress and disadvantage. In Kent, M. W. & Rolf, J. E. (Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathology (Vol. 3, pp. 4974). Hanover, NH: University Press of New England.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 316331.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 335344. doi:10.1017/s0954579412000028Google Scholar
Schlichting, C. D., & Pigliucci, M. (1998). Phenotypic evolution: A reaction norm perspective. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Schlomer, G. L., Del Giudice, M., & Ellis, B. J. (2011). Parent–offspring conflict theory: An evolutionary framework for understanding conflict within human families. Psychological Review, 118, 496521. doi:10.1037/a0024043Google Scholar
Schoebi, D., Way, B. M., Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (2012). Genetic moderation of sensitivity to positive and negative affect in marriage. Emotion, 12, 208212. doi:10.1037/a0026067Google Scholar
Scott, S., & O'Connor, T. G. (2012). An experimental test of differential susceptibility to parenting among emotionally dysregulated children in a randomized controlled trial for oppositional behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. doi:10.1111/j.14697610.2012.02586.xGoogle Scholar
Sergerie, K., Chochol, C., & Armony, J. L. (2008). The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 811830.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., & McEwen, B. S. (2009). Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301, 22522259.Google Scholar
Sih, A. (2011). Effects of early stress on behavioral syndromes: An integrated adaptive perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 14521465. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.015Google Scholar
Simons, R. L., Lei, M. K., Beach, S. R., Brody, G. H., Philibert, R. A., & Gibbons, F. X. (2011). Social environmental variation, plasticity genes, and aggression: Evidence for the differential susceptibility hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 76, 833912. doi:10.1177/0003122411427580Google Scholar
Simons, R. L., Lei, M. K., Stewart, E. A., Beach, S. R. H., Brody, G. H., Philibert, R. A., et al. (2012). Social adversity, genetic variation, street code, and aggression: A Genetically informed model of violent behavior. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 10, 324. doi:10.1177/1541204011422087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Oades, R. D., Psychogiou, L., Chen, W., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J., et al. (2009). Dopamine and serotonin transporter genotypes moderate sensitivity to maternal expressed emotion: The case of conduct and emotional problems in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 10521063.Google Scholar
Stearns, S. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Cicchetti, D., Davies, P. T., & Suor, J. H. (2012). Differential susceptibility in spillover between interparental conflict and maternal parenting practices: Evidence for OXTR and 5-HTT genes. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 431442. doi:10.1037/a0028302Google Scholar
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Martin, M. J., Cicchetti, D., & Hentges, R. F. (2012). An examination of the impact of harsh parenting contexts on children's adaptation within an evolutionary framework. Developmental Psychology, 48, 791805. doi:10.1037/a0026908Google Scholar
Sulik, M. J., Eisenberg, N., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Spinrad, T. L., Silva, K. M., Eggum, N. D., et al. (2012). Interactions between serotonin transporter gene haplotypes and quality of mothers’ parenting predict the development of children's noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 48, 740754. doi:10.1037/a0025938Google Scholar
Suzuki, A., Matsumoto, Y., Shibuya, N., Sadahiro, R., Kamata, M., Goto, K., et al. (2011). The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism modulates the effects of parental rearing on personality traits in healthy subjects. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 385391. doi:10.1111/j.1601–183X.2010.00673.xGoogle Scholar
Sweitzer, M. M., Halder, I., Flory, J. D., Craig, A. E., Gianaros, P. J., Ferrell, R. E., et al. (2013). Polymorphic variation in the dopamine D4 receptor predicts delay discounting as a function of childhood socioeconomic status: Evidence for differential susceptibility. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 499508. doi:10.1093/scan/nss020Google Scholar
Takane, Y., & Cramer, E. M. (1975). Regions of significance in multiple-regression analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 10, 373383. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr1003_10Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent–offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249264.Google Scholar
van der Kooy-Hofland, V. A. C., van der Kooy, J., Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bonsel, G. J. (2012). Differential susceptibility to early literacy intervention in children with mild perinatal adversities: Short- and long-term effects of a randomized control trial. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 337349. doi:10.1037/a0026984Google Scholar
van de Wiel, N. M., van Goozen, S. H., Matthys, W., Snoek, H., & van Engeland, H. (2004). Cortisol and treatment effect in children with disruptive behavior disorders: A preliminary study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 10111018.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2012). Differential susceptibility experiments: Going beyond correlational evidence: Comment on beyond mental health, differential susceptibility articles. Developmental Psychology, 48, 769774. doi:10.1037/a0027536Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Belsky, J., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2012). Serotonin transporter genotype 5HTTLPR as a marker of differential susceptibility? A meta-analysis of child and adolescent gene-by-environment studies. Translational Psychiatry, 2, e147. doi:10.1038/tp.2012.73Google Scholar
Verschoor, E., & Markus, C. R. (2011). Affective and neuroendocrine stress reactivity to an academic examination: Influence of the 5-HTTLPR genotype and trait neuroticism. Biological Psychology, 87, 439449. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.06.001Google Scholar
Vinberg, M., Trajkovska, V., Bennike, B., Knorr, U., Knudsen, G. M., & Kessing, L. V. (2009). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism: Relation to familiar risk of affective disorder, BDNF levels and salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 13801389.Google Scholar
Wakschlag, L. S., Kistner, E. O., Pine, D. S., Biesecker, G., Pickett, K. E., Skol, A. D., et al. (2010). Interaction of prenatal exposure to cigarettes and MAOA genotype in pathways to youth antisocial behavior. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 928937. doi:10.1038/mp.2009.22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Way, B. M., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). The serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism is associated with cortisol response to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 487492.Google Scholar
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. (1977). Kauai's children come of age. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible: A study of resilient children. New York: McGraw–Hill.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whittle, S., Yap, M. B. H., Sheeber, L., Dudgeon, P., Yuecel, M., Pantelis, C., et al. (2011). Hippocampal volume and sensitivity to maternal aggressive behavior: A prospective study of adolescent depressive symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 115129. doi:10.1017/s0954579410000684Google Scholar
Widaman, K. F., Helm, J. L., Castro-Schilo, L., Pluess, M., Stallings, M. C., & Belsky, J. (2012). Distinguishing ordinal and disordinal interactions. Psychological Methods, 17, 615622.Google Scholar
Xie, P., Kranzler, H. R., Poling, J., Stein, M. B., Anton, R. F., Farrer, L. A., et al. (2010). Interaction of FKBP5 with childhood adversity on risk for post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 16841692. doi:10.1038/npp.2010.37Google Scholar
Yaman, A., Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2010). Parenting and toddler aggression in second-generation immigrant families: The moderating role of child temperament. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 208211. doi:10.1037/a0019100Google Scholar
Yap, M. B., Whittle, S., Yucel, M., Sheeber, L., Pantelis, C., Simmons, J. G., et al. (2008). Interaction of parenting experiences and brain structure in the prediction of depressive symptoms in adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 13771385. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1377Google Scholar
Zimmermann, P., Brueckl, T., Nocon, A., Pfister, H., Binder, E. B., Uhr, M., et al. (2011). Interaction of FKBP5 gene variants and adverse life events in predicting depression onset: Results from a 10-year prospective community study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 11071116. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111577Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1999). Vulnerability to psychopathology: A biosocial model. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar