Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T07:35:09.964Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control in maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 December 2020

Jacqueline Bruce*
Affiliation:
Oregon Social Learning Center, Eugene, OR, USA
Hyoun K. Kim
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Family Studies, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
*
Author for Correspondence: Dr. Jacqueline Bruce, Oregon Social Learning Center, 10 Shelton McMurphey Boulevard, Eugene, OR97401; E-mail: jackieb@oslc.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Early adverse experiences are believed to have a profound effect on inhibitory control and the underlying neural regions. In the current study, behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) data were collected during a go/no-go task from adolescents who were involved with the child welfare system due to child maltreatment (n = 129) and low-income, nonmaltreated adolescents (n = 102). The nonmaltreated adolescents were more accurate than the maltreated adolescents on the go/no-go task, particularly on the no-go trials. Paralleling the results with typically developing populations, the nonmaltreated adolescents displayed a more pronounced amplitude of the N2 during the no-go trials than during the go trials. However, the maltreated adolescents demonstrated a more pronounced amplitude of the N2 during the go trials than during the no-go trials. Furthermore, while the groups did not differ during the go trials, the nonmaltreated adolescents displayed a more negative amplitude of the N2 than the maltreated adolescents during no-go trials. In contrast, there was not a significant group difference in amplitude of the P3. Taken together, these results provide evidence that the early adverse experiences encountered by maltreated populations impact inhibitory control and the underlying neural activity in early adolescence.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

In 2017, approximately 674,000 children and adolescents were determined to be the victims of neglect and abuse in the United States (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2019). Furthermore, it has been estimated that 37% of the children and adolescents in the United States will experience at least one child welfare system investigation due to child maltreatment before the age of 18 (Kim, Wildeman, Jonson-Reid, & Drake, Reference Kim, Wildeman, Jonson-Reid and Drake2017). Children and adolescents involved with the child welfare system have typically been exposed to a host of early adverse experiences, including prenatal alcohol and substance exposure; physical, sexual, and emotional abuse; physical and supervisory neglect; and repeated caregiver transitions. As a consequence, these children and adolescents are at increased risk for a multitude of negative outcomes, including academic difficulties, attention and behavior problems, and alcohol and substance use (Aarons, Brown, Hough, Garland, & Wood, Reference Aarons, Brown, Hough, Garland and Wood2001; Clausen, Landsverk, Ganger, Chadwick, & Litrownik, Reference Clausen, Landsverk, Ganger, Chadwick and Litrownik1998; Crozier & Barth, Reference Crozier and Barth2005; Keller, Salazar, & Courtney, Reference Keller, Salazar and Courtney2010; Pilowsky & Wu, Reference Pilowsky and Wu2006; Zima et al., Reference Zima, Bussing, Freeman, Yang, Belin and Forness2000). It has been theorized that the negative outcomes observed among populations who have been exposed to early adverse experiences, at least partially, result from experience-induced alterations in specific cognitive abilities and the underlying neural regions (De Bellis, Reference De Bellis2001; Fishbein, Reference Fishbein2000; Gunnar Fisher, & The Early Experience Stress and Prevention Network, Reference Gunnar and Fisher2006). For example, it has been speculated that experience-induced alterations in inhibitory control and the underlying neural regions contribute to some of the difficulties observed among populations exposed to early adverse experiences (Black, Reference Black1998; De Bellis, Reference De Bellis2001; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2014). Therefore, the current study was designed to investigate behavioral and electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control in maltreated adolescents and low-income, nonmaltreated adolescents in early adolescence. The inclusion of a socioeconomically matched sample of nonmaltreated adolescents ensured that any observed group differences were due to child maltreatment rather than poverty, which is a significant risk factor for child maltreatment.

Inhibitory control and the underlying neural activity

Inhibitory control is a higher order cognitive ability that involves the capacity to voluntarily inhibit prepotent behavioral responses and guide appropriate behaviors through the suppression of competing, irrelevant behaviors (Casey, Tottenham, & Fossella, Reference Casey, Tottenham and Fossella2002; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Thomas, Yang, Ulug, Zimmerman and Casey2002). The results from neuroimaging studies indicate that specific regions of the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, striatum, subthalamic nucleus, and motor cortex underlie this cognitive ability (Aron, Behrens, Smith, Frank, & Poldrack, Reference Aron, Behrens, Smith, Frank and Poldrack2007; Booth et al., Reference Booth, Burman, Meyer, Lei, Trommer, Davenport and Mesulam2005; Bunge, Dudukovic, Thomason, Vaidya, & Gabrieli, Reference Bunge, Dudukovic, Thomason, Vaidya and Gabrieli2002; Casey, Trainor, et al., Reference Casey, Trainor, Orendi, Schubert, Nystrom, Giedd and Rapoport1997; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Thomas, Yang, Ulug, Zimmerman and Casey2002; Liddle, Kiehl, & Smith, Reference Liddle, Kiehl and Smith2001). There is extensive evidence to suggest that inhibitory control and the underlying neural regions have a protracted developmental course that begins in early childhood and continues into emerging adulthood (Casey, Trainor, et al., Reference Casey, Trainor, Orendi, Schubert, Nystrom, Giedd and Rapoport1997; Davis, Bruce, Snyder, & Nelson, Reference Davis, Bruce, Snyder and Nelson2003; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Davidson, Tottenham, Galvan, Spicer, Fossella and Casey2006; Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd, Lusk, Hayashi, Greenstein, Vaituzis and Thompson2004; Rubia et al., Reference Rubia, Smith, Woolley, Nosarti, Heyman, Taylor and Brammer2006; Sowell et al., Reference Sowell, Thompson, Leonard, Welcome, Kan and Toga2004; Thatcher, Walker, & Giudice, Reference Thatcher, Walker and Giudice1987; Troller-Renfree et al., Reference Troller-Renfree, Buzzell, Bowers, Salo, Forman-Alberti, Smith and Fox2019). Superimposed on this general developmental trend, there are significant individual differences in inhibitory control that appear to be relatively stable across development (Eigsti et al., Reference Eigsti, Zayas, Mischel, Shoda, Ayduk, Dadlani and Casey2006; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, Reference Kochanska, Murray and Harlan2000). Importantly, individual differences in inhibitory control have been shown to be associated with a number of outcomes, such as academic difficulties, symptoms of social anxiety, attention and behavior problems, and alcohol and substance use (Blair & Razza, Reference Blair and Razza2007; Casey, Castellanos, et al., Reference Casey, Castellanos, Giedd, Marsh, Hamburger, Schubert and Rapoport1997; McClelland et al., Reference McClelland, Cameron, Connor, Farris, Jewkes and Morrison2007; Pears, Capaldi, & Owen, Reference Pears, Capaldi and Owen2007; Toupin, Déry, Pauzé, Mercier, & Fortin, Reference Toupin, Déry, Pauzé, Mercier and Fortin2000; Troller-Renfree et al., Reference Troller-Renfree, Buzzell, Bowers, Salo, Forman-Alberti, Smith and Fox2019; Wills & Stoolmiller, Reference Wills and Stoolmiller2002).

One of the more commonly used methods for assessing inhibitory control is the go/no-go task, during which participants inhibit prepotent behavioral responses by selectively responding to target stimuli (go trials) and inhibiting responses to infrequent nontarget stimuli (no-go trials). Not surprisingly, children, adolescents, and adults are less accurate on the no-go trials that require inhibitory control than the go trials that do not require inhibitory control (Casey, Trainor, et al., Reference Casey, Trainor, Orendi, Schubert, Nystrom, Giedd and Rapoport1997; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Bruce, Snyder and Nelson2003; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Davidson, Tottenham, Galvan, Spicer, Fossella and Casey2006). In addition to a behavioral index of inhibitory control (i.e., accuracy on the no-go trials), electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control (i.e., event-related potential [ERP] data during the no-go trials) also can be assessed during the go/no-go task. In contrast to behavioral data that reflect the final output from the confluence of multiple cognitive abilities, ERP data have excellent temporal resolution (in milliseconds) and provide information about the temporal sequence of specific cognitive abilities (Luck, Reference Luck2005). Much of the electrophysiological research employing the go/no-go task has focused on two stimulus-locked ERP components, the N2 and P3. The N2 is a frontocentral negative deflection that typically peaks approximately 250–400 ms after the presentation of the stimulus and is more pronounced (i.e., more negative amplitude) during the no-go trials than during the go trials. Although it is widely recognized that the N2 is associated with inhibitory control in general, the specific cognitive ability is still debated. For example, some researchers argue that it reflects inhibition of a planned response (Folstein & Van Petten, Reference Folstein and Van Petten2008) and other researchers argue that it reflects monitoring for response conflict (Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, Van Den Wildenberg, & Ridderinkhof, Reference Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, Van Den Wildenberg and Ridderinkhof2003). The results from source localization studies suggest that the N2 is most likely generated in the ventral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex (Lamm, Zelazo, & Lewis, Reference Lamm, Zelazo and Lewis2006; Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, Van Den Wildenberg and Ridderinkhof2003). The P3 is a centroparietal positive deflection that typically peaks approximately 300–400 ms after the presentation of the stimulus and is more pronounced (i.e., more positive amplitude) in response to less frequent and/or more salient stimuli such as the no-go trials. It is believed to reflect response potentiation following stimulus evaluation (Nieuwenhuis, Aston-Jones, & Cohen, Reference Nieuwenhuis, Aston-Jones and Cohen2005). Evidence suggest that the P3 is most likely generated in the temporal–parietal junction and lateral prefrontal cortex (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Aston-Jones and Cohen2005).

Impact of early adverse experiences on inhibitory control and underlying neural activity

Because the neural regions underlying inhibitory control have a protracted developmental course and extensive bidirectional connections with the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) system and other systems involved in the response to stress (Arnsten, Reference Arnsten2009; Ghashghaei & Barbas, Reference Ghashghaei and Barbas2002; Herman, Ostrander, Mueller, & Figueiredo, Reference Herman, Ostrander, Mueller and Figueiredo2005; Sullivan & Gratton, Reference Sullivan and Gratton2002), early adverse experiences are believed to have a profound influence on the development of inhibitory control and the underlying neural regions (Black, Reference Black1998; De Bellis, Reference De Bellis2001; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2014). There is evidence that early adverse experiences, such as parental deprivation, reduce neuronal spine density and length in the prefrontal cortex in rodents (Helmeke et al., Reference Helmeke, Seidel, Poeggel, Bredy, Abraham and Braun2009; Holmes & Wellman, Reference Holmes and Wellman2009). Similarly, impaired behavioral performance on inhibitory control tasks has been observed among maltreated children in foster care and children adopted from deprived institutions (Bruce, Tarullo, & Gunnar, Reference Bruce, Tarullo and Gunnar2009; Lewis, Dozier, Ackerman, & Sepulveda-Kozakowski, Reference Lewis, Dozier, Ackerman and Sepulveda-Kozakowski2007; Pears, Bruce, Fisher, & Kim, Reference Pears, Bruce, Fisher and Kim2010; Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Nelson, Schlaak, Roeber, Wewerka, Wiik and Gunnar2010). Findings from neuroimaging studies reveal that populations exposed to early adverse experiences also demonstrate atypical patterns of neural activation during inhibitory control tasks (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Fisher, Graham, Moore, Peake and Mannering2013; Mueller et al., Reference Mueller, Maheu, Dozier, Peloso, Mandell, Leibenluft and Ernst2010; Sheinkopf et al., Reference Sheinkopf, Lester, Sanes, Eliassen, Hutchison, Seifer and Casey2009; Smith, Fried, Hogan, & Cameron, Reference Smith, Fried, Hogan and Cameron2004). To date, electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control have not been examined in a maltreated population. However, children who were raised in deprived institutions and children who were prenatally exposed to alcohol have been shown to display less pronounced amplitudes of the N2 and/or P3 than their peers during the go/no-go task (Burden et al., Reference Burden, Andrew, Saint-Amour, Meintjes, Molteno, Hoyme and Jacobson2009; Loman et al., Reference Loman, Johnson, Westerlund, Pollak, Nelson and Gunnar2013; McDermott, Westerlund, Zeanah, Nelson, & Fox, Reference McDermott, Westerlund, Zeanah, Nelson and Fox2012), suggesting that early adverse experiences affect these electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control.

Objectives and hypotheses of the current study

The objective of the current study was to examine behavioral and electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control during the go/no-go task in maltreated adolescents and low-income, nonmaltreated adolescents. It is believed that the early adverse experiences encountered by maltreated populations result in acute and/or chronic activation of the HPA system and other systems involved in the response to stress, which in turn impairs the development and subsequent functioning of critical neural regions (Gunnar & Quevedo, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2007). Furthermore, it has been theorized that the neural regions underlying inhibitory control, particularly the prefrontal cortex, may be especially vulnerable to the effects of early adverse experiences because these neural regions have a protracted developmental course and a high density of glucocorticoid (hormone produced by the HPA system) receptors (Black, Reference Black1998; De Bellis, Reference De Bellis2001; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2014). Consistent with this theory and with prior results with maltreated children (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Dozier, Ackerman and Sepulveda-Kozakowski2007; Pears et al., Reference Pears, Bruce, Fisher and Kim2010), it was hypothesized that the maltreated adolescents would demonstrate poorer behavioral performance during the go/no-go task than the nonmaltreated adolescents. Specifically, the maltreated adolescents were expected to be less accurate on the trials that require inhibitory control (i.e., no-go trials). Based on previous studies with other populations exposed to early adverse experiences (Burden et al., Reference Burden, Andrew, Saint-Amour, Meintjes, Molteno, Hoyme and Jacobson2009; Loman et al., Reference Loman, Johnson, Westerlund, Pollak, Nelson and Gunnar2013; McDermott et al., Reference McDermott, Westerlund, Zeanah, Nelson and Fox2012), it also was hypothesized that the maltreated adolescents would demonstrate atypical electrophysiological performance during the go/no-go task compared to the nonmaltreated adolescents. More precisely, the maltreated adolescents were expected to display less pronounced amplitudes of the N2 and P3 during the trials that require inhibitory control. Because the current study was the first study to examine the electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control with maltreated adolescents, the results of this study provide unique insight into the effect of the early adverse experiences typically encountered by a maltreated population on the specific cognitive abilities supporting inhibitory control.

Method

Participants

The sample in the current study included two groups of 12- to 13-year-olds: adolescents who were involved with the child welfare system due to child maltreatment (n = 129) and low-income, nonmaltreated adolescents (n = 102). To recruit the maltreated adolescents, Oregon Department of Human Services child welfare system staff provided monthly lists of all of the 12- to 13-year-olds who were victims of recent reports of neglect and/or abuse and who were living with at least one biological parent in one of seven counties. The nonmaltreated adolescents were recruited via postcards mailed to the parents of students at local middle schools. The exclusion criteria for both groups were: (a) parent or adolescent was not fluent in English and (b) parent or adolescent could not complete the assessment procedures due to a severe developmental or physical disorder. In addition, the exclusion criteria for the nonmaltreated group were: (a) family had been involved with the child welfare system (verified by parent report and child welfare system records) and (b) adolescent had not consistently lived with at least one biological parent. To ensure that group differences were not attributable to socioeconomic status, the nonmaltreated group also was required to have an annual household income equal to or less than 185% of the poverty level (i.e., cutoff to qualify for reduced price school meals via the National School Lunch Program) and a parent education equal to or less than a 4-year college degree.

Descriptive information about the adolescents is presented by group in Table 1. The maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents did not significantly differ on age, gender, or race/ethnicity, F(1, 229) = 2.15, p = .114, Pearson χ2(2, N = 231) = 1.76, p = .416, and Pearson χ2(1, N = 231) = 2.89, p = .089, respectively. Similarly, the groups did not differ on annual household income or parent education, F(1, 228) = 3.34, p = .069, and F(1, 229) = 0.04, p = .852, respectively. The mean annual household income for both groups corresponded to $20,000–$29,000 per year, and the mean parent education for both groups corresponded to some postsecondary education but did not earn a degree or certificate. To provide an estimate of general intellectual ability, the adolescents completed the matrix reasoning subtest and vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence – Second Edition (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2011). T scores from these subtests are summed to create a Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) with a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. Although both groups performed within the average range, the maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents significantly differ on FSIQ, F(1, 229) = 14.21, p = .000. As shown in Table 1, the maltreated adolescents displayed lower FSIQs than the nonmaltreated adolescents. Thus, subsequent analyses controlled for this variable as relevant.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for adolescent characteristics by group

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .005. ****p < .001.

Procedures

Prior to participation in the study, the adolescents provided informed assent and one of their parents provided informed permission for the adolescents’ participation and informed consent for their own participation. If the State of Oregon was the legal guardian of a maltreated adolescent at the time of participation, the child welfare system caseworker (as a representative of the State) also provided informed permission for the adolescent's participation. The adolescents and parents then completed a 2½-hr laboratory-based assessment. The assessment included a standardized measure of general intellectual ability, computer-administered cognitive tasks, and questionnaires and interviews for the adolescents and questionnaires and interviews for the parents. The adolescents and parents received $35 each for completing this assessment. All study documents and procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the Oregon Social Learning Center and Oregon Public Health Division prior to beginning the study.

Measures

Go/no-go task

Behavioral data and ERP data were recorded during a computer-administered go/no-go task that was presented using the STIM Stimulus Presentation System (James Long Company, Caroga Lake, NY). For each trial, a white letter was presented on a black background for 500 ms with a fixed 1500-ms interstimulus interval. The adolescents were instructed to press the button as quickly and accurately as possible for every letter (go trials) except X” (no-go trials). Prior to beginning the task, the adolescents completed ten practice trials that included performance feedback to ensure comprehension of the task instructions. Performance feedback was not provided during the task. The task consisted of 20 go trials, followed by a pseudorandom order of 75% go trials and 25% no-go trials. The fixed interstimulus interval and increased frequency of go trials induce the prepotent behavioral response of pressing the button that must be inhibited for the no-go trials. The adolescents completed two blocks of 180 trials (i.e., a total of 280 go trials and 80 no-go trials). Accuracy and reaction time in milliseconds were recorded for each trial using the STIM Stimulus Presentation System.

Electroencephalogram (EEG) data acquisition and processing

Prior to collecting the EEG data, a calibration file was collected by running a 50 μV, 10-Hz calibration signal through all channels. The EEG data were recorded using a Lycra cap fitted with tin electrodes in accordance with the International 10–20 System (Jasper, Reference Jasper1958). Data were collected from 26 scalp electrodes and two mastoid electrodes with Cz serving as the reference electrode and AFz serving as the ground electrode. Two channels of electrooculogram (EOG) data were recorded with an electrode placed above and below the left eye for vertical EOG and an electrode placed at the outer canthus of each eye for horizontal EOG. Impedances were tested before and after EEG data collection to ensure that each electrode site had an impedance of 10 KΩ or less. The EEG data were amplified by a custom 32-channel isolated bioelectric amplifier (SA Instrumentation Company, San Diego, CA) using filter settings of 0.1 Hz and 100 Hz. The data were digitized using a sampling rate of 512 Hz with a 16-bit A/D converter (DATAQ Instruments, Inc., Akron, OH).

The EEG Analysis System (James Long Company, Caroga Lake, NY) was used to calibrate, rereference, and artifact score the data. Artifact due to vertical eye movement, identified as rapid increases and decreases of 150 μV, was regressed. The EEG data were rereferenced offline using an average mastoid configuration and were digitally refiltered with a 15-Hz low-pass filter. Epochs containing signals ±200 μV and trials with reaction times of less than 200 ms or errors of commission or omission were excluded from analyses. The EEG data at Fz, FCz, Cz, and Pz were time locked to the presentation of the stimulus, corrected using a baseline window of −150 to −50 ms relative to the stimulus, and quantified separately for the go trials and no-go trials. The adolescents were required to have at least 20 artifact-free ERP trials for both trial types to be included in the analyses of the ERP data. Peak amplitude, rather than mean amplitude, was analyzed for the N2 and P3 because there was significant variability in the latency and topography of these ERP components across adolescents. The N2 was identified as the maximum negative peak between 200 and 500 ms relative to the presentation of the stimulus at Fz and FCz, and the P3 was identified as the maximum positive peak between 400 and 700 ms relative to the presentation of the stimulus at Cz and Pz. The selection of the time window and electrode sites for the ERP components was informed by previous studies that used this task with similar-aged, at-risk samples (Loman et al., Reference Loman, Johnson, Westerlund, Pollak, Nelson and Gunnar2013; McDermott et al., Reference McDermott, Westerlund, Zeanah, Nelson and Fox2012) and refined by visual inspection of the ERP waveforms for each adolescent.

Of the 231 adolescents in the current study, one adolescent declined to complete the go/no-go task and thus was not included in the analyses of the behavioral data or ERP data. In addition, one adolescent declined to complete the EEG data acquisition, four adolescents had unusable EEG data due to an issue during data acquisition, and 13 adolescents had less than 20 artifact-free ERP trials for one or both trial types. Thus, these adolescents were not included in the analyses of the ERP data. None of the adolescents were excluded from analyses due to poor behavioral performance on the task, as all of the adolescents exceeded the established criterion for accuracy (i.e., correctly responding to at least 75% of the go trials). In sum, the analytic sample included 230 adolescents (128 maltreated adolescents and 102 nonmaltreated adolescents; 99.6% of the total sample) for the behavioral data and 212 adolescents (116 maltreated adolescents and 96 nonmaltreated adolescents; 91.8% of the total sample) for the ERP data. The percentage of the maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents included in the analyses of the ERP data (89.9% and 94.1%, respectively) did not significantly differ, Pearson χ2(2, N = 231) = 1.33, p = .249. The mean number of artifact-free ERP trials for the analytic sample by group was as follows: 230.05 (SD = 42.29) for the maltreated group and 241.36 (SD = 32.95) for the nonmaltreated group for the go trials and 45.35 (SD = 13.83) for the maltreated group and 51.20 (SD = 13.91) for the nonmaltreated group for the no-go trials. The maltreated group and nonmaltreated group significantly differ in the number of artifact-free ERP trials for the go trials and no-go trials, F(1, 210) = 4.57, p = .034, and F(1, 210) = 9.33, p = .003, respectively. Thus, subsequent analyses controlled for these variables as relevant.

Data analysis

Prior to analyses, the behavioral data (i.e., percentage of correct responses and average reaction time) and ERP data (i.e., peak amplitude of the N2 and P3) were examined for extreme values (i.e., values more than 3 SD above or below the mean). This examination revealed extreme values for eight adolescents for percentage of correct responses (n = 7 for go trials and n = 1 for no-go trials), one adolescent for average reaction time, three adolescents for peak amplitude of the N2 (n = 2 for go trials and n = 2 for no-go trials), and four adolescents for peak amplitude of the P3 (n = 4 for go trials and n = 0 for no-go trials). To ensure that these extreme values did not have undue influence on the results, these adolescents were excluded from the relevant preliminary analysis. The pattern of results for these preliminary analyses was the same as the pattern of the results for the analyses that included all of the adolescents. Thus, the data from these adolescents were retained for subsequent analyses.

Descriptive data for the behavioral data and ERP data collected during the go/no-go task are presented by group in Table 2. The grand average waveforms displaying the N2 at Fz and FCz are presented by trial type and group in Figure 1, and the grand average waveforms displaying the P3 at Cz and Pz are presented by trial type and group in Figure 2. Repeated measures or one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 25, to examine the behavioral data and ERP data. The degrees of freedom, F values, p values, and effect sizes (partial η2) for all of the ANOVAs examining the behavioral data and ERP data are shown in Table 3. Post hoc paired comparisons using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were conducted for significant main effects and interactions. As noted above, there were significant group differences in general intellectual ability and number of artifact-free ERP trials for the go trials and no-go trials. To ensure that significant main effects of and/or interactions with group for the behavioral data and ERP data were not primarily attributable to the group difference in general intellectual ability or number of artifact-free ERP trials for the go trials and no-go trials, analyses controlled for these variables as relevant. That is, for the behavioral data, analyses with significant main effects of group and/or interactions with group were further investigated by conducting a repeated measures or one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) controlling for general intellectual ability. For the ERP data, analyses with significant main effects of group and/or interactions with group were further investigated by conducting a repeated measures ANCOVA controlling for general intellectual ability and number of artifact-free ERP trials for the go trials and no-go trials. (Because the analyses of the behavioral data included all of the trials, not just the trials included in the analyses of the ERP data, the analyses of the behavioral data did not control for the number of artifact-free ERP trials for the go trials and no-go trials.)

Figure 1. Grand average waveforms displaying the N2 at Fz and FCz for the go trials (dashed line) and no-go trials (solid line) for the maltreated group (black line) and nonmaltreated group (gray line).

Figure 2. Grand average waveforms displaying the P3 at Cz and Pz for the go trials (dashed line) and no-go trials (solid line) for the maltreated group (black line) and nonmaltreated group (gray line).

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for behavioral data and event-related potential (ERP) data by group

Table 3. Results of analyses of variance for behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) data

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .005. ****p < .001.

Results

Behavioral data

Percentage of correct responses

A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to examine percentage of correct responses on the go/no-go task with trial type (go and no-go) as the within-subjects factor and group (maltreated and nonmaltreated) as the between-subjects factor. As shown in Table 3, the main effect of trial type was significant, with a higher percentage of correct responses on the go trials (M = 97.8%) than the no-go trials (M = 64.7%). The main effect of group also was significant, with the nonmaltreated adolescents (M = 83.1%) displaying a higher percentage of correct responses on the task overall than the maltreated adolescents (M = 79.4%). In addition, the interaction between trial type and group was significant. To clarify the nature of this interaction, the simple main effects of trial type and group were examined. Post hoc paired comparisons examining the simple main effect of trial type revealed that maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents displayed a higher percentage of correct responses on the go trials than the no-go trials, F(1, 228) = 570.94, p = .000, partial η2 = .72, and F(1, 228) = 323.22, p = .000, partial η2 = .59, respectively. However, the difference between go trials and no-go trials appeared to be more pronounced for the maltreated adolescents than the nonmaltreated adolescents. Post hoc paired comparisons examining the simple main effect of group indicated that the nonmaltreated adolescents had a higher percentage of correct responses than the maltreated adolescents on the go trials and no-go trials, F(1, 228) = 4.13, p = .043, partial η2 = .02, and F(1, 228) = 7.55, p = .006, partial η2 = .03, respectively. However, the group difference appeared to be more pronounced for the no-go trials than the go trials.

To further examine the main effect of group and the interaction between trial type and group, a repeated measures ANCOVA, controlling for general intellectual ability, was conducted to examine percentage of correct responses with trial type (go and no-go) as the within-subjects factor and group (maltreated and nonmaltreated) as the between-subjects factor. Neither the main effect of general intellectual ability nor the interaction with general intellectual ability was significant. Furthermore, the main effect of group and interaction between trial type and group remained significant even after controlling for general intellectual ability.

Average reaction time

A one-way ANOVA was conducted to examine average reaction time on the go trials with group (maltreated and nonmaltreated) as the between-subjects factor. The main effect of group was not significant, as the groups demonstrated similar reaction times on the go trials.

ERP data

Peak amplitude of the N2

A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to examine peak amplitude of the N2 during the go/no-go task with electrode site (Fz and FCz) and trial type (go and no-go) as the within-subjects factors and group (maltreated and nonmaltreated) as the between-subjects factor. As shown in Table 3, the main effect of electrode was significant, with a more negative amplitude of the N2 at Fz (M = −9.97 μV) than at FCz (M = −8.13 μV). The main effect of trial type and the main effect of group were not significant. However, the interaction between trial type and group was significant. To clarify the nature of this interaction, the simple main effects of trial type and group were examined. Post hoc paired comparisons examining the simple main effect of trial type indicated that the maltreated adolescents displayed a more negative amplitude of the N2 during go trials than during no-go trials, F(1, 210) = 5.75, p = .017, partial η2 = .03. In contrast, the nonmaltreated adolescents displayed a more negative amplitude of the N2 during no-go trials than during go trials, F(1, 210) = 3.27, p = .072, partial η2 = .02. Furthermore, post hoc paired comparisons examining the simple main effect of group revealed that the maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents did not differ in terms of amplitude of the N2 during go trials, F(1, 210) = 0.57, p = .452, partial η2 = .00. Conversely, the nonmaltreated adolescents displayed a more negative amplitude of the N2 during no-go trials than the maltreated adolescents, F(1, 210) = 6.82, p = .010, partial η2 = .03. None of the other interactions with electrode site, trial type, or group were significant.

To further examine the interaction between trial type and group, a repeated measures ANCOVA, controlling for general intellectual ability, number of ERP trials for the go trials, and number of ERP trials for the no-go trials, was conducted to examine peak amplitude of the N2 with electrode site (Fz and FCz) and trial type (go and no-go) as the within-subjects factors and group (maltreated and nonmaltreated) as the between-subjects factor. None of the main effects of or interactions with general intellectual ability, number of ERP trials for the go trials, and number of ERP trials for the no-go trials were significant. Furthermore, the interaction between trial type and group remained significant even after controlling for these variables.

Peak amplitude of the P3

A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to examine peak amplitude of the P3 during the go/no-go task with electrode site (Cz and Pz) and trial type (go and no-go) as the within-subjects factors and group (maltreated and nonmaltreated) as the between-subjects factor. The main effects of electrode site and group were not significant. However, the main effect of trial was significant, with a more positive amplitude of the P3 during the no-go trials (M = 15.60 μV) than during the go trials (M = 6.80 μV). Furthermore, the interaction between electrode site and trial type was significant. To clarify the nature of this interaction, the simple main effects of electrode site was examined. These post hoc paired comparisons revealed that the amplitude of the P3 at Cz (M = 6.82 μV) and Pz (M = 6.78 μV) did not differ during go trials, F(1, 210) = 0.03, p = .872, partial η2 = .00. In contrast, the amplitude of the P3 was more positive at Pz (M = 15.93 μV) than at Cz (M = 15.28 μV) during no-go trials, F(1, 210) = 3.40, p = .066, partial η2 = .02. None of the other interactions with electrode site, trial type, or group were significant.

Discussion

There is extensive evidence demonstrating that children and adolescents who were involved with the child welfare system due to child maltreatment are at elevated risk for negative outcomes across multiple domains of functioning, including academic difficulties, attention and behavior problems, and alcohol and substance use (Aarons et al., Reference Aarons, Brown, Hough, Garland and Wood2001; Clausen et al., Reference Clausen, Landsverk, Ganger, Chadwick and Litrownik1998; Crozier & Barth, Reference Crozier and Barth2005; Keller et al., Reference Keller, Salazar and Courtney2010; Pilowsky & Wu, Reference Pilowsky and Wu2006; Zima et al., Reference Zima, Bussing, Freeman, Yang, Belin and Forness2000). It has been speculated that experience-induced alterations in specific cognitive abilities and the underlying neural regions may contribute to the difficulties observed among maltreated children and adolescents (De Bellis, Reference De Bellis2001; Fishbein, Reference Fishbein2000; Gunnar & Fisher, Reference Gunnar and Fisher2006). Therefore, the current study was designed to examine behavioral and electrophysiological indices of one such cognitive ability, inhibitory control, in maltreated adolescents and low-income, nonmaltreated adolescents in early adolescence. The results of the current study contribute to the growing evidence that early adverse experiences negatively affect behavioral and electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control and provide unique information about the specific cognitive ability supporting inhibitory control affected in a maltreated population.

Consistent with the results of previous studies with children, adolescents, and adults (Casey, Trainor, et al., Reference Casey, Trainor, Orendi, Schubert, Nystrom, Giedd and Rapoport1997; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Bruce, Snyder and Nelson2003; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Davidson, Tottenham, Galvan, Spicer, Fossella and Casey2006), both groups of adolescents were less accurate on the no-go trials that require inhibitory control than the go trials that do not require inhibitory control. In general, the adolescents committed very few errors on the go trials. However, they performed quite poorly on the no-go trials, which suggests that successfully inhibiting a prepotent response during the go/no-go task continues to be a challenging task into early adolescence for maltreated populations and low-income populations. Furthermore, as predicted, the maltreated adolescents were less accurate on the go/no-go task than the nonmaltreated adolescents. This group difference in accuracy was more pronounced on the no-go trials than the go trials, and it remained significant even after controlling for the group difference in general intellectual ability. This pattern of results parallels the results of previous studies with other populations exposed to early adverse experiences, including maltreated children in foster care and children adopted from deprived institutions (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Tarullo and Gunnar2009; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Dozier, Ackerman and Sepulveda-Kozakowski2007; Pears et al., Reference Pears, Bruce, Fisher and Kim2010; Pollak et al., Reference Pollak, Nelson, Schlaak, Roeber, Wewerka, Wiik and Gunnar2010). Taken together, these findings suggest that inhibitory control, as assessed by behavioral performance on inhibitory control tasks, may be particularly vulnerable to the effects of early adverse experiences.

While previous studies have examined electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control in other populations exposed to early adverse experiences (Burden et al., Reference Burden, Andrew, Saint-Amour, Meintjes, Molteno, Hoyme and Jacobson2009; Loman et al., Reference Loman, Johnson, Westerlund, Pollak, Nelson and Gunnar2013; McDermott et al., Reference McDermott, Westerlund, Zeanah, Nelson and Fox2012), the current study was the first such study with maltreated adolescents. Interestingly, the pattern of results for both groups of adolescents for the peak amplitude of the P3, which is believed to reflect response potentiation following stimulus evaluation, was consistent with previous research findings with typically developing populations (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Aston-Jones and Cohen2005). That is, the amplitude of the P3 was more pronounced during the no-go trials than during the go trials for the maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents. In contrast, the maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents demonstrated different patterns of results for the peak amplitude of the N2, which is believed to reflect response inhibition or conflict monitoring. Paralleling the results of previous studies with typically developing populations (Folstein & Van Petten, Reference Folstein and Van Petten2008), the nonmaltreated adolescents displayed a more pronounced amplitude of the N2 during the no-go trials than during the go trials. However, the maltreated adolescents demonstrated a more pronounced amplitude of the N2 during the go trials than during the no-go trials. Furthermore, while the maltreated adolescents and nonmaltreated adolescents did not differ in terms of amplitude of the N2 during the go trials, the nonmaltreated adolescents displayed a more negative amplitude of the N2 during no-go trials than the maltreated adolescents. Importantly, these results continued to be significant even after controlling for the group differences in general intellectual ability and the number of ERP trials included in the analyses. Although additional electrophysiological research with maltreated populations is needed, the divergent pattern of results for the amplitude of the N2 and P3 in the current study is intriguing. A possible explanation for this divergent pattern of results is that specific neural regions may be particularly sensitive to the negative effects of early adverse experiences. That is, source localization studies suggest that the N2 is generated in the ventral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex (Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Zelazo and Lewis2006; Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, Van Den Wildenberg and Ridderinkhof2003) and that the P3 is generated in the temporal–parietal junction and lateral prefrontal cortex (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Aston-Jones and Cohen2005). Perhaps the early adverse experiences encountered by maltreated adolescents have an impact on the development and subsequent functioning of the neural regions that generate the N2, but not the neural regions that generate the P3.

Taken as a whole, the results of the current study indicate that the maltreated adolescents were less accurate on the go/no-go task, particularly during the no-go trials that require inhibitory control, and displayed an atypical pattern of results on an ERP component that reflects response inhibition or conflict monitoring, but not an ERP component that reflects response potentiation, compared to the nonmaltreated adolescents. These results suggest that maltreated adolescents demonstrate an impairment in a specific cognitive ability supporting inhibitory control (rather than a more general cognitive ability such as sustained attention). There are several potential implications of the results of the current study. For example, the results may inform the development of a more precise, neurobiologically based explanatory model of the negative outcomes in maltreated populations. As noted above, difficulties with inhibitory control have been implicated in the etiology of a number of negative outcomes that have been observed among populations exposed to early adverse experiences (e.g., academic difficulties, attention and behavior problems, alcohol and substance use). Thus, inhibitory control may serve as a mechanism underlying the associations between early adverse experiences and subsequent negative outcomes among maltreated children and adolescents. Similarly, the results may illuminate a possible intervention target, contributing to the development of more effective and efficient preventive interventions for maltreated populations. That is, it may be beneficial for preventive interventions to specifically target inhibitory control (e.g., teaching skills that improve inhibitory control and/or compensate for deficits in inhibitory control) in an effort to prevent or ameliorate the negative outcomes observed among maltreated children and adolescents. To date, efforts to target inhibitory control generally have fallen into two categories: laboratory-based training (e.g., repeated practice on a computerized inhibitory control task) and ecologically-based intervention (e.g., school readiness intervention that focuses on self-regulation broadly; Bryck & Fisher, Reference Bryck and Fisher2012). While this line of research is still its infancy, it clearly warrants additional attention given the results of the current study.

Although the current study is an important step in understanding inhibitory control and the underlying neural activity in a maltreated population, it also raises a number of critical questions for future research studies. For example, because inhibitory control continues to develop into emerging adulthood and the adolescents were assessed in early adolescence, it is not possible to determine whether the observed group differences in behavioral and electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control represent a delay (i.e., maturational lag that results in the same end state) or a deficit (i.e., persistent quantitatively or qualitatively different end state) in inhibitory control among the maltreated adolescents. Therefore, future research studies should assess inhibitory control in maltreated populations over time into (at least) emerging adulthood. Future studies also should examine the impact of specific child maltreatment experiences (e.g., type, severity, and developmental timing of child maltreatment) on behavioral and electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control. Difficulties accessing complete child welfare system records resulted in a lack of information about the adolescents’ child maltreatment histories in the current study. However, this information is critical to understanding whether different early adverse experiences have differential effects on inhibitory control and the underlying neural regions. In addition, it is imperative to examine the associations between inhibitory control and the underlying neural activity and subsequent negative outcomes in a maltreated population. Because the adolescents in the current study were assessed into late adolescence, future analyses will investigate the relations between the behavioral and electrophysiological indices of inhibitory control assessed in early adolescence and early-onset alcohol and substance use assessed in late adolescence.

In summary, the results of the current study provide additional evidence that early adverse experiences negatively affect inhibitory control and the underlying neural activity. Specifically, compared to the nonmaltreated adolescents, the maltreated adolescents displayed poorer behavioral performance and atypical electrophysical performance on the trials that require inhibitory control. Given the purported role of inhibitory control in the development of a number of important outcomes, the impairment in inhibitory control observed among the maltreated adolescents may have a profound impact on their functioning in late adolescence and beyond. Although promising, the current study highlights the need for future research with maltreated populations. In addition to replicating the results, it will be critical to determine whether alterations in inhibitory control and the underlying neural activity increase the risk of academic difficulties, attention and behavior problems, and alcohol and substance use in maltreated children and adolescents. Furthermore, it will be important to examine the plasticity (i.e., potential for recovery) of inhibitory control and the underlying neural regions following exposure to early adverse experiences.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Larisa Lilles for project management and the adolescents and parents for their participation.

Funding Statement

Support for this research was provided by the following grant: AA021973, NIAAA, U.S. PHS.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

Aarons, G. A., Brown, S. A., Hough, R. L., Garland, A. F., & Wood, P. A. (2001). Prevalence of adolescent substance use disorders across five sectors of care. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 419426. doi:10.1097/00004583-200104000-00010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 410422. doi:10.1038/nrn2648CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, A. R., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S., Frank, M. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2007). Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 37433752. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0519-07.2007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, J. E. (1998). How a child builds its brain: Some lessons from animal studies of neural plasticity. Preventive Medicine, 27, 168171. doi:10.1006/pmed.1998.0271CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647663. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B. L., Davenport, N. D., … Mesulam, M. M. (2005). Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 94111. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00337.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, J., Fisher, P. A., Graham, A. M., Moore, W. E., Peake, S. J., & Mannering, A. M. (2013). Patterns of brain activation in foster children and nonmaltreated children during an inhibitory control task. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 931941. doi:10.1017/s095457941300028xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, J., Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Disinhibited social behavior among internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 157171. doi:10.1017/s0954579409000108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryck, R. L., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Training the brain: Practical applications of neural plasticity from the intersection of cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology, and prevention science. American Psychologist, 67, 87100. doi:10.1037/a0024657CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33, 301311. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00583-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burden, M. J., Andrew, C., Saint-Amour, D., Meintjes, E. M., Molteno, C. D., Hoyme, H. E., … Jacobson, S. W. (2009). The effects of fetal alcohol syndrome on response execution and inhibition: An event-related potential study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 19942004. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01038.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Marsh, W. L., Hamburger, S. D., Schubert, A. B., … Rapoport, J. L. (1997). Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 374383. doi:10.1097/00004583-199703000-00016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., & Fossella, J. (2002). Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic approaches toward a model of cognitive control. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 237254. doi:10.1002/dev.10030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Nystrom, L. E., Giedd, J. N., … Rapoport, J. L. (1997). A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a Go-No-Go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 835847. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.835CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clausen, J. M., Landsverk, J., Ganger, W., Chadwick, D., & Litrownik, A. (1998). Mental health problems of children in foster care. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 7, 283296. doi:10.1023/a:1022989411119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crozier, J. C., & Barth, R. P. (2005). Cognitive and academic functioning in maltreated children. Children and Schools, 27, 197206. doi:10.1093/cs/27.4.197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, E. P., Bruce, J., Snyder, K., & Nelson, C. A. (2003). The X-trials: Neural correlates of an inhibitory control task in children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 432443. doi:10.1162/089892903321593144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bellis, M. D. (2001). Developmental traumatology: The psychobiological development of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 539564. doi:10.1017/s0954579401003078CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey, B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9, 18. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00454.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durston, S., Thomas, K. M., Yang, Y., Ulug, A. M., Zimmerman, R. D., & Casey, B. J. (2002). A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 5, F9F16. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I.-M., Zayas, V., Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., Ayduk, O., Dadlani, M. B., … Casey, B. J. (2006). Predicting cognitive control from preschool to late adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Science, 17, 478484. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01732.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishbein, D. (2000). The importance of neurobiological research to the prevention of psychopathology. Prevention Science, 1, 89106. doi:10.1023/a:1010090114858CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology, 45, 152170. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghashghaei, H. T., & Barbas, H. (2002). Pathways for emotion: Interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience, 115, 12611279. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00446-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., … Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 81748179. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402680101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Fisher, P. A., & The Early Experience Stress and Prevention Network. (2006). Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 651677. doi:10.10170s0954579406060330CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145173. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmeke, C., Seidel, K., Poeggel, G., Bredy, T. W., Abraham, A., & Braun, K. (2009). Paternal deprivation during infancy results in dendrite- and time-specific changes of dendritic development and spine formation in the orbitofrontal cortex of the biparental rodent Octodon degus. Neuroscience, 163, 790798. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, J. P., Ostrander, M. M., Mueller, N. K., & Figueiredo, H. (2005). Limbic system mechanisms of stress regulation: Hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenocortical axis. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 29, 12011213. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, A., & Wellman, C. L. (2009). Stress-induced prefrontal reorganization and executive dysfunction in rodents. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 773783. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371375.Google Scholar
Keller, T. E., Salazar, A. M., & Courtney, M. E. (2010). Prevalence and timing of diagnosable mental health, alcohol, and substance use problems among older adolescents in the child welfare system. Children and Youth Services Review, 32, 626634. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2009.12.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H., Wildeman, C., Jonson-Reid, M., & Drake, B. (2017). Lifetime prevalence of investigating child maltreatment among US children. American Journal of Public Health, 107, 274280. doi:10.2105/ajph.2016.303545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, C., Zelazo, P. D., & Lewis, M. D. (2006). Neural correlates of cognitive control in childhood and adolescence: Disentangling the contributions of age and executive function. Neuropsychologia, 44, 21392148. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, E., Dozier, M., Ackerman, J., & Sepulveda-Kozakowski, S. (2007). The effect of caregiving instability on adopted children's inhibitory control abilities and oppositional behavior. Developmental Psychology, 43, 14151427. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddle, P. F., Kiehl, K. A., & Smith, A. M. (2001). Event-related fMRI study of response inhibition. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 100109. doi:10.1002/1097-0193(200102)12:2 < 100::aid-hbm1007 > 3.0.co;2-63.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loman, M. M., Johnson, A. E., Westerlund, A., Pollak, S. D., Nelson, C. A., & Gunnar, M. R. (2013). The effect of early deprivation on executive attention in middle childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 3745. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02602.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology, 43, 947959. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, J. M., Westerlund, A., Zeanah, C. H., Nelson, C. A., & Fox, N. A. (2012). Early adversity and neural correlates of executive function: Implications for academic adjustment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, S59S66. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, S. C., Maheu, F. S., Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Mandell, D., Leibenluft, E., … Ernst, M. (2010). Early-life stress is associated with impairment in cognitive control in adolescence: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 48, 30373044. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine system. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 510532. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., Van Den Wildenberg, W., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 1726. doi:10.3758/cabn.3.1.17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pears, K. C., Bruce, J., Fisher, P. A., & Kim, H. K. (2010). Indiscriminate friendliness in maltreated foster children. Child Maltreatment, 15, 6475. doi:10.1177/1077559509337891CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pears, K. C., Capaldi, D. M., & Owen, L. D. (2007). Substance use risk across three generations: The roles of parent discipline practices and inhibitory control. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21, 373386. doi:10.1037/0893-164x.21.3.373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pechtel, P., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: An integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology, 214, 5570. doi:10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilowsky, D. J., & Wu, L.-T. (2006). Psychiatric symptoms and substance use disorders in a nationally representative sample of American adolescents involved with foster care. Journal of Adolescent Health, 38, 351358. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.06.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Nelson, C. A., Schlaak, M. F., Roeber, B. J., Wewerka, S. S., Wiik, K. L., … Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Neurodevelopmental effects of early deprivation in postinstitutionalized children. Child Development, 81, 224236. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01391.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Woolley, J., Nosarti, C., Heyman, I., Taylor, E., & Brammer, M. (2006). Progressive increase of frontostriatal brain activation from childhood to adulthood during event-related tasks of cognitive control. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 973993. doi:10.1002/hbm.20237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheinkopf, S. J., Lester, B. M., Sanes, J. N., Eliassen, J. C., Hutchison, E. R., Seifer, R., … Casey, B. J. (2009). Functional MRI and response inhibition in children exposed to cocaine in utero. Developmental Neuroscience, 31, 159166. doi:10.1159/000207503CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. M., Fried, P. A., Hogan, M. J., & Cameron, I. (2004). Effects of prenatal marijuana on response inhibition: An fMRI study of young adults. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 26, 533542. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2004.04.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 82238231. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1798-04.2004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, R. M., & Gratton, A. (2002). Prefrontal cortical regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function in the rat and implications for psychopathology: Side matters. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 99114. doi:10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00038-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thatcher, R. W., Walker, R. A., & Giudice, S. (1987). Human cerebral hemispheres develop at different rates and ages. Science, 236, 11101113. doi:10.1126/science.3576224CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toupin, J., Déry, M., Pauzé, R., Mercier, H., & Fortin, L. (2000). Cognitive and familial contributions to conduct disorder in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 333344. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00617CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troller-Renfree, S. V., Buzzell, G. A., Bowers, M. E., Salo, V. C., Forman-Alberti, A., Smith, E., … Fox, N. A. (2019). Development of inhibitory control during childhood and its relations to early temperament and later social anxiety: Unique insights provided by latent growth modeling and signal detection theory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60, 622629. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2019). Child maltreatment 2017. Retrieved from Washington, DC: https://www.acf.hhs.gov/cb/research-data-technology/statistics-research/child-maltreatmentGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence – Second edition manual. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Wills, T. A., & Stoolmiller, M. (2002). The role of self-control in early escalation of substance use: A time-varying analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 986997. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.70.4.986CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zima, B. T., Bussing, R., Freeman, S., Yang, X., Belin, T. R., & Forness, S. R. (2000). Behavior problems, academic skill delays and school failure among school-aged children in foster care: Their relationship to placement characteristics. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 9, 87103. doi:10.1023/a:1009415800475CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for adolescent characteristics by group

Figure 1

Figure 1. Grand average waveforms displaying the N2 at Fz and FCz for the go trials (dashed line) and no-go trials (solid line) for the maltreated group (black line) and nonmaltreated group (gray line).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Grand average waveforms displaying the P3 at Cz and Pz for the go trials (dashed line) and no-go trials (solid line) for the maltreated group (black line) and nonmaltreated group (gray line).

Figure 3

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for behavioral data and event-related potential (ERP) data by group

Figure 4

Table 3. Results of analyses of variance for behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) data