Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T13:03:51.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Addressing educational inequalities and promoting learning through studies of stress physiology in elementary school students

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2021

Jelena Obradović*
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Education, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Emma Armstrong-Carter
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Education, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Dr. Jelena Obradović, Stanford University, 485 Lasuen Mall, Stanford, CA94305; E-mail: jelena.obradovic@stanford.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

To be ready to learn, children need to be focused, engaged, and able to bounce back from setbacks. However, many children come to school with heightened or diminished physiological arousal due to exposure to poverty-related risks. While stress physiology plays a role in explaining how adversity relates to processes that support students’ cognitive development, there is a lack of studies of physiological stress response in educational settings. This review integrates relevant studies and offers future directions for research on the role of stress physiology in the school adaptation of elementary school students, focusing on these important questions: (a) What are the links between physiological stress response and learning-related skills and behaviors, and do they vary as a function of proximal and distal experiences outside of school? (b) How are school experiences associated with students’ physiological stress response and related cognitive and behavioral adaptations? (c) How can we leverage measures of students’ physiological stress response in evaluations of school-based interventions to better support the school success of every student? We hope to stimulate a new wave of research that will advance the science of developmental stress physiology, as well as improve the application of these findings in educational policy and practice.

Type
Special Section 2: Early Adversity and Development: Contributions from the Field
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Robust and persistent socioeconomic inequalities in educational opportunities and outcomes across the elementary school grades are well established in the United States (Reardon, 2019). While we know that stress physiology can have an important mediating or moderating role in explaining how environmental adversity relates to social and cognitive processes that support students’ learning and academic achievement (Blair & Raver, Reference Blair and Raver2012; Obradović, Reference Obradović2016), there is a lack of studies on the variability of physiological stress response in schools and classrooms. This review article integrates relevant studies and offers future directions for research on the role of stress physiology in the school adaptation of elementary school students, focusing on these important questions: (a) What are the links between physiological stress response and learning-related skills and behaviors, and do they vary as a function of proximal and distal experiences outside of school? (b) How are school experiences associated with students’ physiological stress response and related cognitive and behavioral adaptations? (c) How can we leverage measures of students’ physiological stress response in evaluations of school-based interventions to better support the school success of every student? This review addresses how school and classroom experiences can ameliorate family socioeconomic disadvantage through nurturing and supportive relationships and practices, as well as exacerbate it through systematic disparities in school quality and discriminatory treatment of students of color. Studying children's stress physiology in relation to elementary school experiences extends the pioneering work of Dr. Gunnar and colleagues, who examined children's stress physiology in child care and preschool settings to advance understanding of how public education systems can holistically support the learning and development of diverse students.

The Relevance of Stress Physiology for Learning

Children whose families experience poverty and other socioeconomic risks perform worse than advantaged peers on direct assessments of attention, language, and executive functions (EFs) (Evans & Fuller-Rowell, Reference Evans and Fuller-Rowell2013; Hackman, Gallop, Evans, & Farah, Reference Hackman, Gallop, Evans and Farah2015; Raver, Blair, & Willoughby, Reference Raver, Blair and Willoughby2013). Socioeconomic differences also register in the development and activity of brain areas that support these learning-related skills (Merz, Wiltshire, & Noble, Reference Merz, Wiltshire and Noble2019). Chronic exposure to family stressors has been shown to explain the association between distal measures of socioeconomic risk and brain functioning in children and adults (Farah, Reference Farah2017; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Evans, Angstadt, Ho, Sripada, Swain and Phan2013), and researchers have proposed children's physiological stress response as a mediating pathway by which early adversity gets biologically embedded, undermining the development of cognitive and self-regulatory skills (Blair & Raver, Reference Blair and Raver2012; Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). Given that attentional, language, and self-regulatory capacities are crucial for children's learning-related behaviors and academic achievement (Fuhs, Nesbitt, Farran, & Dong, Reference Fuhs, Nesbitt, Farran and Dong2014), more studies should examine how children's stress physiology explains socioeconomic educational disparities. In this section, we review extant research that shows links between experiences of adversity and stress physiology and between children's physiological responses and cognitive outcomes. We also propose future directions for research, with a focus on understanding how stressors outside the family context contribute to children's unequal opportunities to learn and thrive at school.

Adversity and stress physiology

The prefrontal cortex, a brain area that supports higher order cognitive skills, is particularly vulnerable to the upregulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPAA), a neuroendocrine stress response system that plays a central role in the body's response to chronic stressors. Elevated concentrations of the hormone cortisol, reflecting the activity of the HPAA, can undermine the development and functioning of the prefrontal cortex because of its high concentration of cortisol receptors (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). Most studies linking adverse experiences to children's HPAA responsivity have examined cortisol concentrations in saliva. Early exposure to psychosocial adversity (e.g., limited family resources, maternal mental health problems, hostile parenting) is consistently associated with heightened cortisol reactivity to standardized laboratory stressors among infants and toddlers (Hunter, Minnis, & Wilson, Reference Hunter, Minnis and Wilson2011). Similarly, chronic adverse experiences, such as maltreatment, are linked to a general pattern of hyper-responsive HPAA activity in children and adolescents (Tarullo & Gunnar, Reference Tarullo and Gunnar2006). In contrast, exposure to neglect, often experienced in foster care or institutional settings, has been linked to hypo-responsive HPAA activity (Bruce, Fisher, Pears, & Levine, Reference Bruce, Fisher, Pears and Levine2009). A recent population-based study showed that many indices of socioeconomic adversity did not robustly relate to HPAA activity in elementary school-aged students, but concentrated neighborhood poverty was linked to altered diurnal cortisol output (Malanchini et al., Reference Malanchini, Engelhardt, Raffington, Sabhlok, Grotzinger, Briley and Tucker-Drob2020).

Stress reactivity can also be measured using indices of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) and the parasympathetic nervous systems (PNS), which reflect more acute biobehavioral changes to environmental stressors and challenges (Obradović, Reference Obradović2012; Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović and Boyce2009). Lower socioeconomic status (SES), as indexed by maternal education and household income, has been directly linked to higher baseline sympathetic arousal in early childhood (Giuliano et al., Reference Giuliano, Karns, Roos, Bell, Petersen, Skowron and Pakulak2018) and lower baseline parasympathetic arousal in middle childhood (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Carrère, Siler, Jones, Bowie and Cooke2012; Hinnant & El-Sheikh, Reference Hinnant and El-Sheikh2013). In adolescents, socioeconomic risk and psychosocial adversity are associated with elevated blood pressure, an integrated measure of both SNS and PNS (e.g., Wilson, Kliewer, Plybon, & Sica, Reference Wilson, Kliewer, Plybon and Sica2000). In contrast, experiences of poverty and socioeconomic risk do not appear to be consistently linked with sympathetic or parasympathetic reactivity (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Carrère, Siler, Jones, Bowie and Cooke2012; Erath, Bub, & Tu, Reference Erath, Bub and Tu2016; Hinnant, El-Sheikh, Keiley, & Buckhalt, Reference Hinnant, El-Sheikh, Keiley and Buckhalt2013), but SNS and PNS reactivity have been shown to moderate the effects of adversity on children's outcomes (McLaughlin, Alves, & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin, Alves and Sheridan2014; Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler, & Boyce, Reference Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler and Boyce2010; Obradović, Portilla, & Ballard, Reference Obradović, Portilla and Ballard2016).

To capture the wear-and-tear of various physiological stress response systems in the context of chronic adversity, researchers have employed a cumulative index of allostatic load (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998). For example, children who grew up in socioeconomically disadvantaged homes had more markers of metabolic and cardiovascular risk in adolescence (Evans & Schamberg, Reference Evans and Schamberg2009; Gallo et al., Reference Gallo, Roesch, Bravin, Savin, Perreira, Carnethon and Isasi2019) and in adulthood (Graves & Nowakowski, Reference Graves and Nowakowski2017). Recent work has examined cortisol concentrations in children's hair as an index of cumulative cortisol output over several months, a biomarker that partially reflects chronic adversity exposure (Bates, Salsberry, & Ford, Reference Bates, Salsberry and Ford2017). Some studies have reported higher concentrations of hair cortisol in children whose families have lower income, less parental education, and fewer resources (Rippe et al., Reference Rippe, Noppe, Windhorst, Tiemeier, van Rossum, Jaddoe and van den Akker2016; Vaghri et al., Reference Vaghri, Guhn, Weinberg, Grunau, Yu and Hertzman2013; Vliegenthart et al., Reference Vliegenthart, Noppe, van Rossum, Koper, Raat and van den Akker2016); however, other researchers have found no significant associations between children's hair cortisol levels and family socioeconomic risk (Gerber et al., Reference Gerber, Endes, Brand, Herrmann, Colledge, Donath and Zahner2017; Ursache, Merz, Melvin, Meyer, & Noble, Reference Ursache, Merz, Melvin, Meyer and Noble2017). More research is needed to understand how hair cortisol concentration complements or diverges from salivary levels in measuring the effects of chronic stressors on HPAA activity.

Stress physiology and cognitive skills

Altered baseline cortisol levels have been linked repeatedly to heightened risk for emotion dysregulation and behavioral problems in children growing up in adverse settings (see Gunnar, Doom, & Esposito, Reference Gunnar, Doom, Esposito, Lerner and Lamb2015; Obradović, Reference Obradović2012 for reviews). Less work has tested how baseline cortisol levels relate to cognitive skills in school-aged children. One longitudinal study of low-income families showed that elevated baseline cortisol, measured at home at ages 2, 3, and 4 years, was associated with poor cognitive performance in preschool (Suor, Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, & Manning, Reference Suor, Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti and Manning2015). Similarly, elevated average diurnal cortisol was associated with lower parent-reported self-regulatory skills in preschoolers (Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Cepeda, Krieger, Maggi, D'Angiulli, Weinberg and Grunau2016). However, only a handful of studies have explicitly tested the hypothesis that elevated baseline cortisol may explain how poverty undermines the development of children's cognitive and self-regulatory capacities (Blair & Raver, Reference Blair and Raver2012; Piccolo, Sbicigo, Grassi-Oliveira, & Fumagalli de Salles, Reference Piccolo, Sbicigo, Grassi-Oliveira and Fumagalli de Salles2014). Heightened baseline salivary cortisol in infancy mediated the association between lower parenting quality and poorer EF skills in preschoolers from low-income families (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Raver, Granger, Mills-Koonce and Hibel2011). Similarly, baseline salivary cortisol levels in toddlerhood partially mediated associations between family instability and 1-year change in preschoolers’ delay of gratification skills (Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, Hentges, & Coe, Reference Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, Hentges and Coe2016). It is critical that this work is extended beyond infancy and preschool. A multisystem index of allostatic load in middle childhood was shown to mediate the link between childhood poverty and young adults’ working memory (Evans & Fuller-Rowell, Reference Evans and Fuller-Rowell2013), indicating that stressful life experiences in the elementary school years may have long-term effects on cognitive capacities.

More research has examined how school-aged children's physiological responses to standardized laboratory challenges relate to cognitive and self-regulatory skills. Most such studies focus on PNS reactivity, as indexed by changes in respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), a biomarker of cardiac vagal control that is sensitive to children's efforts to engage, pay attention, and regulate their emotions (Porges, Reference Porges2007). In preschoolers, greater PNS reactivity to laboratory tasks is positively associated with parent reports of attention, engagement, and self-regulation (Calkins, Graziano, & Keane, Reference Calkins, Graziano and Keane2007; Gentzler, Santucci, Kovacs, & Fox, Reference Gentzler, Santucci, Kovacs and Fox2009; Hastings et al., Reference Hastings, Sullivan, McShane, Coplan, Utendale and Vyncke2008; Kahle, Miller, Helm, & Hastings, Reference Kahle, Miller, Helm and Hastings2018), and with teacher reports of on-task classroom behaviors (Blair & Peters, Reference Blair and Peters2003). Meta-analysis has demonstrated a positive link between RSA reactivity and children's cognitive performance (Graziano & Derefinko, Reference Graziano and Derefinko2013). For example, greater RSA reactivity to an EF task was positively related to reading achievement in elementary school students (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Carrère, Siler, Jones, Bowie and Cooke2012). Studies also show a significant interplay between children's baseline and reactivity levels. Elementary school students who had higher baseline RSA and higher RSA reactivity during a laboratory stressor had the fastest growth in cognitive skills (Hinnant et al., Reference Hinnant, El-Sheikh, Keiley and Buckhalt2013).

The differential susceptibility hypothesis (Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2011) posits that the link between children's environmental adversity and cognitive skills varies as a function of children's physiology (Obradović, Reference Obradović2016). Studies have shown that family economic and psychosocial adversity is linked to children's EFs only in children who demonstrated heightened PNS and cortisol response to laboratory tasks (Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler and Boyce2010, Reference Obradović, Portilla and Ballard2016; Skowron, Cipriano-Essel, Gatzke-Kopp, Teti, & Ammerman, Reference Skowron, Cipriano-Essel, Gatzke-Kopp, Teti and Ammerman2014). This research implies that heightened physiological reactivity may support better cognitive performance among children from advantaged family backgrounds, while undermining the cognitive performance of children from disadvantaged backgrounds. However, since physiological arousal is conceptualized as a moderator, these analyses of interactive effects do not explicitly test how the link between stress physiology and children's skills varies as a function of children's environmental risk. More research is needed to elucidate how physiological baseline arousal and physiological reactivity relate to children's abilities to pay attention, engage in schoolwork, regulate their behavior, and learn; and whether these links vary across different levels of socioeconomic risk. For example, blunted cortisol reactivity to a laboratory challenge was related to lower performance on a memory task only among 6-to 7-year-olds from lower-income families (Raffington, Schmiedek, Heim, & Shing, Reference Raffington, Schmiedek, Heim and Shing2018). Researchers should acknowledge the likelihood of a nonlinear link between stress physiology and cognitive skills; the best performance may be associated with moderate levels of arousal (see Keller, El-Sheikh, Granger, & Buckhalt, Reference Keller, El-Sheikh, Granger and Buckhalt2012; Marcovitch et al., Reference Marcovitch, Leigh, Calkins, Leerks, O'Brien and Blankson2010).

Going beyond family context: neighborhood violence and educational disparities

The majority of extant psychophysiological research focuses on understanding how adverse family experiences and caregiving behaviors relate to children's stress physiology, and the implications for socioemotional behaviors. This important research has informed the design of family-based interventions that have improved caregiving practices and children's behavior, as well as children's stress physiology (Slopen, McLaughlin, & Shonkoff, Reference Slopen, McLaughlin and Shonkoff2014). Future research should build on this work in three key ways. First, studies need to examine environmental stressors beyond the family context (e.g., Malanchini et al., Reference Malanchini, Engelhardt, Raffington, Sabhlok, Grotzinger, Briley and Tucker-Drob2020) and explicitly test how structural inequalities and discriminatory experiences affect children's stress physiology. For example, Bush and colleagues found ethnic/racial differences in how family socioeconomic risk relates to cortisol diurnal output in kindergarten (Bush, Obradović, Adler, & Boyce, Reference Bush, Obradović, Adler and Boyce2011). Second, studies should test the dynamic interplay between adverse events and stress physiology over different time intervals. In adolescents, daily reports of positive and negative affect and experiences were linked to changes in HPAA response during the same day and the following day (Armstrong-Carter, Ivory, Lin, Muscatell, & Telzer, Reference Armstrong-Carter, Ivory, Lin, Muscatell and Telzer2020; Heissel, Sharkey, Torrats-Espinosa, Grant, & Adam, Reference Heissel, Sharkey, Torrats-Espinosa, Grant and Adam2018; Kliewer, Reference Kliewer2006). Third, researchers should examine whether and how physiological adaptations to environmental stressors undermine children's capacities to engage with and benefit from educational opportunities designed to reduce socioeconomic disparities in academic skills and performance. Next, we highlight an exemplar body of research that is addressing these needs.

Using a novel study design, Sharkey and colleagues have examined how community violence affects children's cognitive performance. By comparing performances of children who were tested either one week before or after a violent crime happened in their neighborhood, the researchers isolate an acute effect of neighborhood violence on children's test scores from other potentially co-occurring risk. In preschool children, a homicide near a child's home within a week before testing was related to lower levels of attention, impulse control, and pre-academic skills (Sharkey, Tirado-Strayer, Papachristos, & Raver, Reference Sharkey, Tirado-Strayer, Papachristos and Raver2012). In school-age children, and especially in African American students, homicides or violent crimes that occurred before testing were linked to lower performance on vocabulary and reading tests and standardized English Language Arts assessments (Sharkey, Reference Sharkey2010; Sharkey, Schwartz, Ellen, & Lacoe, Reference Sharkey, Schwartz, Ellen and Lacoe2014). A study of 8- to 12-year-olds showed that living within a half mile of crime that happened the week before testing was associated with faster and more error-prone responses (McCoy, Raver, & Sharkey, Reference McCoy, Raver and Sharkey2015). Trait anxiety moderated the effects of proximal community violence on children's selective attention and coping, providing evidence of differential susceptibility to neighborhood risk.

Sleep and stress physiology have been proposed as key mediating processes that explain how environmental stressors, such as neighborhood violence, contribute to the socioeconomic achievement gap (Heissel, Levy, & Adam, Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017). A small study of 11- to 18-year-olds found that children went to bed later than usual on the night after a violent crime in their neighborhood and had more disrupted cortisol response the next morning (Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Sharkey, Torrats-Espinosa, Grant and Adam2018). In a larger study of 18-year-olds, researchers found that community violence was associated with shorter sleep duration and poorer sleep quality only in African American adolescents (Philbrook, Buckhalt, & El-Sheikh, Reference Philbrook, Buckhalt and El-Sheikh2019). The link between community violence concerns and sleep in African American adolescents was further moderated by PNS functioning. Those who showed lower baseline RSA and higher RSA reactivity were most likely to experience poor sleep in the context of high community violence concerns, echoing previous research that found similar results in the context of high marital conflict (e.g., El-Sheikh, Hinnant, & Erath, Reference El-Sheikh, Hinnant and Erath2011).

Children who attend school after a poor night of sleep can experience fatigue, dysregulated physiological arousal, or low attentional capacity, which in turn can limit their ability to learn (Heissel et al., Reference Heissel, Levy and Adam2017). Recent work by El-Sheikh and colleagues has demonstrated that chronic sleep disruption can have a cumulative negative effect on cognitive performance and academic achievement in elementary school students. In a study of low-income families, African American children and those facing higher economic risk reported greater daytime sleepiness over a period of 2 weeks at age 9 (Philbrook, Shimizu, Buckhalt, & El-Sheikh, Reference Philbrook, Shimizu, Buckhalt and El-Sheikh2018). Further, daytime sleepiness predicted a 2-year longitudinal decrease in children's performance on cognitive assessments in a laboratory setting, standardized reading and math tests, and teacher reports of academic functioning. Sleep efficiency and sleep problems were related to academic functioning among 8- to 10-year-old European American students in a linear fashion, but among African American students the effect was nonlinear, accelerating with the severity of problems (El-Sheikh, Philbrook, Kelly, Hinnant, & Buckhalt, Reference El-Sheikh, Philbrook, Kelly, Hinnant and Buckhalt2019). Differences in how sleep affects academic learning may be explained by additional stressors faced by students of color outside their homes. For example, experiences of daily discrimination were linked to sleep disturbances in adolescents who reported ethnic/racial identity exploration, but not in those who reported ethnic/racial commitment (Yip, Cham, Wang, & El-Sheikh, Reference Yip, Cham, Wang and El-Sheikh2020). The effect of community violence on children's sleep, stress arousal, and cognitive capacities may be further exacerbated by race-related stressors, and the interaction of these processes needs to be better understood.

Future studies should identify specific acute and chronic biobehavioral mechanisms that lead to disparities in children's day-to-day and longitudinal capacities to benefit from educational opportunities. Researchers should examine how across-student and within-student differences in physiological stress arousal at the beginning of the school day are related to students’ learning-related behavior at school. Does dysregulated physiological arousal or reactivity following a night of poor sleep mediate the negative effect of sleepiness on children's cognitive processing and learning-related behaviors? Does dysregulated physiological arousal or reactivity at the beginning of the school day mediate or moderate the link between children's psychological stress and attentional control? Given the prevalence of food insecurity among elementary school students in the United States, how does hunger relate to children's engagement in school activities, and what is the role of physiological arousal in explaining this link or potentially exacerbating the associated fatigue? By understanding differences in students’ biobehavioral readiness to learn, we may identify ways to reduce the socioeconomic achievement gap.

Although we need government programs and policies to address structural inequalities that contribute to community violence and discrimination, we also need to understand how educational programs and policies can affect those risks. For example, one study of New York City middle schools found that the effect of recent neighborhood violence on standardized achievement test scores was exacerbated by attending a school perceived as unsafe or having a weak sense of community (Laurito, Lacoe, Schwartz, Sharkey, & Ellen, Reference Laurito, Lacoe, Schwartz, Sharkey and Ellen2019). Academic performance was lowest among boys and Latinx students in the least safe schools, whereas community violence had no effect on student achievement in schools with better climates. Next, we turn to research linking students’ school experiences to their stress physiology.

Students’ Physiological Stress Arousal in School Context

Two decades ago, Dr. Megan Gunnar and colleagues conducted a set of pioneering studies showing that experiences in childcare settings can significantly affect young children's stress response, as indexed by salivary cortisol measures of diurnal HPAA rhythm (Dettling, Gunnar, & Donzella, Reference Dettling, Gunnar and Donzella1999; Tout, de Haan, Campbell, & Gunnar, Reference Tout, de Haan, Campbell and Gunnar1998). Specifically, children aged 2 to 8 had higher salivary cortisol levels during the day in childcare than they did on weekends at home. In this section, we discuss studies that have extended that original finding, articulating new research directions that may further illuminate how primary school experiences may relate to children's stress physiology in ways that affect their learning and school success.

The school year as a distinct period of development and adaptation

A first wave of studies focused on the start of the preschool or childcare program year as an important transition period. Tests of children's cortisol before and after enrollment found significant differences in cortisol levels between the childcare setting and the home context during the first week of the program (Bruce, Davis, & Gunnar, Reference Bruce, Davis and Gunnar2002; Davis, Donzella, Krueger, & Gunnar, Reference Davis, Donzella, Krueger and Gunnar1999). Studies with larger samples confirmed that young children display higher cortisol levels at school than at home during the transition to preschool and elementary school; however, the duration of this physiological response to school experience was less clear (Parent et al., Reference Parent, Lupien, Herba, Dupéré, Gunnar and Séguin2019). Some studies reported no significant differences between school and home levels by the end of the first week (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Davis and Gunnar2002), while more recent studies have found that it may take 6 months of school for elevated cortisol levels to return to baseline (Hall & Lindorff, Reference Hall and Lindorff2017; Turner-Cobb, Rixon, & Jessop, Reference Turner-Cobb, Rixon and Jessop2008; Yang, Lamb, Kappler, & Ahnert, Reference Yang, Lamb, Kappler and Ahnert2017).

Initial studies found that children who were observed to have worse social skills displayed higher cortisol levels in childcare settings, suggesting that peer experiences and social stressors may be key mechanisms explaining environmental differences in stress physiology (Dettling et al., Reference Dettling, Gunnar and Donzella1999; Tout et al., Reference Tout, de Haan, Campbell and Gunnar1998). Similarly, children with a more difficult temperament were more likely to show elevated cortisol levels during the first week (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Davis and Gunnar2002; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Donzella, Krueger and Gunnar1999). Later studies revealed a more complex interplay of children's physiological reactivity, temperament, and social status, which changed as children adapted differently to peer group dynamics (Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović and Boyce2011; Tarullo, Mliner, & Gunnar, Reference Tarullo, Mliner and Gunnar2011). For example, socially dominant, exuberant children who were well integrated in the peer group showed declines in cortisol levels from fall to spring of the preschool year (Tarullo et al., Reference Tarullo, Mliner and Gunnar2011). Conversely, cortisol increased among highly inhibited children, especially those who engaged in social behaviors that resulted in having friends and being popular. Future studies need to explicitly address the timing of assessments in relation to the phases of the school year. Physiological response should be conceptualized as a set of dynamic longitudinal processes that are informed by individual differences, changes in social experiences and adaptations, and external supports.

Measuring physiological stress response in the school context

Extensive research over the last decade has linked various biomarkers of dysregulated stress physiology to variability in individual student experiences of peer rejection, exclusion, bullying, and victimization in school settings (Murray-Close, Reference Murray-Close2013; Vaillancourt, Hymel, & McDougall, Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2013). This research has also established that indices of both HPAA and autonomic nervous system (ANS) stress response can function as mediators and moderators of associations between negative peer experiences and children's social, behavioral, and health problems (Lafko, Murray-Close, & Shoulberg, Reference Lafko, Murray-Close and Shoulberg2015; Lambe, Craig, & Hollenstein, Reference Lambe, Craig and Hollenstein2019; Ouellet-Morin et al., Reference Ouellet-Morin, Odgers, Danese, Bowes, Shakoor, Papadopoulos and Arseneault2011). However, most of this research focuses on the experiences and behaviors of middle school and high school students. Researchers should examine the relations between negative peer experiences and physiological stress arousal in an elementary school context, as well as the implications of physiological response to social stressors. It is important to extend current research that predominantly focuses on social and emotional behavioral outcomes to test how physiological stress response to negative social experiences may undermine students’ ability to engage with class work, maintain focus, and retain new knowledge.

Further, most extant studies assess children's physiological arousal and reactivity in a controlled setting, in response to structured laboratory tasks, which limits our understanding of stress physiology in the school context. Oberle and colleagues have recently conducted studies that addressed these limitations by collecting saliva samples throughout the day in a typical classroom setting in a Canadian elementary school. Their first study showed that afternoon cortisol levels mediated the link between students’ self-reported problems with anger regulation and peer reports of aggression (Oberle et al., Reference Oberle, McLachlan, Catherine, Brain, Schonert-Reichl, Weinberg and Oberlander2017). In the second study, self-report of perspective-taking skills mediated the link between afternoon cortisol levels and peer reports of prosocial behavior (Oberle, Reference Oberle2018). Future studies should go beyond assessing changes in diurnal cortisol response during the school day to capture students’ physiological reactivity to dyadic, small group, and whole classroom experiences.

Teacher behaviors and classroom quality

The initial studies by Gunnar and colleagues, conducted with small samples of preschoolers attending high-quality childcare programs, found that variation in childcare quality was related to children's cortisol responses (Dettling, Parker, Lane, Sebanc, & Gunnar, Reference Dettling, Parker, Lane, Sebanc and Gunnar2000; Watamura, Kryzer, & Robertson, Reference Watamura, Kryzer and Robertson2009). Follow-up studies with larger samples identified specific aspects of classroom experience that were associated with students’ cortisol response. Greater attachment security to the lead teacher was linked to declining cortisol across the preschool day, controlling for child temperament, maternal attachment security, and a global measure of classroom quality (Badanes, Dmitrieva, & Watamura, Reference Badanes, Dmitrieva and Watamura2012). Teacher-reported conflict with students was related to elevated cortisol in preschoolers during their interactions with the teacher, and teacher report of over-dependent student behavior was related to preschoolers’ elevated cortisol during the school day (Lisonbee, Mize, Payne, & Granger, Reference Lisonbee, Mize, Payne and Granger2008). Although these studies demonstrate that individual students’ interactions and relationships with their teachers are important factors that contribute to physiological adaptation at school, this work needs to be replicated and extended across the elementary grades and across more diverse teacher–student experiences.

Researchers have also studied stress physiology in relation to teacher-level and classroom-level variables, which do not vary across individual students in the same class. In a study of 63 preschoolers in 14 classrooms, Hatfield and colleagues found that higher classroom levels of observed emotional support were associated with greater declines in student cortisol across the day and lower levels of SNS arousal, as indexed by alpha-amylase output (Hatfield, Hestenes, Kintner-Duffy, & O'Brien, Reference Hatfield, Hestenes, Kintner-Duffy and O'Brien2013). Another recent study revealed significant interactions between children's temperament and classroom-level experiences in a sample of 338 kindergarteners in 29 classrooms (Roubinov, Hagan, Boyce, Essex, & Bush, Reference Roubinov, Hagan, Boyce, Essex and Bush2017). The association between a difficult temperament, as indexed by negative emotionality, and overall cortisol output across three school days was evident only for students of teachers who displayed lower levels of emotional support and encouragement. The importance of measuring teacher experiences and stressors was further demonstrated by a study of 406 middle-grade students in 17 classrooms, which linked teachers’ self-report of burnout to higher morning cortisol among students (Oberle & Schonert-Reichl, Reference Oberle and Schonert-Reichl2016). Given the large amount of time that children spend in school, we need to better understand how school-based experiences with peers and teachers elicit changes in students’ physiological stress arousal; and, further, how the variability in physiological response to such experiences affects behavioral adaptation and learning.

Unequal and unjust school experiences

Research on children's physiological stress reactivity should also incorporate the fact that some demographic groups receive unequal and unjust treatment in schools. A meta-analysis of 35 studies showed that K-12 teachers treated Latinx and African American students worse than European American peers in many domains: holding lower expectations, asking fewer questions, providing less encouragement, making more referrals for special-needs testing, and making fewer referrals for gifted-and talented testing (Tenenbaum & Ruck, Reference Tenenbaum and Ruck2007). In a recent study of 156 Latinx third and fourth graders in 19 schools, 73% of students reported experiencing ethnic discrimination at school that more often came from teachers than from peers (Brown & Tam, Reference Brown and Tam2019). A higher level of perceived teacher discrimination was related to longitudinal decreases in student-reported school belonging and academic interest, controlling for academic performance. The study also revealed an important interplay of school-level characteristics – teacher discrimination, percent of Latinx students, and observed multiculturalism – in predicting students’ perception of whether their Latinx peers value working hard and doing well in school. Given that experiences of racial and ethnic discrimination are linked to lower academic self-esteem, attendance, achievement, and attainment among Latinx and African American adolescents (Benner et al., Reference Benner, Wang, Shen, Boyle, Polk and Cheng2018), it is critical that we understand whether and how the stress of racism contributes to dysregulated stress physiology that in turn may undermine learning and worsen academic disparities.

Beyond discriminatory treatment by individual teachers and peers, we must also attend to systemic inequities in school disciplinary practices. Disparities in exclusionary discipline start in preschool and persist into higher grades, with African American students receiving disproportionately more out-of-school suspensions than white peers (Gregory & Fergus, Reference Gregory and Fergus2017). Even after controlling for teacher report of individual student behavior and accounting for various teacher, classroom-level, and school-level factors, African American elementary school students were more likely to receive office disciplinary referrals (e.g., Rocque, Reference Rocque2010). While there has been some recent progress in understanding and addressing the link between racial discipline gap and racial achievement gap (Bottiani, Bradshaw, & Gregory, Reference Bottiani, Bradshaw and Gregory2018; Gregory & Fergus, Reference Gregory and Fergus2017), researchers know little about how these highly stressful and potentially traumatizing unjust practices affect the stress physiology and allostatic load of children who receive or witness such treatment on a daily basis.

Moderations by environmental risk

The association between young children's caregiving experiences and stress physiology also varies as a function of their exposure to environmental risk. In a large study of infants and toddlers, family socioeconomic risk, as indexed by parental income, education, occupational prestige, household density, and neighborhood noise and safety, moderated the association between how much time children spent in center-based care and their resting cortisol levels at home (Berry, Blair, Granger, & The Family Life Project Key Investigators, Reference Berry, Blair and Granger2016). An increase in hours spent in childcare was linked to elevated cortisol only among children who experienced low socioeconomic risk at home. The opposite was true for children exposed to high socioeconomic risk, for whom more time in childcare was linked to lower cortisol. While this study did not address whether higher or lower resting cortisol levels were optimal in these samples, it highlights the importance of understanding the interplay of children's experiences across home, neighborhood, and educational settings.

Recently, Miles et al. (Reference Miles, Dmitrieva, Hurwich-Reiss, Badanes, Mendoza, Perreira and Watamura2018) found that differences in children's cortisol levels between home and childcare settings vary as a function of the children's ethnicity, immigration status, and poverty exposure. The children of Latinx immigrants, in contrast to non-immigrant and non-Latinx peers, did not exhibit a diurnal decline in cortisol level in childcare, even though they experienced a robust diurnal decline at home. Children in classrooms with peers who experienced higher poverty showed a flatter cortisol decline on childcare days, independent of their own poverty exposure and the observed childcare quality. Moreover, having a teacher who spoke Spanish moderated the effect of childcare exposure on cortisol response at school; children of both Latinx immigrants and Latinx Americans showed steeper cortisol declines at school, similar to home patterns. This study demonstrates that Spanish-speaking teachers may create a classroom environment that is physiologically less stressful for Spanish-speaking Latinx children. More studies should identify specific classroom practices that buffer socioeconomically disadvantaged children from experiencing the classroom environment as physiologically and psychologically stressful in a way that undermines their school success.

Acute stressors

Moderate levels of cortisol or PNS response have been linked to optimal performance on cognitive tests in preschool students (Blair, Granger, & Razza, Reference Blair, Granger and Razza2005; Marcovitch et al., Reference Marcovitch, Leigh, Calkins, Leerks, O'Brien and Blankson2010). Although this research needs to be extended to elementary school students and to academic tests, the findings are consistent with adult and animal studies that show an inverted U-curve association between stress arousal and cognition (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). Acute stressors such as high-stakes academic testing can elicit changes in physiological stress response that deviate from optimal, moderate levels, thus undermining performance on cognitive and achievement tests. Among low-income children attending 3rd through 8th grades, high-stakes testing was linked to heightened cortisol levels just before the test, relative to cortisol levels during non-testing weeks (Heissel, Adam, Doleac, Figlio, & Meer, Reference Heissel, Adam, Doleac, Figlio and Meer2019). Children who displayed the greatest positive or negative changes in cortisol levels scored significantly worse on the test. Moreover, elementary school students who lived in disadvantaged neighborhoods and experienced chronic adversity were particularly susceptible to the acute stress of high-stakes testing, exhibiting a greater deviation from baseline in their cortisol response. This is consistent with a study which found that experimentally manipulated financial worries negatively impacted performance on cognitive tests among adults who lived in poverty, whereas no effect was found for well-off adults (Mani, Mullainathan, Shafir, & Zhao, Reference Mani, Mullainathan, Shafir and Zhao2013). Ecologically valid conditions of scarcity were also found to reduce within-subject cognitive performance. Thus, students who face chronic family and neighborhood stressors and experience negative stereotypes and discrimination in school contexts can be further disproportionately affected by acute stressors, leading to underperformance on standardized tests and further excerbation of socioeconomic disparities in academic achievement.

However, educators can ameliorate the negative effect of acute stress on children's cognitive capacities. In a small study of 4- to 6-year-olds, researchers tested whether the examiner's demeanor moderated the association between children's physiological response to a fire alarm and their memory of this event 2 weeks later (Quas, Bauer, & Boyce, Reference Quas, Bauer and Boyce2004). Among children who displayed heightened ANS response to the acute stressor, the presence of a supportive and emotionally responsive examiner was related to better memory of the event, whereas a serious, emotionally unsupportive affect was linked to less accurate recall. Recent studies of high school students indicate that individual differences in students’ anxiety, self-regulation skills, and motivational orientation can further moderate links between physiological stress arousal and school functioning (Lee, Jamieson, Miu, Josephs, & Yeager, Reference Lee, Jamieson, Miu, Josephs and Yeager2019). For example, adolescents who believed that intelligence could not be changed (i.e., endorsed a fixed mindset) exhibited higher diurnal cortisol on days when they received negative feedback about their school work (i.e., their grades were declining; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Jamieson, Miu, Josephs and Yeager2019). This was not the case for adolescents who believed that intelligence could be developed. Together, these studies demonstrate that environmental supports and individual appraisal of acute academic stressors play a significant role in understanding students’ physiological response to stress and its implications for cognitive performance.

Impact of School-Based Interventions on Students’ Physiological Arousal

In light of the numerous connections between stress physiology and cognitive skills, it is important to identify scalable, affordable, and contextually appropriate strategies that positively affect children's stress physiology and resultant outcomes in educational settings. Measuring stress physiology as an outcome of school-based interventions has several advantages. First, school programs such as mindfulness or meditation exercises directly target children's physiological sensations as fundamental to the regulation of breathing, attention, emotions, and behavior. In many cases, indices of physiological arousal may be the most proximal metrics of program effectiveness. Measures of stress physiology are more objective than student or adult reports and can be more sensitive indicators of short-term change than Likert-scale ratings. Second, programs that target the quality of student–teacher relationships and classroom/school climate may indirectly affect students’ physiological stress arousal by creating more supportive, validating, and safe learning contexts. The research reviewed above, linking stressful school experiences to students’ stress physiology, underscores the need to identify ways of eliminating stressors and promoting more optimal physiological arousal in the school context. This is especially important for students who otherwise show positive behavioral and academic outcomes and thus may not receive the additional supports they need for healthy development and adaptation. Finally, physiological stress response measures can be used to identify students who benefit more from these programs. Understanding how the impact of an intervention varies as a function of students’ stress arousal can lead to more effective interventions. Next, we review school-based programs that have incorporated measures of children's physiological arousal in evaluation studies and discuss how other programs should leverage physiological assessments to advance understanding of their impact on students’ healthy development and learning.

Mindfulness practices

Over the past two decades, elementary schools have adopted classroom-based mindfulness practices to help students cope with daily stressors and challenges by improving their ability to stay focused, regulate emotions, and connect with others in a kind and empathetic manner. Practices vary from simple deep breathing or calming techniques integrated in the daily school schedule to multiweek special curricula that teach explicit mindfulness strategies, such as how to increase and maintain awareness of momentary physiological sensations, internal thoughts and feelings, and environmental stimuli. A few randomized control trials have demonstrated positive effects on directly assessed EF skills in preschoolers (Zelazo, Forston, Masten, & Carlson, Reference Zelazo, Forston, Masten and Carlson2018) and self-report of emotion regulation in elementary school (Black & Fernando, Reference Black and Fernando2014; van de Weijer-Bergsma, Langenberg, Brandsma, Oort, & Bögels, Reference van de Weijer-Bergsma, Langenberg, Brandsma, Oort and Bögels2012). Given the relative ease and scalability of mindfulness practices (e.g., Bakosh, Snow, Tobias, Houlihan, & Barbosa-Leiker, Reference Bakosh, Snow, Tobias, Houlihan and Barbosa-Leiker2016), rigorous evaluation work should be conducted with socioeconomically and racially diverse students in large, public school districts. A study of 300 African American middle school students showed that universal stress reduction mindfulness training in Baltimore public schools significantly reduced self-report of internalizing symptoms and poor emotion regulation strategies such as negative coping and rumination (Sibinga, Webb, Ghazarian, & Ellen, Reference Sibinga, Webb, Ghazarian and Ellen2016).

To better understand the benefits of such programs, future evaluation studies need to examine students’ physiological stress responses. A meta-analysis of 45 randomized control trials conducted with adults found that meditation interventions significantly decreased various markers of physiological arousal (e.g., blood pressure, heart rate, cortisol) when compared to an active control condition (Pascoe, Thompson, & Ski, Reference Pascoe, Thompson and Ski2017). Among African American 9th graders, a 3-month breathing awareness intervention administered by health education teachers significantly decreased blood pressure and heart rate both during school hours and over a 24-hour period (Gregoski, Barnes, Tingen, Harshfield, & Treiber, Reference Gregoski, Barnes, Tingen, Harshfield and Treiber2011). The intervention, consisting of 10-min breathing sessions every day (including at home on weekends), was found to be more effective than life-skills training or regular health education classes. Notably, the effect was not detected on student-reported perceived stress, highlighting the importance of including direct physiological measures in addition to student questionnaires.

When selecting a physiological system and type of response marker to be measured, researchers should consider how that system and marker respond to different stressors and should understand the functional implications of different response patterns. Two examples here show the complexity of interpreting stress response patterns. In an evaluation study of a school-based mindful yoga intervention administered to high school students who were at risk of dropping out of school, the control group demonstrated a posttest decrease in SNS reactivity to an EF task, whereas the intervention group showed a nonsignificant increase (Fishbein et al., Reference Fishbein, Miller, Herman-Stahl, Williams, Lavery, Markovitz and Johnson2016). The authors interpreted the maintenance of higher SNS reactivity during the cognitively demanding task as evidence of sustained engagement, which could potentially protect against drug misuse behaviors associated with lower SNS arousal. Meanwhile, an evaluation of an elementary school mindfulness intervention found that the control group exhibited significant posttest changes in the diurnal cortisol pattern and a flatter slope, while the intervention group showed higher morning cortisol levels and a declining diurnal slope, similar to what was observed before the intervention (Schonert-Reichl et al., Reference Schonert-Reichl, Oberle, Lawlor, Abbott, Thomson, Oberlander and Diamond2015). Given that the intervention had positive effects on EFs, well-being, social skills, peer relationships, and academic achievement, it is feasible that the declining cortisol pattern across the day is evidence of a buffering effect against the flattened diurnal cortisol pattern that have been associated with school stressors or lack of emotional support (e.g., Hatfield et al., Reference Hatfield, Hestenes, Kintner-Duffy and O'Brien2013; Hatfield & Williford, Reference Hatfield and Williford2017; Miles et al., Reference Miles, Dmitrieva, Hurwich-Reiss, Badanes, Mendoza, Perreira and Watamura2018). Future studies should link the variability in children's physiological stress responses and intervention-related changes in physiological responsivity to other measures of students’ experiences and behavior to fully understand the implications of stress physiology for school adaptation. This approach can help identify the most optimal patterns of physiological responsivity in this context.

Reappraisal of physiological arousal

Studies of college students have demonstrated the most direct links between acute stress arousal and performance on cognitive and achievement tests. Anticipation of and participation in a test-taking experience can elicit psychological symptoms of anxiety and stress as well as a heightened physiological stress arousal, which in turn can undermine optimal performance. Jamieson and colleagues have showed that college students’ subjective appraisal of physiological arousal can be experimentally manipulated in a way that changes both their physiological arousal and performance on a test (Jamieson, Nock, & Mendes, Reference Jamieson, Nock and Mendes2012). College students who were taught to reappraise their physiological “jitters” as a sign of excitement that could enhance test performance demonstrated heightened SNS response as well as higher scores on both practice and actual GRE math tests, compared with students who were not assigned to the reappraisal condition (Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, & Schmader, Reference Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock and Schmader2010). Moreover, experimentally manipulated changes in SNS response in a laboratory setting positively predicted students’ scores on a regular standardized math test. In a different study, college students in a reappraisal condition showed improvements in physiological arousal as well as attentional bias to negative emotional stimuli (Jamieson et al., Reference Jamieson, Nock and Mendes2012). Experimentally manipulated changes in cardiovascular reactivity were linked with lower threat-related attentional bias (Jamieson et al., Reference Jamieson, Nock and Mendes2012). Others have replicated the finding that reappraising test anxiety as positive excitement rather than stress leads to better performance on a college exam the next day (Brady, Hard, & Gross, Reference Brady, Hard and Gross2018).

Given the prevalence of formative and summative direct assessments in elementary schools, it is important to extend this research to younger students. Laboratory studies of school-aged children show that cognitive reappraisal is associated with physiological stress arousal and the processing of emotion-laden content. In a community sample of children and adolescents, higher levels of self-reported cognitive reappraisal during a socially evaluative laboratory challenge were related to higher cortisol response (Johnson, Perry, Hostinar, & Gunnar, Reference Johnson, Perry, Hostinar and Gunnar2019). Further, cognitive reappraisal may have important implications for learning. Davis and colleagues found that 6- to 13-year-olds who were instructed to cognitively reappraise their emotions while watching a sad film had enhanced recall of information from an educational film that was shown immediately afterward, compared to children who were instructed to ruminate or received no instructions (Davis & Levine, Reference Davis and Levine2013). Cognitive reappraisal of a sad film was also linked to lower RSA reactivity during the film in 5- to 6-year-olds, suggesting that reappraisal instructions can change PNS regulation of negative emotions (Davis, Quiñones-Camacho, & Buss, Reference Davis, Quiñones-Camacho and Buss2016). Further, the cognitive reappraisal strategies of preschoolers and elementary school students have been linked to changes in late positive potential amplitudes, a neural marker associated with the intensity of emotional response (e.g., DeCicco, O'Toole, & Dennis, Reference DeCicco, O'Toole and Dennis2014).

Together these studies suggest that teacher-instructed cognitive reappraisal of psychological stress and related physiological stress arousal may be an effective, scalable, affordable, and universally appropriate strategy to help elementary school students cope with acute learning-related challenges and educational assessments. These strategies do not require the adoption of a complex intervention program or extensive training; rather, they are conceived as simple and brief exercises that can precede everyday learning activities to help young children momentarily achieve a physiological state that supports, rather than undermines, learning objectives. For example, a teacher may ask students to notice and reappraise their fast heart rate as something that will help them do well on an activity, rather than a sign of being worried or unsure about their performance. Future studies should examine how individual differences in students’ physiological and emotional reactivity as well as experiences of environmental stressors, including stereotype threat, may moderate the effectiveness of these approaches.

Teacher–student relationships, classroom practices, and school experiences

Programs and practices that directly target students’ physiological arousal are not the only opportunities to understand how classroom-based interventions affect stress physiology and related functioning. A recent intervention designed to improve dyadic relationship quality between teachers and preschool students who were identified as showing disruptive behaviors showed a significant effect on children's cortisol decline during the preschool day (Hatfield & Williford, Reference Hatfield and Williford2017). Students who were randomly assigned to the 7-week intervention program met with their teachers for a 10- to 15-minute private session outside the classroom two to three times per week. Teachers were instructed to let the child lead the activity during this protected time and to engage by observing, labeling, and reflecting on the child's actions and emotions. Preschoolers in the intervention had a significant decline in their morning cortisol levels, while preschoolers who participated in regular classroom programming exhibited an increase in cortisol across the morning. Although this finding needs to be replicated with larger, more diverse samples and extended to elementary school students, it is consistent with prior evidence that relationship-focused interventions for foster caregivers led to better regulated cortisol functioning in children (Bernard, Peloso, Laurenceau, Zhang, & Dozier, Reference Bernard, Peloso, Laurenceau, Zhang and Dozier2015; Slopen et al., Reference Slopen, McLaughlin and Shonkoff2014). A recent evaluation of the small-group mindfulness and reflection training found that this intervention did not improve preschoolers’ EF skills more than the small-group literacy training, even though it was more effective than a business-as-usual control group (Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Forston, Masten and Carlson2018). It may be that the personal attention and relationship building that happens in small-group activities has a greater effect on students’ stress physiology than the content of the program.

Future research should seek to understand how efforts to improve classroom relationships can affect students’ physiological stress-regulation, especially among students who are seen as disruptive and thus at higher risk for experiencing conflict with teachers (Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce, & Obradović, Reference Portilla, Ballard, Adler, Boyce and Obradović2014; Zimmermann, Reference Zimmermann2018). There is growing interest in helping educators recognize how experiences of trauma can have profound effects on students’ physiological arousal, attentional biases, emotion processing, and behavioral control, and in providing educators with appropriately supportive pedagogies (e.g., Overstreet & Chafouleas, Reference Overstreet and Chafouleas2016). Since exposure to trauma disrupts the functioning of physiological stress systems that can influence children's adaptation, coping, and interactions with others (Tarullo & Gunnar, Reference Tarullo and Gunnar2006), it is important to examine indices of physiological stress arousal in impact evaluations of trauma-informed school practices.

Measures of physiological arousal can also augment evaluations of culturally responsive interventions designed to address teachers’ implicit biases, improve students’ sense of belonging and safety, and reduce racial disparities in disciplinary referrals (Bottiani et al., Reference Bottiani, Bradshaw and Gregory2018; Bradshaw et al., Reference Bradshaw, Pas, Bottiani, Debnam, Reinke, Herman and Rosenberg2018; Okonofua, Walton, & Eberhardt, Reference Okonofua, Walton and Eberhardt2016). We need to identify school-based processes that can support students who face higher systemic inequalities and discrimination, in and out of school. African American students who show positive adaptation on cognitive and behavioral measures also often exhibit heightened levels of physiological stress response and elevated allostatic load that is linked to poor health outcomes in adulthood (Hostinar & Miller, Reference Hostinar and Miller2019). For example, African American children in a low-income, rural setting who were rated by teachers as having high levels of self-control, academic skills, and social skills at age 11–13 exhibited high allostatic load at age 19, a marker that includes elevated blood pressure and overnight stress hormones (Brody, Lei, Chen, & Miller, Reference Brody, Lei, Chen and Miller2014). There is a great need to identify school-based programs that can reduce the physiological trade-offs that are observed in African American students who are seen as successful and resilient according to more traditional school metrics.

School recess also offers a unique opportunity to understand the role of stress physiology in adaptation. Recess presents elementary school students with daily opportunities to practice regulating positive and negative emotions, resolving interpersonal conflicts, expressing respect and empathy, and engaging in positive peer relationships (Dusenbury & Weissberg, Reference Dusenbury and Weissberg2017; London, Westrich, Stokes-Guinan, & McLaughlin, Reference London, Westrich, Stokes-Guinan and McLaughlin2015). Physical activity in school settings has been positively linked with school engagement and cognitive performance (Best, Reference Best2010; Owen et al., Reference Owen, Parker, Van Zanden, MacMillan, Astell-Burt and Lonsdale2016). After engaging in physical activity at school, preschoolers displayed higher levels of EFs than after being sedentary (Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Anand, Graff, Murphy, Qu, Völgyi and Tylavsky2013). Elementary school students randomly assigned to 90 min per week of physically active lessons showed greater improvement on standardized academic tests than the control group across a 3-year period (Donnelly & Lambourne, Reference Donnelly and Lambourne2011). Further, studies show that children from lower-income families and children who display lower levels of working memory may benefit more from school-based physical exercise (Budde et al., Reference Budde, Voelcker-Rehage, Pietrassyk-Kendziorra, Machado, Ribeiro and Arafat2010; Tine & Butler, Reference Tine and Butler2012). In addition to supporting individual student health, school recess can promote a positive school climate. In economically disadvantaged schools, a program designed to improve student safety and inclusivity at recess was associated with teacher and principal perceptions of a positive, supportive school climate (London et al., Reference London, Westrich, Stokes-Guinan and McLaughlin2015).

Even though regulation of physiological arousal may explain how recess beneficially affects students’ acute cognitive performance and long-term school success, studies linking physical activity and physiological reactivity in the school context are limited and primarily focused on showing that physical activity can induce cardiovascular or hormonal responses (Budde et al., Reference Budde, Voelcker-Rehage, Pietrassyk-Kendziorra, Machado, Ribeiro and Arafat2010; Tine & Butler, Reference Tine and Butler2012). Future studies should examine whether acute or chronic changes in students’ stress response may mediate or moderate the positive effects of physical activity on acute cognitive performance and cumulative academic achievement. Given that recess can also be a context for negative and discriminatory peer interactions that have been linked to students’ stress physiology (Brody et al., Reference Brody, Lei, Chen and Miller2014, Reference Brody, Miller, Yu, Beach and Chen2016; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2013), researchers should consider whether the recess is organized as an inclusive experience that supports cognitive and academic performance of all students (London et al., Reference London, Westrich, Stokes-Guinan and McLaughlin2015). Future research should test whether a well-managed recess reduce socioeconomic gaps in student achievement as well as in associated physiological trade-offs. This work should also address the impact of withholding recess as a punishment (London, Reference London2019).

Methodological and Practical Considerations

Researchers who are examining students’ physiological stress response in relation to learning-related skills, behaviors, and experiences must carefully consider: (a) which index of physiological stress response to use; (b) when to measure the stress response; and (c) how variability in the response should be interpreted. Since a detailed review of the methodological considerations for all physiological markers is beyond the scope of this paper, we encourage readers to consult other sources as they hone their study design (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017; Dennis, Buss, & Hastings, Reference Dennis, Buss and Hastings2012; Hinnant, Philbrook, Erath, & El-Sheikh, Reference Hinnant, Philbrook, Erath and El-Sheikh2018; Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović, Boyce, Mayes and Lewis2012). We also urge cross-disciplinary collaborations that include experts in education, developmental science, and psychophysiology, to ensure a high level of ecological validity, psychometric reliability, and analytic rigor. Further, researchers studying physiological stress response in schools should partner with local schools and communities to design studies that have clear implications for educational policy and practice.

Parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) responses

Variability in children's PNS arousal, as indexed by heart rate variability or RSA, has been linked to adaptive behaviors that support learning. For example, young children's greater PNS response during EF tasks is related to higher levels of concurrent EF performance (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Carrère, Siler, Jones, Bowie and Cooke2012; Graziano & Derefinko, Reference Graziano and Derefinko2013). Similarly, PNS response to other cognitive or socio-emotional laboratory challenges is positively linked to parent report of children's cognitive and emotional self-regulation, and teacher report of on-task behavior (Blair & Peters, Reference Blair and Peters2003; Gentzler et al., Reference Gentzler, Santucci, Kovacs and Fox2009; Hastings et al., Reference Hastings, Sullivan, McShane, Coplan, Utendale and Vyncke2008; Kahle et al., Reference Kahle, Miller, Helm and Hastings2018). Thus, PNS response has been identified as a physiological marker of sustained attention, self-regulation, and social engagement (Hinnant et al., Reference Hinnant, Philbrook, Erath and El-Sheikh2018; Porges, Reference Porges2007). Changes in PNS arousal can be elicited using mild-to-moderate challenges that children encounter while working on learning-related activities.

More basic research is needed to identify optimal levels of PNS reactivity for performance on cognitive and academic tests among diverse groups of elementary school students. It is important that this work investigate both between-subject and within-subject variability to confirm or qualify limited evidence suggesting a curvilinear association between PNS arousal and cognitive performance (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Granger and Razza2005; Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Graziano and Keane2007; Marcovitch et al., Reference Marcovitch, Leigh, Calkins, Leerks, O'Brien and Blankson2010). Experimental manipulation of external learning supports can identify factors that promote or undermine children's learning and performance via changes in physiological arousal. In the classroom context, PNS measures can be employed to understand how physiological arousal interacts with students’ self-regulatory capacities as well as classroom supports to promote focused attention, intrinsic motivation, classroom engagement, and dyadic interactions. PNS reactivity could be measured in response to specific classroom activities as well as assessment experiences. Further, measuring changes in RSA can elucidate how classroom exercises in breathing, mindfulness, or reappraisal can promote optimal physiological arousal for learning and test performance in elementary school students.

Recent advances in wearable technology make studying PNS response in educational settings more feasible, as new fitness trackers include measures of heart rate variability. However, the quality of raw heart rate variability data varies considerably in devices marketed to the general public. Some commercial trackers sample data at a much lower frequency than the research-based standard, and some apply proprietary algorithms to deal with missing or implausible data points. These features limit the accessibility and reliability of raw data and prevent the use of novel second-to-second analytic strategies, which can help researchers understand the dynamics of physiological adaptation (e.g., Gates, Gatzke-Kopp, Sandsten, & Blandon, Reference Gates, Gatzke-Kopp, Sandsten and Blandon2015). Despite these limitations, commercial trackers can be used at scale to measure variability in overall patterns of physiological responses in the classroom environment. Such studies can help us understand how classroom-level factors differentially impact students’ physiology and how physiology interacts with classroom experiences to further affect students’ behavior. By measuring PNS arousal in all children at the same time, researchers will advance our understanding of the role of peers’ stress physiology in co-regulating students’ arousal, social affiliation, and engagement.

Sympathetic nervous system (SNS) responses

Most studies examining individual differences in children's SNS arousal have examined how this physiological response to environmental threats and rewards relates to children's emotional and behavioral dysregulation and symptoms of developmental psychopathology (Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović, Boyce, Mayes and Lewis2012). Three biomarkers of SNS response have been employed by developmental researchers: cardiac measure of pre-ejection period, peripheral measure of skin conductance level, and salivary measure of alpha amylase. Momentary changes in the first two markers can be captured in response to specific stimuli. Pre-ejection period reactivity has been linked to children's sensitivity to rewards and behavioral approach tendencies, whereas skin conductance reactivity has been linked to children's sensitivity to fearful stimuli and behavioral measures of anxiety, inhibition, and behavioral problems (Brenner, Beauchaine, & Sylvers, Reference Brenner, Beauchaine and Sylvers2005; Kalvin, Bierman, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Kalvin, Bierman and Gatzke-Kopp2016; Murray-Close, Reference Murray-Close2013). In contrast, salivary alpha amylase reactivity is measured 5–10 min after the onset of a stressor, thus representing a cumulative marker of sympathetic response to the stressful experience (Granger, Kivlighan, El-Sheikh, Gordis, & Stroud, Reference Granger, Kivlighan, El-Sheikh, Gordis and Stroud2007). While elevated salivary alpha amylase response to laboratory challenges has been found among children who report high levels of social anxiety (Payne, Hibel, Granger, Tsao, & Zeltzer, Reference Payne, Hibel, Granger, Tsao and Zeltzer2014), more research is needed to understand the relevance of this biomarker for adaptation of elementary school students.

Most SNS research has been conducted with children diagnosed with, or at risk for, learning or behavioral problems (e.g., Lorber, Reference Lorber2004; Murray-Close, Reference Murray-Close2013) or in the context of family adversity (El-Sheikh et al., Reference El-Sheikh, Kouros, Erath, Cummings, Keller and Staton2009). We need more community studies of diverse elementary school students. Assessment of cardiac pre-ejection periods is currently not practical outside of the laboratory; however, skin conductance levels can be studied at scale in classroom settings with wearable technology. Geršak et al. (Reference Geršak, Vitulić, Prosen, Starc, Humar and Geršak2020) used wearable armband trackers with 7-year-old children to show that an active teaching style was associated with greater skin conductance reactivity and greater recall of geometry facts 6 months later, compared to a control group exposed to a traditional teaching method. Given the ease with which educational researchers can incorporate wearable trackers in field studies, more validation work is needed to demonstrate that these methods yield high-quality, reliable skin conductance data from children in educational settings. Skin conductance response is most reliably measured in the palm of the hand, which has a high density of eccrine sweat glands, but wearable trackers typically place sensors on wrists and palms, resulting in a lower signal.

Since skin conductance reactivity is associated with children's responses to threating and fearful stimuli as well as heightened state and trait anxiety (Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović, Boyce, Mayes and Lewis2012), this biomarker could be used to study how unequal and unjust school experiences affect students’ classroom engagement, academic performance, and general school belonging. Future research should also test how social buffers, such as warm and supportive teachers, can protect children at risk for test anxiety or stereotype threat. Parent–child relationship quality has been found to moderate the effect of temperament on skin conductance level reactivity to fear-inducing films, such that fearful children with low-quality parenting showed high reactivity, while the inverse was true for fearful children with high-quality parenting (Gilissen, Koolstra, van Ijzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van der Veer, Reference Gilissen, Koolstra, van Ijzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van der Veer2007). Similar studies could help us understand physiological responses to school experiences, and their effects on learning-related behaviors.

Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPAA) responses

In children, HPPA responses are typically studied by measuring salivary concentrations of cortisol, the human glucocorticoid hormone secreted by adrenal glands and passively diffused into saliva. Salivary cortisol levels naturally peak 30–40 min after waking, known as the cortisol awakening response, and they gradually decrease throughout the day until reaching a low point in the middle of the night, several hours after the onset of sleep (Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović, Boyce, Mayes and Lewis2012). Cortisol response to specific stressors or challenges can be detected in saliva approximately 15–20 min after the onset of the experience, and the pattern of reactivity may vary across the day. Any study of cortisol levels needs to account for this daily rhythm by assessing cortisol response at the same time of day or by controlling for the time of sample collection. Researchers can measure cortisol awakening response, reactivity to a specific experience, or changes in the overall diurnal rhythm, but in all cases, they should take multiple measures to reliably capture variability in the response and to distinguish between variance which is situational and that which can be attributed to more stable factors. Longitudinal studies should also address how long-term stability and changes in HPAA functioning affect cognitive development and learning. In one study, young children who showed moderate fluctuations in baseline cortisol during the first 5 years of life performed better on EF tasks at age 5, compared to children with either highly stable or highly variable longitudinal profiles (Blair, Berry, & The Family Life Project Investigators, Reference Blair, Berry and The Family Life Project Investigators2017).

In educational settings, cortisol responses have been employed to understand (a) how diurnal rhythm differs between school and home settings among preschoolers (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Davis and Gunnar2002; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Donzella, Krueger and Gunnar1999; Parent et al., Reference Parent, Lupien, Herba, Dupéré, Gunnar and Séguin2019); (b) links between the quality of classroom experiences and students’ cortisol levels in preschool and elementary school (Badanes et al., Reference Badanes, Dmitrieva and Watamura2012; Gunnar, Kryzer, Van Ryzin, & Phillips, Reference Gunnar, Kryzer, Van Ryzin and Phillips2010; Watamura et al., Reference Watamura, Kryzer and Robertson2009); and (c) cortisol reactivity to acute learning-related laboratory stressors (Kahle et al., Reference Kahle, Miller, Helm and Hastings2018; Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler and Boyce2010, Reference Obradović, Portilla and Ballard2016; Quas et al., Reference Quas, Bauer and Boyce2004, Reference Quas, Carrick, Alkon, Goldstein and Boyce2006; Skowron et al., Reference Skowron, Cipriano-Essel, Gatzke-Kopp, Teti and Ammerman2014) or learning-related interventions (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Quiñones-Camacho and Buss2016; Hatfield & Williford, Reference Hatfield and Williford2017). This work needs to be extended to studies of larger, more diverse samples of students experiencing a greater range of educational opportunities and risks. Further, researchers should investigate how cortisol adaptations relate to behavioral outcomes. For example, in a study of preschoolers’ cortisol responses to family daycare experiences, researchers found that elevated cortisol placed girls at risk for anxious, vigilant behavior and boys at risk for angry, aggressive behavior (Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, Kryzer, Van Ryzin and Phillips2010). New research should focus on learning-related classroom behavior such as classroom engagement, frustration tolerance, and participation in group activities.

Cortisol response is particularly sensitive to experiences of social evaluation and to perceived or actual threats to psychological or physical safety. As such, cortisol response could be especially useful for studying whether educational settings are safe, nurturing, and validating spaces that promote the learning of all students. Researchers could use cortisol response to evaluate dyadic interventions that target student-teacher relationship quality, group interventions that target collaborative power dynamics and communication among peers, classroom-level interventions that aim to create a more inclusive and supportive climate, or school-level policies that address discriminatory disciplinary practices or trauma-informed pedagogies. Researchers should analyze short-term and long-term physiological changes and adaptation to these interventions. While large-scale evaluations may be impeded by methodological, practical, and ethical concerns around saliva collections during the school day, promising new technologies may address those issues – for example, through a skin patch that continuously records levels of cortisol in sweat (Parlak, Keene, Marais, Curto, & Salleo, Reference Parlak, Keene, Marais, Curto and Salleo2018; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Mun, Kwon, Park, Bao and Park2019).

Interpretation considerations

Researchers who are interested in embedding measures of stress physiology in real-world settings where children develop and learn need to consider additional factors which may impact the validity and applicability of their findings. First, when using a new technology, unless it is explicitly designed for use with children, researchers should verify that the technology captures reliable and valid data from children in naturalistic settings. Second, studies should control for experiences that can evoke changes in physiological response but are not the target of the investigation or intervention, such as body posture, body temperature, and rate of speech (Bush et al., Reference Bush, Obradović, Adler and Boyce2011). Third, the timing of data collection should account for changes in social and behavioral dynamics across the school year (Hall & Lindorff, Reference Hall and Lindorff2017; Obradović & Boyce, Reference Obradović and Boyce2011; Tarullo et al., Reference Tarullo, Mliner and Gunnar2011; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Lamb, Kappler and Ahnert2017). For example, one study of low-income preschoolers in a Head Start program found that cortisol levels were lower in students who had just attended an art class, compared to peers who attended a homeroom class, but the effect was significant only at the middle and end of the school year (Brown, Garnett, Anderson, & Laurenceau, Reference Brown, Garnett, Anderson and Laurenceau2017). Fourth, researchers studying how stress physiology explains socioeconomic educational gaps should consider that elementary school teacher reports of student behavior and achievement show systemic racial/ethnic and gender disparities, after controlling for performances on direct assessments (e.g., Garcia, Sulik, & Obradović, Reference Garcia, Sulik and Obradović2019). Linking variability in students’ physiological responses to direct assessments of behaviors and skills will be critical to translating research findings to inform more equitable educational practice. Finally, researchers should follow new science which shows that children's stress physiology is a dynamic process of adaptation to environmental experiences, rather than a biological trait indicating individual vulnerability or sensitivity (Obradović, Reference Obradović2016).

Conclusion

To be ready to learn, children need to be focused, engaged, and able to bounce back from setbacks. However, many children come to school with heightened or diminished physiological arousal due to chronic exposure to poverty-related risks. Interventions at the school and classroom level can help students achieve physiological responses that promote learning and well-being, and through such strategies we can reduce disparities in educational experiences and outcomes. Studying stress physiology in public elementary schools has potential to address the known inequities in developmental psychology research (Brown, Mistry, & Yip, Reference Brown, Mistry and Yip2019), without placing undue burden on the families of underserved students. Larger and more representative samples will provide a more complete understanding of multifinality and equifinality processes that underlie adaptive functioning across a more diverse set of experiences, and thus can inform the development and implementation of culturally responsive interventions. By highlighting novel extant research and articulating future directions, we hope to stimulate a new wave of research that will advance the science of developmental stress physiology, as well as improve the application of these findings in educational policy and practice. For this to happen, the work needs to be situated in genuine research–practice partnerships where school leaders, teachers, and students collaborate with scientists to design studies that yield actionable results.

Financial Statement

The preparation of this article was supported by a Jacobs Foundation Advanced Research Fellowship grant to Dr. Jelena Obradović and a doctotoral fellowship to Emma Armstrong-Carter funded by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, through Grant R305B140009 to the Board of Trustees of the Leland Stanford Junior University.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

Adam, E. K., Quinn, M. E., Tavernier, R., McQuillan, M. T., Dahlke, K. A., & Gilbert, K. E. (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 2541. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armstrong-Carter, E., Ivory, S., Lin, L. C., Muscatell, K. A., & Telzer, E. H. (2020). Role fulfillment mediates the association between daily family assistance and cortisol awakening response in adolescents. Child Development, 91(3), 754768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badanes, L. S., Dmitrieva, J., & Watamura, S. E. (2012). Understanding cortisol reactivity across the day at child care: The potential buffering role of secure attachments to caregivers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 156165. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.05.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakosh, L. S., Snow, R. M., Tobias, J. M., Houlihan, J. L., & Barbosa-Leiker, C. (2016). Maximizing mindful learning: Mindful awareness intervention improves elementary school students’ quarterly grades. Mindfulness, 7, 5967. doi:10.1007/s12671-015-0387-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, R., Salsberry, P., & Ford, J. (2017). Measuring stress in young children using hair cortisol: The state of the science. Biological Research for Nursing, 19, 499510. doi:10.1177/1099800417711583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, D. R., Carrère, S., Siler, C., Jones, S., Bowie, B., & Cooke, C. (2012). Autonomic regulation on the stroop predicts reading achievement in school age children. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6, 1018. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01130.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benner, A. D., Wang, Y., Shen, Y., Boyle, A. E., Polk, R., & Cheng, Y.-P. (2018). Racial/ethnic discrimination and well-being during adolescence: A meta-analytic review. American Psychologist, 73, 855883. doi:10.1037/amp0000204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernard, K., Peloso, E., Laurenceau, J.-P., Zhang, Z., & Dozier, M. (2015). Examining change in cortisol patterns during the 10-week transition to a new child-care setting. Child Development, 86, 456471. doi:10.1111/cdev.12304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, D., Blair, C., Granger, D. A., & The Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2016). Child care and cortisol across infancy and toddlerhood: Poverty, peers, and developmental timing. Family Relations, 65, 5172. doi:10.1111/fare.12184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on children's executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Review, 30, 331351. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, D. S., & Fernando, R. (2014). Mindfulness training and classroom behavior among lower-income and ethnic minority elementary school children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 12421246. doi:10.1007/s10826-013-9784-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., Berry, D., & The Family Life Project Investigators, (2017). Moderate within-person variability in cortisol is related to executive function in early childhood. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 81, 8895. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.03.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., Granger, D., & Razza, R. (2005). Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development, 76, 554567. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00863.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., & Peters, R. (2003). Physiological and neurocognitive correlates of adaptive behavior in preschool among children in Head Start. Developmental Neuropsychology, 42, 479497. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2401_04CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C., & Raver, C. (2012). Individual development and evolution: Experiential canalization of self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48, 647657. doi:10.1037/a0026472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., Raver, C. C., Granger, D., Mills-Koonce, R., Hibel, L., & The Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2011). Allostasis and allostatic load in the context of poverty in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 845857. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000344CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottiani, J. H., Bradshaw, C. P., & Gregory, A. (2018). Nudging the gap: Introduction to the special issue “Closing in on discipline disproportionality.”. School Psychology Review, 47, 109117. doi:10.17105/SPR-2018-0023.V47-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradshaw, C. P., Pas, E. T., Bottiani, J. H., Debnam, K. J., Reinke, W. M., Herman, K. C., & Rosenberg, M. S. (2018). Promoting cultural responsivity and student engagement through double check coaching of classroom teachers: An efficacy study. School Psychology Review, 47, 118134. doi:10.17105/SPR-2017-0119.V47-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brady, S. T., Hard, B. M., & Gross, J. J. (2018). Reappraising test anxiety increases academic performance of first-year college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 395406. doi:10.1037/edu0000219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brenner, S. L., Beauchaine, T. P., & Sylvers, P. D. (2005). A comparison of psychophysiological and self-report measures of BAS and BIS activation. Psychophysiology, 42, 108115. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00261.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Lei, M. K., Chen, E., & Miller, G. E. (2014). Neighborhood poverty and allostatic load in African American youth. Pediatrics, 134, 13621368. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-1395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Miller, G. E., Yu, T., Beach, S. R. H., & Chen, E. (2016). Supportive family environments ameliorate the link between racial discrimination and epigenetic aging: A replication across two longitudinal cohorts. Psychological Science, 27, 530541. doi:10.1177/0956797615626703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, E. D., Garnett, M. L., Anderson, K. E., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2017). Can the arts get under the skin? Arts and cortisol for economically disadvantaged children. Child Development, 88, 13681381. doi:10.1111/cdev.12652CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C. S., Mistry, R. S., & Yip, T. (2019). Moving from the margins to the mainstream: Equity and justice as key considerations for developmental science. Child Development Perspectives, 13, 235240. doi:10.1111/cdep.12340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C. S., & Tam, M. (2019). Ethnic discrimination predicting academic attitudes for Latinx students in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 65, 101061. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2019.101061CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, J., Davis, E. P., & Gunnar, M. R. (2002). Individual differences in children's cortisol response to the beginning of a new school year. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 635650. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00031-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, J., Fisher, P. A., Pears, K. C., & Levine, S. (2009). Morning cortisol levels in preschool-aged foster children: Differential effects of maltreatment type. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 1423. doi:10.1002/dev.20333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budde, H., Voelcker-Rehage, C., Pietrassyk-Kendziorra, S., Machado, S., Ribeiro, P., & Arafat, A. M. (2010). Steroid hormones in the saliva of adolescents after different exercise intensities and their influence on working memory in a school setting. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 382391. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.07.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, N. R., Obradović, J., Adler, N., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Kindergarten stressors and cumulative adrenocortical activation: The “first straws” of allostatic load? Development and Psychopathology, 23, 10891106. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000514CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., Graziano, P. A., & Keane, S. P. (2007). Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for behavior problems. Biological Psychology, 74, 144153. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Donzella, B., Krueger, W. K., & Gunnar, M. R. (1999). The start of a new school year: Individual differences in salivary cortisol response in relation to child temperament. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 188196. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199911)35:3 < 188::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-K3.0.CO;2-K>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. L., & Levine, L. J. (2013). Emotion regulation strategies that promote learning: Reappraisal enhances children's memory for educational information. Child Development, 84, 361374. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01836.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. L., Quiñones-Camacho, L. E., & Buss, K. A. (2016). The effects of distraction and reappraisal on children's parasympathetic regulation of sadness and fear. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 344358. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCicco, J. M., O'Toole, L. J., & Dennis, T. A. (2014). The late positive potential as a neural signature for cognitive reappraisal in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39, 497515. doi:10.1080/87565641.2014.959171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, T. A., Buss, K. A., & Hastings, P. D. (2012). Physiological measures of emotion from a developmental perspective: State of the science. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77, 15. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00654.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dettling, A. C., Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (1999). Cortisol levels of young children in full-day childcare centers: Relations with age and temperament. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24, 519536. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00009-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dettling, A. C., Parker, S. W., Lane, S., Sebanc, A., & Gunnar, M. R. (2000). Quality of care and temperament determine changes in cortisol concentrations over the day for young children in childcare. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 819836. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00028-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnelly, J. E., & Lambourne, K. (2011). Classroom-based physical activity, cognition, and academic achievement. Preventive Medicine, 52, 3642. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dusenbury, L., & Weissberg, R. P. (2017). Social emotional learning in elementary school: Preparation for success. Retrieved from Collaborative for academic, social, and emotional learning (CASEL) website: www.casel.orgGoogle Scholar
El-Sheikh, M., Hinnant, J. B., & Erath, S. (2011). Developmental trajectories of delinquency symptoms in childhood: The role of marital conflict and autonomic nervous system activity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 1632. doi:10.1037/a0020626CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El-Sheikh, M., Kouros, C. D., Erath, S., Cummings, E. M., Keller, P. S., & Staton, L. (2009). Marital conflict and children's externalizing behavior: Pathways involving interactions between parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous system activity. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74, 779. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00501.xGoogle Scholar
El-Sheikh, M., Philbrook, L. E., Kelly, R. J., Hinnant, J. B., & Buckhalt, J. A. (2019). What does a good night's sleep mean? Nonlinear relations between sleep and children's cognitive functioning and mental health. Sleep, 42, 115. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsz078Google ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 723. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erath, S. A., Bub, K. L., & Tu, K. M. (2016). Responses to peer stress predict academic outcomes across the transition to middle school. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 36, 528. doi:10.1177/0272431614556350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. W., & Fuller-Rowell, T. E. (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and young adult working memory: The protective role of self-regulatory capacity. Developmental Science, 16, 688696. doi:10.1111/desc.12082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. W., & Schamberg, M. A. (2009). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 65456549. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811910106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. (2017). The neuroscience of socioeconomic status: Correlates, causes, and consequences. Neuron, 96, 5671. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishbein, D., Miller, S., Herman-Stahl, M., Williams, J., Lavery, B., Markovitz, L., … Johnson, M. (2016). Behavioral and psychophysiological effects of a yoga intervention on high-risk adolescents: A randomized control trial. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 518529. doi:10.1007/s10826-015-0231-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuhs, M. W., Nesbitt, K. T., Farran, D. C., & Dong, N. (2014). Longitudinal associations between executive functioning and academic skills across content areas. Developmental Psychology, 50, 16981709. doi:10.1037/a0036633CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallo, L. C., Roesch, S. C., Bravin, J. I., Savin, K. L., Perreira, K. M., Carnethon, M. R., … Isasi, C. R. (2019). Socioeconomic adversity, social resources, and allostatic load among Hispanic/Latino youth: The study of Latino youth. Psychosomatic Medicine, 81, 305312. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000668CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, E. B., Sulik, M. J., & Obradović, J. (2019). Teachers’ perceptions of students’ executive functions: Disparities by gender, ethnicity, and ELL status. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111, 918931. doi:10.1037/edu0000308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gates, K. M., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Sandsten, M., & Blandon, A. Y. (2015). Estimating time-varying RSA to examine psychophysiological linkage of marital dyads. Psychophysiology, 52, 10591065. doi:10.1111/psyp.12428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentzler, A. L., Santucci, A. K., Kovacs, M., & Fox, N. A. (2009). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia predicts emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in at-risk and control children. Biological Psychology, 82, 156163. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerber, M., Endes, K., Brand, S., Herrmann, C., Colledge, F., Donath, L., … Zahner, L. (2017). In 6- to 8-year-old children, hair cortisol is associated with body mass index and somatic complaints, but not with stress, health-related quality of life, blood pressure, retinal vessel diameters, and cardiorespiratory fitness. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 76, 110. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geršak, V., Vitulić, H. S., Prosen, S., Starc, G., Humar, I., & Geršak, G. (2020). Use of wearable devices to study activity of children in classroom; Case study–Learning geometry using movement. Computer Communications, 150, 581588. doi:10.1016/j.comcom.2019.12.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilissen, R., Koolstra, C. M., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van der Veer, R. (2007). Physiological reactions of preschoolers to fear-inducing film clips: Effects of temperamental fearfulness and quality of the parent–child relationship. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 187195.10.1002/dev.20188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giuliano, R. J., Karns, C. M., Roos, L. E., Bell, T. A., Petersen, S., Skowron, E. A., … Pakulak, E. (2018). Effects of early adversity on neural mechanisms of distractor suppression are mediated by sympathetic nervous system activity in preschool-aged children. Developmental Psychology, 54, 16741686. doi:10.1037/dev0000499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granger, D. A., Kivlighan, K. T., El-Sheikh, M., Gordis, E. B., & Stroud, L. R. (2007). Salivary alpha-amylase in biobehavioral research: Recent developments and applications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1098, 122144. doi:10.1196/annals.1384.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graves, K. Y., & Nowakowski, A. C. H. (2017). Childhood socioeconomic status and stress in late adulthood: A longitudinal approach to measuring allostatic load. Global Pediatric Health, 4, 112. doi:10.1177/2333794X17744950CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, P., & Derefinko, K. (2013). Cardiac vagal control and children's adaptive functioning: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 94, 2237. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, A., & Fergus, E. (2017). Social and emotional learning and equity in school discipline. The Future of Children, 27, 117136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregoski, M. J., Barnes, V. A., Tingen, M. S., Harshfield, G. A., & Treiber, F. A. (2011). Breathing awareness meditation and life skills training programs influence upon ambulatory blood pressure and sodium excretion among African American adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48, 5964. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.05.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Doom, J. R., & Esposito, E. A.. (2015). Psychoneuroendocrinology of stress: Normative development and individual differences. In Lerner, R. M., & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, socioemotional processes (Vol. 3, pp. 106151). Somerset, NJ: John Wiley & Sons IncGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Kryzer, E., Van Ryzin, M. J., & Phillips, D. A. (2010). The rise in cortisol in family day care: Associations with aspects of care quality, child behavior, and child sex. Child Development, 81, 851869. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01438.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18, 686702. doi:10.1111/desc.12246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J., & Lindorff, A. (2017). Children's transition to school: Relationships between preschool attendance, cortisol patterns, and effortful control. The Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 34, 118. doi:10.1017/edp.2017.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastings, P. D., Sullivan, C., McShane, K. E., Coplan, R. J., Utendale, W. T., & Vyncke, J. D. (2008). Parental socialization, vagal regulation, and preschoolers’ anxious difficulties: Direct mothers and moderated fathers. Child Development, 79, 4564. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01110.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatfield, B. E., Hestenes, L. L., Kintner-Duffy, V. L., & O'Brien, M. (2013). Classroom emotional support predicts differences in preschool children's cortisol and alpha-amylase levels. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 347356. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatfield, B. E., & Williford, A. P. (2017). Cortisol patterns for young children displaying disruptive behavior: Links to a teacher-child, relationship-focused intervention. Prevention Science, 18, 4049. doi:10.1007/s11121-016-0693-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heissel, J. A., Adam, E. K., Doleac, J. L., Figlio, D. N., & Meer, J. (2019). Testing, stress, and performance: How students respond physiologically to high-stakes testing. Education Finance and Policy, 3, 150. doi:10.1162/edfp_a_00306.Google Scholar
Heissel, J. A., Levy, D. J., & Adam, E. K. (2017). Stress, sleep, and performance on standardized tests: Understudied pathways to the achievement gap. AERA Open, 3, 117. doi:10.1177/2332858417713488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heissel, J. A., Sharkey, P. T., Torrats-Espinosa, G., Grant, K., & Adam, E. K. (2018). Violence and vigilance: The acute effects of community violent crime on sleep and cortisol. Child Development, 89, 323331. doi:10.1111/cdev.12889CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinnant, J. B., & El-Sheikh, M. (2013). Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing symptoms in middle to late childhood: Sex, baseline respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity as predictors. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 419436. doi:10.1017/S0954579412001150CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinnant, J. B., El-Sheikh, M., Keiley, M., & Buckhalt, J. A. (2013). Marital conflict, allostatic load, and the development of children's fluid cognitive performance. Child Development, 84, 20032014. doi:10.1111/cdev.12103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinnant, J. B., Philbrook, L. E., Erath, S. A., & El-Sheikh, M. (2018). Approaches to modeling the development of physiological stress responsivity. Psychophysiology, 55, e13027. doi:10.1111/psyp.13027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostinar, C. E., & Miller, G. E. (2019). Protective factors for youth confronting economic hardship: Current challenges and future avenues in resilience research. American Psychologist, 74, 641652. doi:10.1037/amp0000520CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, A. L., Minnis, H., & Wilson, P. (2011). Altered stress responses in children exposed to early adversity: A systematic review of salivary cortisol studies. Stress, 14, 614626. doi:10.3109/10253890.2011.577848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 208212. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamieson, J. P., Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Mind over matter: Reappraising arousal improves cardiovascular and cognitive responses to stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 417422. doi:10.1037/a0025719CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, A. E., Perry, N. B., Hostinar, C. E., & Gunnar, M. R. (2019). Cognitive–affective strategies and cortisol stress reactivity in children and adolescents: Normative development and effects of early life stress. Developmental Psychobiology, 61, 9991013. doi:10.1002/dev.21849CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahle, S., Miller, J. G., Helm, J. L., & Hastings, P. D. (2018). Linking autonomic physiology and emotion regulation in preschoolers: The role of reactivity and recovery. Developmental Psychobiology, 60, 775788. doi:10.1002/dev.21746CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalvin, C. B., Bierman, K. L., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2016). Emotional reactivity, behavior problems, and social adjustment at school entry in a high-risk sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 15271541. doi:10.1007/s10802-016-0139-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, P. S., El-Sheikh, M., Granger, D. A., & Buckhalt, J. A. (2012). Interactions between salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase as predictors of children's cognitive functioning and academic performance. Physiology & Behavior, 105, 987995. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, P., Evans, G. W., Angstadt, M., Ho, S. S., Sripada, C. S., Swain, J. E., … Phan, K. L. (2013). Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 1844218447. doi:10.1073/pnas.1308240110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliewer, W. (2006). Violence exposure and cortisol responses in urban youth. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 13, 109120. doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm1302_2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lafko, N., Murray-Close, D., & Shoulberg, E. K. (2015). Negative peer status and relational victimization in children and adolescents: The role of stress physiology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44, 405416. doi:10.1080/15374416.2013.850701CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambe, L. J., Craig, W. M., & Hollenstein, T. (2019). Blunted physiological stress reactivity among youth with a history of bullying and victimization: Links to depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47, 19811993. doi:10.1007/s10802-019-00565-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurito, A., Lacoe, J., Schwartz, A. L., Sharkey, P., & Ellen, I. G. (2019). School climate and the impact of neighborhood crime on test scores. RSF: The Russell Sage Foundation Journal of the Social Sciences, 5, 141166. doi:10.7758/rsf.2019.5.2.08CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, H. Y., Jamieson, J. P., Miu, A. S., Josephs, R. A., & Yeager, D. S. (2019). An entity theory of intelligence predicts higher cortisol levels when high school grades are declining. Child Development, 90, 849867. doi:10.1111/cdev.13116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisonbee, J. A., Mize, J., Payne, A. L., & Granger, D. A. (2008). Children's cortisol and the quality of teacher-child relationships in child care. Child Development, 79, 18181832. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01228.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
London, R. (2019). The right to play: Eliminating the opportunity gap in elementary school recess. Phi Delta Kappan, 101, 4852. doi:org/10.1177/0031721719885921CrossRefGoogle Scholar
London, R. A., Westrich, L., Stokes-Guinan, K., & McLaughlin, M. (2015). Playing fair: The contribution of high-functioning recess to overall school climate in low-income elementary schools. Journal of School Health, 85, 5360. doi:10.1111/josh.12216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 531552. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.531CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 434445. doi:10.1038/nrn2639CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malanchini, M., Engelhardt, L. E., Raffington, L. A., Sabhlok, A., Grotzinger, A. D., Briley, D. A., … Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2020). Weak and uneven associations of home, neighborhood, and school environments with stress hormone output across multiple timescales. Molecular Psychiatry, 117(6), 11641188. doi:10.1038/s41380-020-0747-z.Google Scholar
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341, 976980. doi:10.1126/science.1238041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcovitch, S., Leigh, J., Calkins, S. D., Leerks, E. M., O'Brien, M., & Blankson, A. N. (2010). Moderate vagal withdrawal in 3.5-year-old children is associated with optimal performance on executive function tasks. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 603608. doi:10.1002/dev.20462CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCoy, D. C., Raver, C. C., & Sharkey, P. (2015). Children's cognitive performance and selective attention following recent community violence. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 56, 1936. doi:10.1177/0022146514567576CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 3344. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Alves, S., & Sheridan, M. A. (2014). Vagal regulation and internalizing psychopathology among adolescents exposed to childhood adversity: Vagal regulation and stress vulnerability. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 10361051. doi:10.1002/dev.21187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merz, E. C., Wiltshire, C. A., & Noble, K. G. (2019). Socioeconomic inequality and the developing brain: Spotlight on language and executive function. Child Development Perspectives, 13, 1520. doi:10.1111/cdep.12305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, E. M., Dmitrieva, J., Hurwich-Reiss, E., Badanes, L., Mendoza, M. M., Perreira, K. M., … Watamura, S. E. (2018). Evidence for a physiologic home–school gap in children of Latina immigrants. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 52, 86100. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.03.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray-Close, D. (2013). Psychophysiology of adolescent peer relations I: Theory and research findings. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 23, 236259. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2012.00828.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberle, E. (2018). Social-emotional competence and early adolescents’ peer acceptance in school: Examining the role of afternoon cortisol. Plos One, 13. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0192639CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberle, E., McLachlan, K., Catherine, N. L. A., Brain, U., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Weinberg, J., & Oberlander, T. F. (2017). Afternoon cortisol provides a link between self-regulated anger and peer-reported aggression in typically developing children in the school context. Developmental Psychobiology, 59, 688695. doi:10.1002/dev.21522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberle, E., & Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (2016). Stress contagion in the classroom? The link between classroom teacher burnout and morning cortisol in elementary school students. Social Science & Medicine, 159, 3037. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.04.031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obradović, J. (2012). How can the study of physiological reactivity contribute to our understanding of adversity and resilience processes in development? Development and Psychopathology, 24, 371387. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obradović, J. (2016). Physiological responsivity and executive functioning: Implications for adaptation and resilience in early childhood. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 6570. doi:10.1111/cdep.12164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obradović, J., & Boyce, T. (2011). Physiological reactivity moderates the effects of social hierarchy position on adaptation of kindergarten children. Presented at the Biannual meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
Obradović, J., & Boyce, W. T. (2009). Individual differences in behavioral, physiological, and genetic sensitivities to contexts: Implications for development and adaptation. Developmental Neuroscience, 31, 300308. doi:10.1159/000216541Google ScholarPubMed
Obradović, J., & Boyce, W. T. (2012). Stress reactivity in child development research. In Mayes, L., & Lewis, M. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of environment in human development (pp. 655681). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press..CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obradović, J., Bush, N. R., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N. E., & Boyce, W. T. (2010). Biological sensitivity to context: The interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on socioemotional behavior and school readiness. Child Development, 81, 270289. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01394.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obradović, J., Portilla, X. A., & Ballard, P. J. (2016). Biological sensitivity to family income: Differential effects on early executive functioning. Child Development, 87, 374384. doi:10.1111/cdev.12475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okonofua, J. A., Walton, G. M., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2016). A vicious cycle: A social–psychological account of extreme racial disparities in school discipline. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 381398. doi:10.1177/1745691616635592CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ouellet-Morin, I., Odgers, C. L., Danese, A., Bowes, L., Shakoor, S., Papadopoulos, A. S., … Arseneault, L. (2011). Blunted cortisol responses to stress signal social and behavioral problems among maltreated/bullied 12-year-old children. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 10161023. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overstreet, S., & Chafouleas, S. M. (2016). Trauma-informed schools: Introduction to the special issue. School Mental Health, 8, 16. doi:10.1007/s12310-016-9184-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, K. B., Parker, P. D., Van Zanden, B., MacMillan, F., Astell-Burt, T., & Lonsdale, C. (2016). Physical activity and school engagement in youth: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Educational Psychologist, 51, 129145. doi:10.1080/00461520.2016.1151793CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, F. B., Anand, K. J. S., Graff, J. C., Murphy, L. E., Qu, Y., Völgyi, E., … Tylavsky, F. A. (2013). Early adversity, socioemotional development, and stress in urban 1-year-old children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 163, 17331739. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.08.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parent, S., Lupien, S., Herba, C. M., Dupéré, V., Gunnar, M. R., & Séguin, J. R. (2019). Children's cortisol response to the transition from preschool to formal schooling: A review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 99, 196205. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.09.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parlak, O., Keene, S. T., Marais, A., Curto, V. F., & Salleo, A. (2018). Molecularly selective nanoporous membrane-based wearable organic electrochemical device for noninvasive cortisol sensing. Science Advances, 4, 28882904. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aar2904CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascoe, M. C., Thompson, D. R., & Ski, C. F. (2017). Yoga, mindfulness-based stress reduction and stress-related physiological measures: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 86, 152168. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017. 08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, L. A., Hibel, L. C., Granger, D., Tsao, J., & Zeltzer, L. (2014). Relationship of salivary alpha amylase and cortisol to social anxiety in healthy children undergoing laboratory pain tasks. Journal of Child and Adolescent Behaviour, 2, 112. doi:10.4172/2375-4494.1000129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philbrook, L. E., Buckhalt, J. A., & El-Sheikh, M. (2019). Community violence concerns and adolescent sleep: Physiological regulation and race as moderators. Journal of Sleep Research, 29, e12897. doi:10.1111/jsr.12897.Google ScholarPubMed
Philbrook, L. E., Shimizu, M., Buckhalt, J. A., & El-Sheikh, M. (2018). Sleepiness as a pathway linking race and socioeconomic status with academic and cognitive outcomes in middle childhood. Sleep Health, 4, 405412. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2018.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piccolo, L. D. R., Sbicigo, J. B., Grassi-Oliveira, R., & Fumagalli de Salles, J. (2014). Do socioeconomic status and stress reactivity really impact neurocognitive performance? Psychology & Neuroscience, 7, 567575. doi:10.3922/j.psns.2014.4.16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74, 116143. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Portilla, X. A., Ballard, P. J., Adler, N. E., Boyce, W. T., & Obradović, J. (2014). An integrative view of school functioning: Transactions between self-regulation, school engagement, and teacher-child relationship quality. Child Development, 85, 19151931. doi:10.1111/cdev.12259Google ScholarPubMed
Quas, J. A., Bauer, A., & Boyce, W. T. (2004). Physiological reactivity, social support, and memory in early childhood. Child Development, 75, 797814. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00707.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quas, J. A., Carrick, N., Alkon, A., Goldstein, L., & Boyce, W. T. (2006). Children's memory for a mild stressor: The role of sympathetic activation and parasympathetic withdrawal. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 686702. doi:10.1002/dev.20184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffington, L., Schmiedek, F., Heim, C., & Shing, Y. L. (2018). Cognitive control moderates parenting stress effects on children's diurnal cortisol. PLoS One, 13, 191215. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0191215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raver, C. C., Blair, C., & Willoughby, M. T. (2013). Poverty as a predictor of 4-year-olds’ executive function: New perspectives on models of differential susceptibility. Developmental Psychology, 49, 292304. doi:10.1037/a0028343CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reardon. (2019). Educational opportunity in early and middle childhood: Using full population administrative data to study variation by place and age. RSF: The Russell Sage Foundation Journal of the Social Sciences, 5, 4068. doi:10.7758/rsf.2019.5.2.03CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rippe, R. C. A., Noppe, G., Windhorst, D. A., Tiemeier, H., van Rossum, E. F. C., Jaddoe, V. W. V., … van den Akker, E. L. T. (2016). Splitting hair for cortisol? Associations of socio-economic status, ethnicity, hair color, gender and other child characteristics with hair cortisol and cortisone. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 66, 5664. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rocque, M. (2010). Office discipline and student behavior: Does race matter? American Journal of Education, 116, 557581. doi:10.1086/653629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roubinov, D. S., Hagan, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Essex, M. J., & Bush, N. R. (2017). Child temperament and teacher relationship interactively predict cortisol expression: The prism of classroom climate. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 17631775. doi:10.1017/S0954579417001389CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott, D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T. F., & Diamond, A. (2015). Enhancing cognitive and social–emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: A randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 51, 5266. doi:10.1037/a0038454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharkey, P. (2010). The acute effect of local homicides on children's cognitive performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 1173311738. doi:10.1073/pnas.1000690107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharkey, P., Schwartz, A. E., Ellen, I. G., & Lacoe, J. (2014). High stakes in the classroom, high stakes on the street: The effects of community violence on student's standardized test performance. Sociological Science, 1, 199220. doi:10.15195/v1.a14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharkey, P. T., Tirado-Strayer, N., Papachristos, A. V., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The effect of local violence on children's attention and impulse control. American Journal of Public Health, 102, 22872293. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2012.300789CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibinga, E. M. S., Webb, L., Ghazarian, S. R., & Ellen, J. M. (2016). School-based mindfulness instruction: An RCT. Pediatrics, 137, 20152532. doi:10.1542/peds.2015-2532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skowron, E. A., Cipriano-Essel, E., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Teti, D. M., & Ammerman, R. T. (2014). Early adversity, RSA, and inhibitory control: Evidence of children's neurobiological sensitivity to social context. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 964978. doi:10.1002/dev.21175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slopen, N., McLaughlin, K. A., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2014). Interventions to improve cortisol regulation in children: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 133, 312326. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-1632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., Hentges, R. F., & Coe, J. L. (2016). Family instability and children's effortful control in the context of poverty: Sometimes a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 112. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000407Google Scholar
Suor, J. H., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., & Manning, L. G. (2015). Tracing differential pathways of risk: Associations among family adversity, cortisol, and cognitive functioning in childhood. Child Development, 86, 11421158. doi:10.1111/cdev.12376CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2006). Child maltreatment and the developing HPA axis. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 632639. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarullo, A. R., Mliner, S., & Gunnar, M. R. (2011). Inhibition and exuberance in preschool classrooms: Associations with peer social experiences and changes in cortisol across the preschool year. Developmental Psychology, 47, 13741388. doi:10.1037/a0024093CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tenenbaum, H. R., & Ruck, M. D. (2007). Are teachers’ expectations different for racial minority than for European American students? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 253273. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tine, M. T., & Butler, A. G. (2012). Acute aerobic exercise impacts selective attention: An exceptional boost in lower-income children. Educational Psychology, 32, 821834. doi:10.1080/01443410.2012.723612CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tout, K., de Haan, M., Campbell, E. K., & Gunnar, M. R. (1998). Social behavior correlates of cortisol activity in child care: Gender differences and time-of-day effects. Child Development, 69, 1247. doi:10.2307/1132263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner-Cobb, J. M., Rixon, L., & Jessop, D. S. (2008). A prospective study of diurnal cortisol responses to the social experience of school transition in four-year-old children: Anticipation, exposure, and adaptation. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 377389. doi:10.1002/dev.20298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ursache, A., Merz, E. C., Melvin, S., Meyer, J., & Noble, K. G. (2017). Socioeconomic status, hair cortisol and internalizing symptoms in parents and children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 78, 142150. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaghri, Z., Guhn, M., Weinberg, J., Grunau, R. E., Yu, W., & Hertzman, C. (2013). Hair cortisol reflects socio-economic factors and hair zinc in preschoolers. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 331340. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.06.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2013). The biological underpinnings of peer victimization: Understanding why and how the effects of bullying can last a lifetime. Theory into Practice, 52, 241248. doi:10.1080/00405841.2013.829726CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Langenberg, G., Brandsma, R., Oort, F. J., & Bögels, S. M. (2012). The effectiveness of a school-based mindfulness training as a program to prevent stress in elementary school children. Mindfulness, 5, 238248. doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0171-9Google Scholar
Vliegenthart, J., Noppe, G., van Rossum, E. F. C., Koper, J. W., Raat, H., & van den Akker, E. L. T. (2016). Socioeconomic status in children is associated with hair cortisol levels as a biological measure of chronic stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 65, 914. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.11.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, S. L., Cepeda, I., Krieger, D., Maggi, S., D'Angiulli, A., Weinberg, J., & Grunau, R. E. (2016). Higher cortisol is associated with poorer executive functioning in preschool children: The role of parenting stress, parent coping and quality of daycare. Child Neuropsychology, 22, 853869. doi:10.1080/09297049.2015.1080232CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watamura, S. E., Kryzer, E. M., & Robertson, S. S. (2009). Cortisol patterns at home and child care: Afternoon differences and evening recovery in children attending very high quality full-day center-based child care. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 475485. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, D. K., Kliewer, W., Plybon, L., & Sica, D. A. (2000). Socioeconomic status and blood pressure reactivity in healthy black adolescents. Hypertension, 35, 496500. doi:10.1161/01.HYP.35.1.496CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, P.-J., Lamb, M. E., Kappler, G., & Ahnert, L. (2017). Children's diurnal cortisol activity during the first year of school. Applied Developmental Science, 21, 3041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, J. C., Mun, J., Kwon, S. Y., Park, S., Bao, Z., & Park, S. (2019). Electronic skin: Recent progress and future prospects for skin-attachable devices for health monitoring, robotics, and prosthetics. Advanced Materials, 31, 150. doi:10.1002/adma.201904765Google ScholarPubMed
Yip, T., Cham, H., Wang, Y., & El-Sheikh, M. (2020). Discrimination and sleep mediate ethnic/racial identity and adolescent adjustment: Uncovering change processes with slope-as-mediator mediation. Child Development, 91(3), 10211043. https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111/cdev.13276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., Forston, J. L., Masten, A. S., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). Mindfulness plus reflection training: Effects on executive function in early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 208. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmermann, C. R. (2018). The penalty of being a young black girl: Kindergarten teachers’ perceptions of children's problem behaviors and student–teacher conflict by the intersection of Race and Gender. The Journal of Negro Education, 87, 154168. doi:10.7709/jnegroeducation.87.2.0154CrossRefGoogle Scholar