Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T12:35:56.181Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship between olfactory function and social cognition in euthymic bipolar patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 June 2013

Guillermo Lahera*
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine and Medical Specialties (Psychiatry), University of Alcala, Madrid, Spain
Salvador Ruiz-Murugarren
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Príncipe de Asturias University Hospital, Alcala, Madrid, Spain
Alberto Fernández-Liria
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine and Medical Specialties (Psychiatry), University of Alcala, Madrid, Spain Department of Psychiatry, Príncipe de Asturias University Hospital, Alcala, Madrid, Spain
Jerónimo Saiz-Ruiz
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine and Medical Specialties (Psychiatry), University of Alcala, Madrid, Spain Department of Psychiatry, Ramón y Cajal University Hospital, Mental Health National Net IRYCIS: CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain
Benjamin E. Buck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill, NC, USA
David L. Penn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill, NC, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: Guillermo Lahera, Lope de Rueda 3, 28009 Madrid, Spain. (Email guillermo.lahera@uah.es)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective/Introduction

There is a close functional and neuroanatomical relationship between olfactory ability and emotional processing. The present study seeks to explore the association between olfactory ability and social cognition, especially facial emotion perception, in euthymic bipolar patients.

Methods

Thirty-nine euthymic outpatients meeting DSM-IV-TR criteria for bipolar disorder and 40 healthy volunteers matched on socio-demographic criteria were recruited. Both groups were assessed at one time point with the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT), the Emotion Recognition Test, and The Faux Pas Recognition Test, as well as measures of general cognition and functioning.

Results

The bipolar patients showed a significant impairment in olfactory identification (UPSIT) and social cognition measures compared to healthy controls. Analyses revealed significant relationships between olfactory identification and facial emotion recognition, theory of mind, general cognition, and a trend-level relationship with functioning. Controlling for age and cigarettes smoked, relationships remained significant between olfactory function and facial emotion recognition.

Conclusion

There is a deficit of olfactory identification in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder that is correlated with a deficit in both verbal and non-verbal measures of social cognition.

Type
Original Research
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Introduction

Olfactory function, which is typically understood as being composed of the two independent hierarchical processes of odor detection, as well as odor discrimination and encoding,Reference Martzke, Kopala and Good 1 is suspected to be a marker of cognitive impairment that is associated with various psychiatric conditions.Reference Atanasova, Graux and El Hage 2 A range of cognitive domains have been suggested as being closely connected on a functional or neuroanatomical level to the olfactory system, including memory, emotion processing, and social functioning.Reference Kivity, Ortega-Hernandez and Shoenfeld 3

These patterns have been explored at length in an attempt to better understand the cognitive impairments associated with many psychiatric disorders. Olfactory function has been found to be impaired in persons with schizophrenia,Reference Kopala, Clark and Hurwitz 4 Reference Moberg, Arnold and Doty 7 seasonal affective disorder,Reference Postolache, Doty and Wehr 8 , Reference Postolache, Wehr and Doty 9 and major depressive disorder.Reference Pause, Miranda, Göder, Aldenhoff and Ferstl 10 , Reference Lombion-Pouthier, Vandel, Nezelof, Haffen and Millot 11

Until recently, research on olfactory function in bipolar disorder had been relatively neglected, in part due to early findings about the relatively intact olfactory function of persons with bipolar disorder compared to other disorders such as schizophrenia,Reference Hurwitz, Kopala, Clark and Jones 12 as well as other studies that showed contradictory results.Reference Amsterdam, Settle, Doty and Abelman 13 Reference Swiecicki, Zatorski and Bzinkowska 17 More recent studies, however, have shown olfactory dysfunction in a bipolar population as well. Odor identification deficits have been found in patients with affective psychoses, beginning with the first episode and independent of treatment.Reference Brewer, Pantelis and Anderson 18 Cumming etal.Reference Cumming, Matthews and Park 19 found that persons with bipolar disorder showed deficits in odor identification, and that this deficit was significantly related to social competence. Similarly, Hardy etal.Reference Hardy, Rosedale and Messinger 20 have also found that in a bipolar sample, increased odor sensitivity was related to depressive symptoms, and decreased odor sensitivity was related to manic symptoms, as well as poor employment, social avoidance, and social fear.

As this area has developed, attention has turned toward the relationships found by the aforementioned studies between olfactory function and social functioning. As there is a close functional and neuroanatomical relationship between olfactory ability and the regulation of emotions,Reference Millot and Brand 21 Reference Ludvigson and Rottman 23 it could be the case that olfactory dysfunction is a particularly strong marker for cognitive impairments associated with or underlying social functioning. This could be a result of neuroanatomical connections between the olfactory system and systems underlying social functioning. Some researchers attribute this bidirectional interaction to the connections between olfactory pathways and the limbic system, whereas others note that both functions require the integrity of common neural substrates, such as the orbitofrontal, temporal, and entorhinal cortices, as well as the amygdala.Reference Pitkänen, Pikkarainen, Nurminen and Ylinen 24 Reference Savic 27

Social cognition has been defined as “how individuals think about themselves, others, social situations, and their interactions,”Reference Penn, Spaulding and Reed 28 and elsewhere in developmental and animal model research as, “the cognitive ability to infer mental states to oneself and to others in terms of thought, emotion, and intention, which makes it possible to predict the behavior of others and to understand the social information of the surroundings.”Reference Baron-Cohen, Leslie and Frith 29

While work on social cognition in bipolar disorder is still limited, recent years have seen growth in this area. In particular, theory of mind (a subdomain of social cognition) is impaired in people with bipolar disorder, not only in symptomatic phases, but also in euthymia.Reference Kerr, Dunbar and Bentall 30 Reference Martino, Strejilevich, Fassi, Marengo and Igoa 33 Lahera etal.Reference Lahera, Ruiz-Murugarren and Iglesias 34 have shown that social cognition deficits are more pronounced in low-functioning persons with bipolar disorder, and that therefore social cognition could partially predict the relationship between clinical variables and functioning.

In the present study, we examine olfactory function and social cognition deficits in bipolar disorder. First, we evaluated group differences between the bipolar group and control group, hypothesizing that the bipolar group will exhibit deficits in olfactory function, specifically odor recognition. Second, we examined the possibility that olfactory function is a marker of social cognition deficits associated with bipolar disorder by examining the relationship between odor identification and two subdomains of social cognition—emotion perception and theory of mind—in a sample of euthymic bipolar patients. Finally, we also examine relationships of smell identification with general cognition and functioning, and we predict that olfactory performance is also significantly correlated with measures in each of those domains.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-nine euthymic outpatients fulfilling Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) criteria for bipolar disorder type I or II (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) 35 were recruited from two different mental health centers in Madrid, Spain. Diagnoses were confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR. Patients were required to be in state of euthymia, defined as a score lower than 8 on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) and a score lower than 6 on the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) sustained during the last 3 months. Participants were required to be between the ages of 18 and 70 years.

We also recruited 40 healthy controls with sociodemographic characteristics similar to those of the study group. Exclusion criteria for both patients and controls were current psychiatric comorbidity in axis I, substance abuse or dependence (in the last year), a history of head injury, neurological illness, mental retardation, electro-convulsive therapy in the last year, and any medical condition that could affect olfactory functioning (eg, common cold or allergies). All the subjects provided written statements of informed consent, and the Ethics Committee of the Príncipe de Asturias Hospital approved the study. Demographic information for both control and patient groups can be found in Table 1.

Table 1 Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of participants

1T-test

2Chi square

Measures

Evaluation of olfactory discrimination

To evaluate olfactory discrimination, we used the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT), a test that has been widely used since 1984. The 40-item UPSIT is a standardized, 4-alternative, forced-choice test of olfactory identification. It is composed of 4 booklets containing 10 odorants each, with 1 odorant per page. The stimuli are embedded in “scratch-and-sniff” microcapsules that are fixed and positioned on strips at the bottom of each page. A multiple-choice question with four response alternatives for each item is located above each odorant strip. The split half reliability of the UPSIT has ranged from .93 to .97, 2-week test-retest reliability has been shown to be .95, and long-term reliability has been shown to be .94.Reference Doty, Shaman and Dann 36

Evaluation of emotion perception

The emotion recognition testReference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright and Jolliffe 37 consists of a series of photographs of facial expressions by the same model depicting 7 basic emotions (eg, happiness, sadness, anger, fear, surprise, disgust, and discomfort) and 9 complex mental states (eg, scheming, guilt, reflection, doubt, admiration, seduction, interest, arrogance, and boredom) for the patient to identify from the photographs. In one-third of the pictures, the entire face is shown; another third shows only the eyes; and in the final third, only the mouth can be seen. All the photographs are randomly ordered, and scores range from 0 to 60, with performance indexed as number correct.

Evaluation of theory of mind

The comprehension faux pas testReference Stone, Baron-Cohen and Knight 38 , Reference Gregory, Lough and Stone 39 is a verbal test that measures the ability to understand socially awkward situations. There are 20 strips, 10 of which describe faux pas situations and 10 of which do not. If a faux pas is identified, 2 clarifying questions are asked: “Why shouldn't the character have said what he or she did?” and “Why do you think he or she did say it?” To comprehend that a faux pas has occurred, the subject has to understand 2 mental states, namely, that the person making the faux pas does not, at that moment, know he or she has done so, and that the other character was offended or hurt by the first character.

Evaluation of general cognition

Asarnow etal.'s span of apprehension test (SAT)Reference Asarnow, Nuechterlein and Marder 40 was used to assess the capacity for sustained attention. This task presents a sequence of target and non-target stimuli at a fixed rate of 1 per second for a short period of 0.1 seconds, so that the presence of the target stimulus must be captured. The computerized version of the test included in the COGLAB Neuropsychological Test BatteryReference Spaulding, Garbin and Dras 41 (Spanish version by Gurpegui and Cerezo)Reference Gurpegui, Alvarez, Bousoño, Ciudad, Carlos Gómez, Olivares and Bousoño 42 was used. The final capacity for sustained attention score ranged from 0 to 30. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)Reference Heaton 43 was used to assess executive function. This test measures the ability to adequately use information to solve problems and to design a course of action. It also measures whether the patient has the cognitive flexibility to change certain criteria to meet the demands of reality (ie, when the patient receives feedback that his/her answers are incorrect). The results are tabulated as the number of categories completed and are based on the number of correct answers, the number of random errors, and the number of perseverative errors.

Functioning

The Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) is a 24-item, interviewer-rated scale of psychosocial functioning with items separated into domains including autonomy, occupational functioning, cognitive functioning, financial issues, interpersonal relationships, and leisure time. Higher scores reflect greater impairment. Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve analyses in previous studies have demonstrated a sore of 11 to be an appropriate cut-point distinguishing bipolar patients from nonclinical controls.Reference Rosa, Sánchez-Moreno and Martínez-Aran 44 To complement the use of the FAST, a re-analysis using the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF, taken from DSM-IV)Reference Heaton 43 was performed as well.

In order to best examine the relationship between smell identification and social cognition, it is also important to note the potential for confounds between these two variables. First, it has been shown that odor and taste identification abilities decline with age,Reference Kaneda, Maeshima and Goto 45 as do cognitive abilities in individuals with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.Reference Green 46 Second, as there is a higher rate of cigarette smoking in bipolar samples,Reference Diaz, James and Botts 47 and smoking has long been known to impair one's olfactory abilities over time,Reference Frye, Schwartz and Doty 48 cigarette smoking remains another potential confounding factor.

Analyses

We used SPSS version 19 for statistical analyses. We tested the normality of the variables using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. To evaluate group differences in olfactory function and emotion perception, we conducted an independent sample t-test comparing the bipolar group and the control group on each of those variables. Faux pas test data were not collected among controls, so they were omitted from the analysis pertaining to group differences. Next, we examined Pearson correlations between the smell identification task and the neurocognitve, social cognitive, and functioning measures. We recomputed the above as partial correlations, while controlling for the influence of age and number of cigarettes smoked per day using linear regression.

To evaluate the relationship between olfactory function and social cognition, we completed two hierarchical regression analyses—one predicting emotion perception and the other predicting performance on a faux pas task. First we entered the following variables at step 1: age and number of smoked cigarettes per day. Then we entered measures of neurocognition at step 2, including Asarnow's SAT and the WCST. Third, we entered the odor identification variable, which was represented by the number of smells correctly identified. F tests were conducted at each step of the hierarchical regression; these tested for the significance of the change in R2 value.

Findings

Sample characteristics

The sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1. The group of patients with bipolar disorder and the control group presented similar sociodemographic characteristics, with a trend toward higher age in bipolar group (mean = 46.82, SD =14.81) than the control group (mean = 39.78, SD = 18.8).

Group differences on olfactory performance and face emotion recognition

There was a significant difference on performance between the two groups regarding odor identification (t = 3.56, p = .001) with control participants answering more items correctly on the UPSIT (mean = 31.85, SD = 3.6) than participants with bipolar disorder (mean = 27.58, SD = 6.7). Furthermore, bipolar patients showed an impairment in facial emotion recognition, with a significantly lower score in the emotion recognition test (mean = 41.78, SD = 6.9) than healthy controls (mean = 47.97, SD = 3.6), t = 4.859, p < .001.

Relationships between olfactory performance, social cognition, general cognition, and functioning

Correlations were computed between performance on the UPSIT and each measure of social cognition, general cognition, and the two measures of functional outcome in the patient group. Better performance on the UPSIT was associated with better emotion recognition and Faux Pas Recognition Test, as well as a greater number of correct WCST categories and number correct on the Asarnow Test. Finally, performance on the UPSIT was associated at trend level with global functioning as measured by the GAF (Table 2).

Table 2 Correlations between performance on the UPSIT and each measure of social cognition, general cognition and global functioning among individuals with bipolar disorder

We recomputed the above correlations, while controlling for the influence of age and number of cigarettes smoked per day. The UPSIT was still significantly correlated with the emotion recognition test (r = .39, p = .03) and the WCST total (r = .40, p = .03). However, the relationships between the UPSIT and the comprehension faux pas test, GAF, FAST, and SAT were no longer statistically significant after controlling for age and cigarettes smoked.

Multiple regression models predicting social cognition from UPSIT

For the first regression analysis, we predicted performance on the emotion recognition test in the patient group. For the first step, we entered age and number of cigarettes smoked per day. This first model significantly predicted performance on the emotion recognition test (R 2 = .312, F = 7.029, p = .003). In the second step, we entered performance on both measures of general cognitive functioning. We found that general cognitive variables significantly improved the model (ΔR 2 = .169, p = .017), which contributed 16.9% of variance to the model predicting emotion recognition. In the final step, we entered performance on the UPSIT. This test approached statistical significance in improving the model (p = .055), which contributed 6.5% of variance to the model predicting emotion recognition, beyond the influence of general cognitive ability, age, and cigarette smoking. Alternatively, the partial correlation between the UPSIT and the emotion recognition test approached significance (r = .354, p = .055), independent of the influence of general cognitive ability, age, and smoking. The full model R 2 after all three steps was .546.

Repeating the same regression analysis predicting the comprehension faux pas test, we again first entered age and number of cigarettes smoked per day. This first model did not significantly predict performance on the emotion recognition test (R 2 = .074, F = 1.274, p = .294). Entering the two measures of cognitive ability at step two improved the model (ΔR 2 = .183, p = .017), which contributed 18.3% of variance to the model predicting emotion recognition. At step 3, UPSIT did not significantly improve the model (ΔR 2 = .019, p = .396). The full model R 2 after all three steps was .276.

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to examine the relationships between olfactory ability and social cognition in bipolar disorder. First, the hypothesis that bipolar patients are impaired in olfactory identification was supported. Second, olfactory function was related to each of the social cognition measures in the study, as well as with two tasks of general cognition. Finally, olfactory function approached statistical significance in predicting performance on facial emotion recognition when accounting for age, smoking, and general cognitive ability, although it did not predict performance on the theory of mind measure when controlling for these variables.

In previous work, Kohler etal.Reference Kohler, Barrett, Gur, Turetsky and Moberg 49 reported an association between facial emotion recognition and odor identification in schizophrenia. Years before, other studies had reported impaired odor identification in patients with autism and severe social difficulties.Reference Suzuki, Critchley, Rowe, Howlin and Murphy 50 , Reference Bennetto, Kuschner and Hyman 51 The present study extends this association to patients with bipolar disorder. The influence of odor perception on emotions, mood, or behavior in humans is still an open research question. In the majority of mammals, the sense of smell is the principal medium for social interaction, and plays important roles in mating, parenting, group affiliation, territorial boundaries, and the identification of enemies.Reference Malaspina, Corcoran and Goudsmit 52 In humans, neuroanatomical and imaging studies show an overlap of olfactory and limbic neural structures.Reference Van Toller 53 , Reference Zald and Pardo 54 While many more studies are needed to confirm these findings, there is preliminary evidence of a link between these two systems, both in clinical and normal populations. This reported overlap between smell and social deficits (in autism, psychosis, and now bipolar disorder) opens the way to a better comprehension of the physiopathology of these disabling psychiatric conditions. Furthermore, it suggests a potential use of olfactory tests as markers of subclinical cases, as have been already studied in Parkinson's and Alzheimer's disease.Reference Rahayel, Frasnelli and Joubert 55 , Reference Haehner, Hummel and Reichmann 56

Our findings indicate that general cognitive factors accounted for much of the relationship between olfactory function and theory of mind. This suggests that olfactory deficits might be a marker of general cognitive disability that leads to impaired performance on theory of mind tasks. However, the pattern of findings from the facial emotion recognition test suggests that above and beyond the influence of cognitive measures, a trend-level relationship persists between olfactory function and emotion perception. While previous work has contended that relationships between olfactory function and cognitive and clinical variables are unrelated, the present study suggests that responses to these findings should be tempered as they apply to social cognition. Nonetheless, a consistent relationship still remains between the two constructs. Future research could explore biological or psychological models that connect emotion recognition and olfactory identification. While our findings are not sufficient to create such a model, this article points to the merits of exploring the relationship between olfactory function and social cognition, and in particular olfactory function and facial affect perception.

This study had a number of limitations. First, all bipolar patients were receiving pharmacological treatment. Conducting studies on patients free from medications is ethically problematic, and, in addition, various studies have found a null effect of psychopharmaceuticals on olfactory capacity.Reference Martzke, Kopala and Good 1 , Reference Moberg, Agrin and Gur 5 , Reference Pause, Miranda, Göder, Aldenhoff and Ferstl 10 , Reference Rupp 57 Second, the emotion recognition test has been widely used but has not been sufficiently validated in the literature in terms of internal consistency and test-retest reliability. Third, there was a statistical trend towards difference between the groups in average age. This is controlled for in this article's more advanced analyses, but it remains a complication for the interpretation of the simple correlations. The number of participants was relatively small, and these relationships, while notable, should also be interpreted with caution, particularly as they pertain to differentiating between subtypes of social cognition. Finally, it is important to clarify that UPSIT measures only identification of odors. Other dimensions of olfactory perception such as odor detection, pleasantness, intensity, memory, familiarity, and discrimination were not assessed in this study. However, the administration of 9 different olfactory tests to 97 healthy subjects revealed that a number of nominally distinct tests were measuring a common source of variance.Reference Doty, Smith, McKeown and Raj 58

Conclusion

The bipolar patients showed a significant impairment in olfactory identification compared to healthy controls. As hypothesized, olfactory dysfunction in bipolar patients was correlated with a deficit in both verbal and nonverbal measures of social cognition. When accounting for age, cigarettes smoked, and general cognitive ability, this relationship remained significant between olfactory function and facial emotion recognition. Future research could explore biological or psychological models that connect emotion recognition and olfactory identification in patients with affective disorders.

Disclosures

Guillermo Lahera has been on the speakers’/advisory boards for Otsuka, Lundbeck, Janssen, and Lilly. Salvador Ruiz-Murugarren has been speaker for Otsuka. Jerónimo Sáiz-Ruiz has been on the speakers’/advisory boards of Servier, Janssen, and Shire. The remaining authors have nothing to disclose.

Footnotes

The authors wish to thank the Foundation for Biomedical Research (Príncipe de Asturias University Hospital) for the grant support.

References

1. Martzke, JS, Kopala, LC, Good, KP. Olfactory dysfunction in neuropsychiatric disorders: review and methodological considerations. Biol Psychiatry. 1997; 42(8): 721732.Google Scholar
2. Atanasova, B, Graux, J, El Hage, W, etal. Olfaction: a potential cognitive marker of psychiatric disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008; 32: 13151325.Google Scholar
3. Kivity, S, Ortega-Hernandez, OD, Shoenfeld, Y. Olfaction—a window to the mind. Isr Med Assoc J. 2009; 11: 238243.Google ScholarPubMed
4. Kopala, L, Clark, C, Hurwitz, TA. Olfactory deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1993; 8: 245250.Google Scholar
5. Moberg, PJ, Agrin, R, Gur, RE, etal. Olfactory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999; 21: 325341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Stedman, TJ, Clair, AL. Neuropsychological, neurological and symptom correlates of impaired olfactory identification in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1998; 32: 2330.Google Scholar
7. Moberg, PJ, Arnold, SE, Doty, RL, etal. Impairment of odor hedonics in men with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003; 160: 17841789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Postolache, TT, Doty, RL, Wehr, TA, etal. Monorhinal odor identification and depression scores in patients with seasonal affective disorder. J Affect Disord. 1999; 56(1): 2735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Postolache, TT, Wehr, TA, Doty, RL, etal. Patients with seasonal affective disorder have lower odor detection thresholds than control subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002; 59(12): 11191122.Google Scholar
10. Pause, BM, Miranda, A, Göder, R, Aldenhoff, JB, Ferstl, R. Reduced olfactory performance in patients with major depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2001; 35(5): 271277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Lombion-Pouthier, S, Vandel, P, Nezelof, S, Haffen, E, Millot, JL. Odor perception in patients with mood disorders. J Affect Disord. 2006; 90(2–3): 187191.Google Scholar
12. Hurwitz, TA, Kopala, L, Clark, C, Jones, B. Olfactory deficits in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1988; 23: 123128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Amsterdam, JD, Settle, RG, Doty, RL, Abelman, E. Taste and smell perception in depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1987; 22: 14811485.Google Scholar
14. Warner, MD, Peabody, CA, Csernansky, JG. Olfactory functioning in schizophrenia and depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1990; 27: 457458.Google Scholar
15. Serby, M, Larson, P, Kalkstein, D. Olfactory senses in psychoses. Biol Psychiatry. 1990; 28: 829830.Google Scholar
16. Solomon, GS, Petrie, WM, Hart, JR, Brackin, HB. Olfactory dysfunction discriminates Alzheimer's dementia from major depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998; 10: 6467.Google Scholar
17. Swiecicki, L, Zatorski, P, Bzinkowska, D, etal. Gustatory and olfactory function in patients with unipolar and bipolar depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009; 33: 827834.Google Scholar
18. Brewer, WJ, Pantelis, C, Anderson, V, etal. Stability of olfactory identification deficits in neuroleptic-naïve patients with first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2001; 158: 107115.Google Scholar
19. Cumming, AG, Matthews, NL, Park, S. Olfactory identification and preference in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011; 261(4): 251259.Google Scholar
20. Hardy, C, Rosedale, M, Messinger, JW, etal. Olfactory acuity is associated with mood and function in a pilot study of stable bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar Disord. 2012; 14(1): 109117.Google Scholar
21. Millot, JL, Brand, G. Effects of pleasant and unpleasant ambient odors on human voice pitch. Neurosci Lett. 2001; 297: 6163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Miltner, W, Matjak, C, Braun, H, Diekmann, H, Brody, S. Emotional qualities of odors and their influence on the startle reflex in humans. Psychophysiology. 1994; 31: 107110.Google Scholar
23. Ludvigson, HW, Rottman, TR. Effects of ambient odors of lavender and cloves on cognition, memory, affect and mood. Chem Senses. 1989; 14: 525536.Google Scholar
24. Pitkänen, A, Pikkarainen, M, Nurminen, N, Ylinen, A. Reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the hippocampal formation, perirhinal cortex, and postrhinal cortex in rat: a review. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2000; 911: 369391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Rolls, ET, Kringelbach, ML, de Araujo, IE. Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odours in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci. 2003; 189: 695703.Google Scholar
26. Royet, JP, Hudry, J, Zald, DH, etal. Functional neuroanatomy of different olfactory judgments. Neuroimage. 2001; 13(3): 506519.Google Scholar
27. Savic, I. Processing of odorous signals in humans. Brain Res Bull. 2001; 54: 307312.Google Scholar
28. Penn, DL, Spaulding, W, Reed, D, etal. Cognition and social functioning in schizophrenia. Psychiatry. 1997; 60(4): 281291.Google Scholar
29. Baron-Cohen, S, Leslie, AM, Frith, U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. 1985: 3746.Google Scholar
30. Kerr, N, Dunbar, RI, Bentall, RP. Theory of mind deficits in bipolar affective disorder. J Affect Disord. 2003; 73: 253259.Google Scholar
31. Lahera, G, Montes, JM, Benito, A, etal. Theory of mind deficit in bipolar disorder: is it related to a previous history of psychotic symptoms? Psychiatry Res. 2008; 161: 309317.Google Scholar
32. Montag, C, Ehrlich, A, Neuhaus, K, etal. Theory of mind impairments in euthymic bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 2010; 123: 264269.Google Scholar
33. Martino, DJ, Strejilevich, SA, Fassi, G, Marengo, E, Igoa, A. Theory of mind and facial emotion recognition in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2011; 189(3): 379384.Google Scholar
34. Lahera, G, Ruiz-Murugarren, S, Iglesias, P, etal. Social cognition and global functioning in bipolar disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2012; 200(2): 135141.Google Scholar
35. American Psychiatric Association. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, text rev. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.Google Scholar
36. Doty, RL, Shaman, P, Dann, M. Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav. 1984; 32(3): 489502.Google Scholar
37. Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Jolliffe, T. Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition. 1997; 4: 311331.Google Scholar
38. Stone, VE, Baron-Cohen, S, Knight, RT. Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998; 10(5): 640656.Google Scholar
39. Gregory, C, Lough, S, Stone, V, etal. Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease: theoretical and practical implications. Brain. 2002; 125(Pt 4): 752764.Google Scholar
40. Asarnow, RF, Nuechterlein, KH, Marder, SR. Span of apprehension performance, neuropsychological functioning, and indices of psychosis-proneness. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1983; 171(11): 662669.Google Scholar
41. Spaulding, W, Garbin, C, Dras, S. Cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenic patients and schizotypal college students. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1989; 177: 717728.Google Scholar
42. Gurpegui, M, Alvarez, E, Bousoño, M, Ciudad, A, Carlos Gómez, J, Olivares, JM, Bousoño, M. Effect of olanzapine or risperidone treatment on some cognitive functions in a one-year follow-up of schizophrenia outpatients with prominent negative symptoms. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007; 17(11): 725734.Google Scholar
43. Heaton, RK. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Manual. Odessa, FL: Neuropsychological Assessment Resources; 1981.Google Scholar
44. Rosa, AR, Sánchez-Moreno, J, Martínez-Aran, A, etal. Validity and reliability of the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) in bipolar disorder. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2007; 3: 5.Google Scholar
45. Kaneda, H, Maeshima, K, Goto, N, etal. Decline in taste and odor discrimination abilities with age, and relationship between gustation and olfaction. Chem Senses. 2000; 25(3): 331337.Google Scholar
46. Green, MF. Cognitive impairment and functional outcome in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006; 67(Suppl 9): 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47. Diaz, FJ, James, D, Botts, S, etal. Tobacco smoking behaviors in bipolar disorder: a comparison of the general population, schizophrenia, and major depression. Bipolar Disord. 2009; 11(2): 154165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48. Frye, RE, Schwartz, BS, Doty, RL. Dose-related effects of cigarette smoking on olfactory function. JAMA. 1990; 263(9): 12331236.Google Scholar
49. Kohler, CG, Barrett, FS, Gur, RC, Turetsky, BI, Moberg, PJ. Association between facial emotion recognition and odor identification in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007; 19(2): 128132.Google Scholar
50. Suzuki, Y, Critchley, HD, Rowe, A, Howlin, P, Murphy, DGM. Impaired olfactory identification in Asperger's syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003; 15(1): 105107.Google Scholar
51. Bennetto, L, Kuschner, ES, Hyman, SL. Olfaction and taste processing in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 62(9): 10151021.Google Scholar
52. Malaspina, D, Corcoran, C, Goudsmit, N. The impact of olfaction on human social functioning. In: Brewer W, Castle D, Pantelis C, eds. Olfaction and the Brain. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2006: 121143.Google Scholar
53. Van Toller, S. Emotion and the brain. In: Van Toller S, Dodd GH, eds. Perfumery: The Psychology and Biology of Fragrance. London: Chapman and Hall; 1988: 1946.Google Scholar
54. Zald, DH, Pardo, JV. Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997; 94: 41194124.Google Scholar
55. Rahayel, S, Frasnelli, J, Joubert, S. The effect of Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease on olfaction: a meta-analysis. Behav Brain Res. 2012; 231(1): 6074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56. Haehner, A, Hummel, T, Reichmann, H. Olfactory dysfunction as a diagnostic marker for Parkinson's disease. Expert Rev Neurother. 2009; 9(12): 17731779.Google Scholar
57. Rupp, CI. Dysfunctions in olfactory processing in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2003; 16: 181185.Google Scholar
58. Doty, RL, Smith, R, McKeown, DA, Raj, J. Tests of human olfactory function: principal components analysis suggests that most measure a common source of variance. Percept Psychophys. 1994; 56(6): 701707.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of participants

Figure 1

Table 2 Correlations between performance on the UPSIT and each measure of social cognition, general cognition and global functioning among individuals with bipolar disorder