Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T04:37:08.071Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does Self-Rated Health Predict Death in Older Adults with Depressive Symptoms?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 February 2012

Philip St. John*
Affiliation:
Section of Geriatric Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of Manitoba, Canada Centre on Aging, University of Manitoba, Canada
Patrick Montgomery
Affiliation:
Vancouver Island Health Authority
*
Correspondence and requests for offprints should be sent to / La correspondance et les demandes de tirés-à-part doivent être adressées à: Philip St. John Associate Professor, Head Section of Geriatrics, University of Manitoba GE 545 Health Sciences Centre, 820 Sherbrook Street Winnipeg, MB, Canada R3A 1R9 (pstjohn@hsc.mb.ca)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We analysed a prospective cohort study to determine (1) if self-rated health (SRH) predicts mortality in older adults with and without depressive symptoms, and (2) to determine if there is an interaction between SRH and depressive symptoms on mortality. We followed 1,751 community-dwelling adults aged 65 and older over five years. Measurements included age, gender, education, the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-D), SRH, the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS), and the Older Americans Resource Survey (OARS). Our outcome measure was time to death. Analyses were conducted in those with, and those without, depressive symptoms. We constructed Cox regression models with an interaction term for the CES-D and SRH. The hazard ratio (HR) for mortality was 1.63 for those with depressive symptoms; it was 1.68 for those without. No significant interaction was found between depressive symptoms and SRH for mortality.

Résumé

Nous avons analysé une étude de cohorte prospective afin de déterminer (1) si l’auto-évaluation de son propre état de santé (AES) prédit la mortalité chez les personnes plus âgées, avec et sans symptômes dépressifs, et (2) de déterminer s’il y a une interaction entre l’AES et les symptômes dépressifs sur la mortalité. Nous avons suivi pendant cinq ans 1 751 adultes âgés de 65 ans qui habitaient dans les communautés. Les mesures comprenaient l’âge, le sexe, le niveau d’éducation, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-D), the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (MMS), et le questionnaire Older Americans’ Research Survey (OARS). Notre indicateur de résultats était la duree de vie avant le décès. Les analyses ont été faites sur des sujets avant ou non des symptômes dépressifs. Nous avons construit des modèles de régression de Cox avec un terme d’interaction pour la CES-D et l’AES. L’indice de risque pour la mortalite était de 1,63 pour les sujets atteints de symptômes dépressifs ; il était de 1,68 pour les sujets sans symptômes. Aucune interaction significative n’a été trouvée entre les symptômes dépressifs et l’A-ES pour la mortalité.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Association on Gerontology 2012

Background

Simply asking people how they feel about their health has long been an important component of health status assessment. However, in clinical practice and in research, simple assessments of health have been overshadowed by more-technical questioning and diagnostic testing. This has led one senior researcher and clinician (Blazer, Reference Blazer2008) to lament the trend away from simply asking people how they feel:

Clinical medicine has moved away from listening to patients (their complaints) and toward seeing them, and what can be seen (a magnetic resonance image or a number on a symptoms screening scale). It has moved from a hear-and-feel discipline to a see-and-do discipline. Algorithms are increasingly replacing the art of listening to patients and assessing their views of their health. ‘‘How do you feel...?’’ is a question all too often ignored.

Self-rated health (SRH) is a simple, reliable (Lundberg & Manderbacka, Reference Lundberg and Manderbacka1996) question which may be useful in clinical practice, research, and general health surveys. The subjective assessment of one’s own health is associated with biomedical markers of disease (Jylhä, Volpato, & Guralnik, Reference Jylha, Volpato and Guralnik2006) and with objective measures of health. Gama et al. (Reference Gama, Damian, de Perez, Lopez, Lopez and Gavira Iglesias2000) found that SRH is strongly related to activities of daily living (ADLs), particularly mobility-related ADLs. However, SRH may be more than simply a reflection on one’s health – it may also reflect an individual’s efforts to achieve health-related goals (Bailis, Segall, & Chipperfield, Reference Bailis, Segall and Chipperfield2003), health beliefs and behaviours (Manderbacka, Reference Manderbacka1998), or sociocultural influences (Menec, Shooshtari, & Lambert, Reference Menec, Shooshtari and Lambert2007). SRH may predict mortality and contain information that is not entirely reflected in underlying medical conditions and risk factors, at least in middle-aged persons (Heidrich, Leise, Lowel, & Keil, Reference Heidrich, Liese, Lowel and Keil2002; Heistaro, Jousilahti, Lahelma, Vartiainen, & Puska, Reference Heistaro, Jousilahti, Lahelma, Vartiainen and Puska2001).

Since first reported, the association between SRH and mortality (Mossey & Shapiro, Reference Mossey and Shapiro1982) has been replicated in many studies in many countries and many settings (DeSalvo, Bloser, Reynolds, He, & Muntner, Reference DeSalvo, Bloser, Reynolds, He and Muntner2006; Idler & Benyamini, Reference Idler and Benyamini1997). In some populations, however, the question may not be an accurate predictor of death. The association between SRH and death may be weaker in African-Americans (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Moody-Ayers, Landefeld, Walter, Lindquist and Segal2007), women (Helmer, Barberger-Gateau, Letenneur, & Dartigues, Reference Helmer, Barberger-Gateau, Letenneur and Dartigues1999), those with lower social position (Dowd & Zajacova, Reference Dowd and Zajacova2007; Singh-Manoux et al., Reference Singh-Manoux, Dugravot, Shipley, Ferrie, Martikainen and Goldberg2007), and those with severe cognitive impairment (Walker, Maxwell, Hogan, & Ebly, Reference Walker, Maxwell, Hogan and Ebly2004). Importantly, Benjamins, Hummer, Eberstein, and Nam (Reference Benjamins, Hummer, Eberstein and Nam2004) have reported that SRH is predictive of most causes of death, except those most strongly associated with depression: suicide, homicide, and accidents. It is possible, therefore, that SRH may not accurately predict death in those with depressive symptoms. Depressive symptoms may result in a different assessment of subjective health. Han (Reference Han2002) found that depressive symptoms predicted a change in SRH over time; however, Han did not examine the effect of SRH or depressive symptoms on mortality.

The objectives of this study involving adults aged 65 and over residing in the community were twofold: (1) To determine if SRH predicts mortality in older adults with and without depressive symptoms; and (2) to determine if there is an interaction between SRH and depressive symptoms in influencing mortality over a five-year time frame.

Methods

Sample

Data used in this study are from the Manitoba Study of Health and Aging (MSHA), a longitudinal study of aging and cognition conducted in conjunction with the Canadian Study of Health and Aging (CSHA, 1994). Manitoba had, in 1991, a relatively stable population of 1,091,940 (Statistics Canada, 1992). Between February 1991 and November 1992, 1,751 individuals aged 65 and over living in the community were interviewed in person. These older persons were randomly selected according to health region and age group from a list provided by Manitoba Health, one of the most complete listings of residents available. There was an over-sampling of the oldest old.

In 1996 and 1997, attempts were made to re-interview the 1,751 individuals who had participated in the 1991–1992 interviews. By 1996–1997, 417 of these individuals had died. Telephone interviews were conducted with family members or friends for individuals who had died between 1991 and 1997. For those family and friends who consented, death certificates and administrative records were obtained for those who were deceased.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Manitoba, and it was in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Measures

Time 1 Predictors. Depressive symptoms were measured using the CES-D scale (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). A score of greater than or equal to 16 was considered abnormal. SRH was assessed using the question: “How would you say your health is these days? (very good; pretty good; not too good; poor; or very poor)” Since the number of observations in some cells was small, SRH was dichotomized into good (very good/good) versus poor (not too good/poor/very poor) for some analyses.

The Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS) was used to assess cognition (Teng & Chui, Reference Teng and Chui1987). We analyzed the 3MS both as a continuous variable (100 points), and also by stratifying the sample into those with normal cognition (a score of 78 or above) or abnormal cognition (a score below 78). Functional status was based on self-report. It was assessed using the seven-item ADL and the seven-item Instrumental ADL (IADL) scales from the Older American Resources Utilization Survey (OARS; Fillenbaum, Reference Fillenbaum1988). For our analyses, we summed the scores of individual items and used the scale as a continuous variable, scored from 0 to 28. Education was measured as number of years of completed schooling; age is in years.

Outcome Measures. We considered the time to death as the primary outcome for these analyses. Death over the five-year interval was obtained by proxy report, death certificate, and administrative records. Mortality was coded as either 1 (deceased by the end of MSHA-2) or 0 (alive at the end of the study). Time to death was determined using the methodology of the Canadian Study of Health and Aging (Dubois & Hebert, Reference Dubois and Hebert2001). Briefly, if death certificate data were available (n = 247), we obtained the date of death from this. If death certificate data were not available, then we used the date of death from proxy report (n = 73) or administrative data (n = 97). The participants were considered to have been censored after the time-2 study period.

Statistical Analyses

The analyses (n = 1,751) included all participants for whom complete data were available. Associations between time-1 variables and mortality were sought using chi-square tests for categorical variables, and student’s t-tests (unequal variance) for continuous variables. We constructed Kaplan–Meier plots and used log rank tests. To adjust for the effect of confounding variables, we constructed Cox proportional hazards regression models with the outcome being the time to death. Covariates included CES-D score, age, gender, education, 3MS score, OARS score, and SRH. For these analyses, we dichotomized SRH into good versus poor since there were small numbers in some cells. Continuous variables were entered directly into the model, while categorical variables were entered as dummy variables. We sought interactions by stratifying analyses and by incorporating interaction terms into Cox proportional hazards models. We conducted analyses in SPSS version 9 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and Stata version 10 (Statacorp, college Station, TX).

Results

There were 1,751 participants. Depressive symptoms were present in 13.8 per cent of the total sample. There were 240 persons with depressive symptoms and 1,507 persons without depressive symptoms at time 1. Overall, 24.7 per cent had poor SRH; 63.8 per cent of those with depressive symptoms had poor SRH versus 18.5 per cent of those without (p < .0001, chi-square test). The mean age of the sample participants was 77.5 years (78.3 years in those with poor SRH versus 77.3 years in those with good SRH; p = 0.008, student’s t-test); 58.5 per cent were women (62.0 per cent of those with poor SRH versus 57.3 per cent of those with good SRH; p = 0.08, chi-square test). The mean education level was 9.3 years (8.2 years in those with poor SRH versus 9.7 in those with good SRH; p < .001, student’s test).

Over the five-year period, 417 participants died. Depressive symptoms and poor SRH both predicted death. Poor SRH predicted mortality over five years in those with depressive symptoms (41.2% of those with poor SRH vs. 26.4% of those with good SRH; p = .015, chi-square test), and in those without depressive symptoms (19.8% of those with good SRH vs. 31.2% of those with poor SRH; p < .001, chi-square test). The hazard ratio (HR, 95% confidence interval) of death over five years was 1.83 (1.50, 2.24) for those with poor SRH versus good SRH. In those with depressive symptoms, the HR was 1.63 (1.01, 2.63). In those without depressive symptoms, the HR was 1.68 (1.31, 2.14). There was a gradient in risk across SRH (see Figures 1 and 2).

Figure 1: Kaplan–Meier plot of five-year mortality in those without depressive symptoms. Self-rated health predicted mortality in those with no depressive symptoms, and there was a gradient in this effect.

Figure 2: Kaplan–Meier plot of five-year mortality in those with depressive symptoms. Self-rated health also predicted mortality in those with depressive symptoms, and there was a gradient in this effect.

We noted no interaction in the effect of depressive symptoms and SRH on mortality. When we constructed Cox regression models and included an interaction term SRH*CES-D, this term was not significant. We also conducted stratified analyses. The effect of SRH on mortality was similar in those with, and in those without, depressive symptoms in Cox regression models (see Table 1).

Table 1: Results of Cox regression models. (Self-rated health predicts death in those with and in those without depressive symptoms)

3MS = Modified Mini-Mental State Examination; OARS = Older American Resource Survey; SRH = self-rated health; CES-D = Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression. Note that education and the 3MS score are highly co-linear.

95% confidence intervals in parentheses.

In regression diagnostics, we also observed the previously documented effect of severe cognitive impairment (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Maxwell, Hogan and Ebly2004), where SRH predicted mortality in those with mild and moderate cognitive impairment but not severe cognitive impairment. As well, we noted that education and the 3MS score were highly correlated (data not shown; available on request). Since omitting either the 3MS or education from the models did not alter the associations between SRH, depression, and mortality, we chose to present the full model including both variables.

Discussion

Both SRH and depressive symptoms predicted death. There was no interaction: SRH predicted death in those with depressive symptoms and in those without depressive symptoms.

This study does have some limitations. First, we used the CES-D as a measure of depressive symptoms. Although this is a reliable, valid measure, it is not a measure of clinically diagnosed major depression. It is possible the results we obtained might have been different in those individuals with major depression. A second limitation is that our sample was racially homogeneous, reflecting the demographic make-up of older adults in Manitoba. Other researchers have shown that the association between SRH and mortality is not as strong in African-Americans as in non-African-Americans (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Moody-Ayers, Landefeld, Walter, Lindquist and Segal2007). More diverse samples from other studies could show different results. A third study limitation is that both health and depressive symptoms are not stable over time, and the relationship between these two factors and mortality can vary over time.

Our study also has a number of strengths. Reliable, valid measures were gathered by trained interviewers. As well, the study was population-based with representation from the entire province of Manitoba; this representation, however, may also be a limitation. The results we observed may not hold true in clinical samples with sicker individuals.

Our findings generally support the predictive validity of a simple question about general health. Previous studies considered depressive symptoms and depression a confounding factor (Bardage, Isacson, & Pedersen, Reference Bardage, Isacson and Pedersen2001; Blazer, Hybels, & Pieper, Reference Blazer, Hybels and Pieper2001). These studies found that after adjusting for the effect of depression, SRH predicts mortality. However, few of the previous studies considered an interaction between the two factors. Since older adults with depression may perceive their health differently than do those without depression, it is important to also consider interactions. Our study data support the view that SRH is an accurate predictor of death even in persons with depressive symptoms.

SRH has proved to be a remarkably accurate and potentially useful global measure of health. Its use has largely been limited to epidemiologic studies and health surveys. Further study into its usefulness in clinical settings and in populations seeking health care is warranted. Simple questions on health may prove extremely useful in clinical settings, and further research should be conducted in these populations. These simple questions may be a useful adjuvant to complex diagnostic medical tests. They might also be a useful alternative to complex diagnostic tests.

Footnotes

*

The Manitoba Study of Health and Aging (MSHA) was funded primarily by Manitoba Health, with additional funding provided through the Canadian Study of Health and Aging by the Seniors Independence Research Program of the National Health Research and Development Program of Health Canada. The second wave of the MSHA was funded primarily by Manitoba Health’s Healthy Communities Development Fund with additional funding provided through the Canadian Study of Health and Aging by the Seniors Independence Research Program of the National Health Research and Development Program of Health Canada (Project No. 6606-3954-MC[S])). The results and conclusions are those of the authors, and no official endorsement by Manitoba Health or other funding agencies is intended or should be inferred.

Conflict of Interest: Neither author has any financial, personal, or potential conflict of interest.

Authors and Sponsor’s role: Dr. Montgomery was involved in the initial planning of the MSHA. Dr. St. John and Dr. Montgomery conducted clinical examinations and planned these analyses. The sponsors played no role in interpreting the results.

This paper was presented at the American Geriatrics Society Annual meeting in Washington, DC, 2008.

References

Bailis, D.S., Segall, A., & Chipperfield, J.G. (2003). Two views of self-rated general health status. Social Science & Medicine, 56(2), 203217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bardage, C., Isacson, D., & Pedersen, N.L. (2001). Self-rated health as a predictor of mortality among persons with cardiovascular disease in Sweden. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 29(1), 1322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamins, M.R., Hummer, R.A., Eberstein, I.W., & Nam, C.B. (2004). Self-reported health and adult mortality risk: An analysis of cause-specific mortality. Social Science & Medicine, 59(6), 12971306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blazer, D.G. (2008). How do you feel about.? Health outcomes in late life and self-perceptions of health and well-being. The Gerontologist, 48(4), 415422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blazer, D.G., Hybels, C.F., & Pieper, C.F. (2001). The association of depression and mortality in elderly persons: A case for multiple, independent pathways. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56(8), M505M509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CSHA. (1994). Canadian study of health and aging: Study methods and prevalence of dementia. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 150(6), 899913.Google Scholar
DeSalvo, K.B., Bloser, N., Reynolds, K., He, J., & Muntner, P. (2006). Mortality prediction with a single general self-rated health question. A meta-analysis. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 21(3), 267275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowd, J.B., & Zajacova, A. (2007). Does the predictive power of self-rated health for subsequent mortality risk vary by socioeconomic status in the US? International Journal of Epidemiology, 36(6), 12141221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, M.F., & Hebert, R. (2001). Imputation of missing dates of death or institutionalization for time-to-event analyses in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. International Psychogeriatrics, 13(Suppl. 1), 9197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fillenbaum, G.G. (1988). Multidimensional functional assessment of older adults: The Duke Older Americans Resources and Services procedures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gama, E.V., Damian, J.E., de Perez, M.J., Lopez, M.R., Lopez, P.M., & Gavira Iglesias, F.J. (2000). Association of individual activities of daily living with self-rated health in older people. Age and Ageing, 29(3), 267270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, B. (2002). Depressive symptoms and self-rated health in community-dwelling older adults: A longitudinal study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50(9), 15491556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heidrich, J., Liese, A.D., Lowel, H., & Keil, U. (2002). Self-rated health and its relation to all-cause and cardiovascular mortality in southern Germany. Results from the MONICA Augsburg cohort study 1984-1995. Annals of Epidemiology, 12(5), 338345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heistaro, S., Jousilahti, P., Lahelma, E., Vartiainen, E., & Puska, P. (2001). Self rated health and mortality: A long term prospective study in eastern Finland. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 55(4), 227232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmer, C., Barberger-Gateau, P., Letenneur, L., & Dartigues, J.F. (1999). Subjective health and mortality in French elderly women and men. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 54(2), S84S92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Idler, E.L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38(1), 2137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jylha, M., Volpato, S., & Guralnik, J.M. (2006). Self-rated health showed a graded association with frequently used biomarkers in a large population sample. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 59(5), 465471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S.J., Moody-Ayers, S.Y., Landefeld, C.S., Walter, L.C., Lindquist, K., Segal, M.R., et al. . (2007). The relationship between self-rated health and mortality in older black and white Americans. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 55(10), 16241629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundberg, O., & Manderbacka, K. (1996). Assessing reliability of a measure of self-rated health. Scandinavian Journal of Social Medicine, 24(3), 218224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manderbacka, K. (1998). Examining what self-rated health question is understood to mean by respondents. Scandinavian Journal of Social Medicine, 26(2), 145153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menec, V.H., Shooshtari, S., & Lambert, P. (2007). Ethnic differences in self-rated health among older adults: A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis. Journal of Aging and Health, 19(1), 6286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mossey, J.M., & Shapiro, E. (1982). Self-rated health: A predictor of mortality among the elderly. American Journal of Public Health, 72(8), 800808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh-Manoux, A., Dugravot, A., Shipley, M.J., Ferrie, J.E., Martikainen, P., Goldberg, M., et al. . (2007). The association between self-rated health and mortality in different socioeconomic groups in the GAZEL cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36(6), 12221228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. (1992). Profile of census metropolitan areas and census agglomerations, 1991 census – 100% data. Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: Ministry of Industry, Science and Technology.Google Scholar
Walker, J.D., Maxwell, C.J., Hogan, D.B., & Ebly, E.M. (2004). Does self-rated health predict survival in older persons with cognitive impairment? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52(11), 18951900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teng, E.L., & Chui, H.C. (1987). The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48(8), 314318.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1: Kaplan–Meier plot of five-year mortality in those without depressive symptoms. Self-rated health predicted mortality in those with no depressive symptoms, and there was a gradient in this effect.

Figure 1

Figure 2: Kaplan–Meier plot of five-year mortality in those with depressive symptoms. Self-rated health also predicted mortality in those with depressive symptoms, and there was a gradient in this effect.

Figure 2

Table 1: Results of Cox regression models. (Self-rated health predicts death in those with and in those without depressive symptoms)