Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T04:41:53.680Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Determinants of Loneliness among Older Adults in Canada

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 February 2015

Jenny de Jong Gierveld*
Affiliation:
Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute
Norah Keating
Affiliation:
Department of Human Ecology, University of Alberta
Janet E. Fast
Affiliation:
Department of Human Ecology, University of Alberta
*
*La correspondance et les demandes de tirés-à-part doivent être adressées à : / Correspondence and requests for offprints should be sent to: Jenny de Jong Gierveld, Ph.D. Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute (NIDI) P.O. Box 11650 2502 AR The Hague, The Netherlands (gierveld@nidi.nl)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to determine the key determinants of loneliness of older Canadians. We drew on the assumptions concerning the importance of person-environment fit to test the relative importance of personal characteristics, deprived living conditions, social network/social engagement, and satisfaction in explaining loneliness. Data comprised a sample of 3,799 respondents over age 65 drawn from Statistics Canada’s General Social Survey, Cycle 22. Personal characteristics, social network size and composition, and satisfaction with network contact were found to be related to loneliness, as were indicators of living in economically and socially challenging conditions. Older adults who had experienced a recent downturn in their financial situation, and who lacked the help needed to cope with a recent personal challenge, reported higher levels of loneliness. A striking feature of our findings is the relatively low scores on loneliness of older Canadians compared to older adults in other countries.

Résumé

L'objectif de cette étude était de découvrir les déterminants principaux de la solitude des Canadiens âgés. Nous avons tiré sur des hypothèses concernant l'importance de la concordance entre la personne et l'environnement pour tester l'importance relative des caractéristiques personnelles, des conditions de vie privées, du réseau social/engagement social, et de la plausibilité de l'explication de la solitude. Les données comprenaient un échantillon de 3 799 répondants âgés de plus de 65, tirées de l'Enquête sociale générale de Statistique Canada, Cycle 22. Les caractéristiques personnelles, la dimension et la composition des réseaux sociaux, et la satisfaction avec les contacts du réseau, tous sont révélés d'être liés à la solitude, de même que les indicateurs de vivre sous des conditions économiquement et socialement difficiles. Les personnes âgées qui ont connu un recul récente de leurs situations financières, et qui n'avaient pas l'aide nécessaire pour faire face à un défi personnel récente, ont déclaré des niveaux plus élevés de la solitude. Une caractéristique frappante de nos résultats, cependant, c'est les scores relativement faibles sur la solitude des Canadiens âgés par rapport aux personnes âgées dans d'autres pays.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Association on Gerontology 2015 

A varied social network of kin and non-kin relationships and optimal exchanges of support within the network have been shown to be important determinants of feeling socially embedded and consequently of general well-being in later life (Hawkley et al., Reference Hawkley, Hughes, Waite, Masi, Thisted and Cacioppo2008). Conceptualizations of loneliness have long addressed this relationship. More than 30 years ago, Perlman and Peplau (1981, p. 31) defined loneliness as “the unpleasant experience that occurs when a person’s network of social relations is deficient in some important way quantitatively or qualitatively”. De Jong Gierveld (Reference De Jong Gierveld1987) posited a similar definition, frequently used in European countries, which states:

loneliness is a situation experienced by the individual as one where there is an unpleasant or inadmissible lack (quantity or quality) of certain relationships. This includes situations in which the number of existing relationships is smaller than is considered desirable or acceptable, as well as situations where the intimacy one wishes for has not been realized. (p. 120)

Central to both definitions are the assumptions that loneliness is a subjective and negative experience, and that it is the outcome of a cognitive evaluation of the match between the quantity and quality of existing relationships and individuals’ expectations regarding them.

Over the years, much has been learned about the links between social networks and loneliness. Important determinants have been identified, and researchers have constructed and tested conceptual loneliness models that integrate socio-demographic characteristics, factors regarding the size and composition of the social network, and the evaluation of social relationships to explain the intensity of loneliness (De Jong Gierveld, Reference De Jong Gierveld1987; Hawkley et al., Reference Hawkley, Hughes, Waite, Masi, Thisted and Cacioppo2008). In this article, we describe our study that built on this foundational work by further theorizing relevant determinants of loneliness, and exploring empirically the relative importance of determinants of loneliness in the Canadian context.

Given that there is only a small body of research on loneliness of the older population in Canada (Havens & Hall, Reference Havens and Hall2001; Van Tilburg, Havens, & De Jong Gierveld, Reference Van Tilburg, Havens and De Jong Gierveld2004; Wu & Penning, Reference Wu and Penning2015), and given the established importance of loneliness to well-being in later life, it is important to deepen our understanding of the factors associated with loneliness in Canada. We have built on previous research by including several previously identified explanatory factors associated with loneliness but address, in addition, deprived living conditions of older adults as indicators of environmental press that may influence the depth of loneliness of older Canadians.

Conceptual Background

Loneliness is most often conceptualized as one of the possible outcomes of the subjective evaluation of the number of kin and non-kin relationships in one’s personal network, of the support exchanges within this network, and of individual expectations regarding the network. Social isolation and loneliness are conceptually distinct (Hortulanus, Machielse, & Meeuwesen, Reference Hortulanus, Machielse and Meeuwesen2006). An individual who is well positioned in terms of objective social participation can occupy virtually any position on the subjective continuum, depending on his or her relationship expectations or wishes.

Acknowledging the importance of the concept of these “relationship standards”, Perlman and Peplau (Reference Perlman, Peplau, Gilmour and Duck1981) developed a cognitive discrepancy approach to loneliness that focuses on the subjective evaluation of relationships in association with personal standards for what constitutes an optimal network of social relationships. The assumption is that the size and other characteristics of an older person’s network of relationships are, to a certain extent, a matter of personal choice, related to personal standards or wishes of the person. Thus, some older adults are committed to maintaining close contacts with kin or with neighbours and friends while others avoid frequent encounters.

Gerontologists might argue that such a strong emphasis on the importance of personal preferences and agency masks constraints that may accompany later life. Wahl, Iwarsson, and Oswald (2012, p. 306) made this point when they stated that while there is broad consensus that both personal and environmental resources contribute to aging well, their contributions have been largely overlooked. Following this argument, personal characteristics such as age, sex, marital status, and health – often included as control variables in loneliness models – can be more explicitly conceptualized as resources that create opportunities and constraints that influence the risk of loneliness.

Environmental press, first described by Lawton & Nahemow (Reference Lawton, Nahemow, Eisdorfer and Lawton1973), also is relevant in understanding a negative outcome such as loneliness. Victor, Scambler, Bond, and Bowling (Reference Victor, Scambler, Bond and Bowling2000) found that some older adults are confronted with environmental challenges such as deprived living conditions, a shortage of social network members, and low quality of interactions with others, and that these factors place them at risk of loneliness (Victor et al., Reference Victor, Scambler, Bond and Bowling2000). De Jong Gierveld and Tesch-Römer (Reference De Jong Gierveld and Tesch-Römer2012) have concurred, providing evidence linking poor living conditions to loneliness. In our current study, we added living conditions, as an example of environmental resources, to the social resources that have long formed a core element of theoretical thinking about loneliness conceptualization.

Thus, in designing this study, we built on theoretical ideas about loneliness as formulated by De Jong Gierveld, Van Tilburg, and Dykstra (Reference De Jong Gierveld, Van Tilburg, Dykstra, Vangelisti and Perlman2006) and related loneliness models (De Jong Gierveld, Reference De Jong Gierveld1987; De Jong Gierveld & Tesch-Römer, Reference De Jong Gierveld and Tesch-Römer2012; Hawkley et al., Reference Hawkley, Hughes, Waite, Masi, Thisted and Cacioppo2008). We drew on the assumption from environmental gerontology that older adults “are particularly sensitive to the nature and character of P-E interactions” (Wahl et al, Reference Wahl, Iwarsson and Oswald2012, p. 307), thus making explicit personal resources and environmental and social demands that might influence loneliness. Satisfaction, a core element of existing loneliness models, is viewed as the individual’s assessment of the fit between their resources and environmental demands. Based on this set of assumptions, we hypothesized elements of a loneliness model with four blocks of determinants of loneliness: (a) personal characteristics, (b) deprived living conditions, (c) social network/social engagement, and (d) satisfaction.

Personal Characteristics

Older adults face an elevated risk of loneliness as they age (Penning, Liu, & Chou, Reference Penning, Liu and Chou2014) in part because of reduced personal resources related to marital status and health. Widowhood and divorce, increasingly likely with age, are risk factors for loneliness (Allen, Blieszner, & Roberto, Reference Allen, Blieszner and Roberto2000; Dykstra & De Jong Gierveld, Reference Dykstra and De Jong Gierveld2004), whereas marriage is known to offer a great degree of protection against loneliness (De Jong Gierveld, Broese van Groenou, Hoogendoorn, & Smit, Reference De Jong Gierveld, Broese van Groenou, Hoogendoorn and Smit2009; Waite & Lehrer, Reference Waite and Lehrer2003). Older adults who are no longer married or never married, and who do not share a household with a partner, frequently have to rely on people outside the household for support and help. Maintaining contact with network members living elsewhere requires more time, initiative, and perseverance, although contemporary modes of communication and travel have facilitated contact among network members who do not live close to each other (Ajrouch, Akiyama, & Antonucci, Reference Ajrouch, Akiyama, Antonucci, Wahl, Tesch-Roemer and Hoff2007). Widowhood is more frequently a woman’s than a man’s experience. Aartsen and Jylhä (Reference Aartsen and Jylhä2011) showed that the higher incidence of loneliness among women could be explained in part by the unequal distribution of the risk of becoming widowed among men and women. In a review of more than 300 studies on gender differences in subjective well-being including loneliness, Pinquart and Sorensen (Reference Pinquart and Sörensen2001a) found that older women had significantly lower well-being than men did. Based on these findings, we formulated hypothesis 1: being older, being unmarried, and being female are associated with higher levels of loneliness.

Studies have shown repeatedly that older adults who are in poor health are prone to high levels of loneliness (De Jong Gierveld et al., Reference De Jong Gierveld, Van Tilburg, Dykstra, Vangelisti and Perlman2006; Korporaal, Broese van Groenou, & Van Tilburg, Reference Korporaal, Broese van Groenou and Van Tilburg2008; Pinquart & Sörensen, Reference Pinquart and Sörensen2001b; Victor et al., Reference Victor, Scambler, Bond and Bowling2000). In addition to physical health problems, experiencing mental health problems also affects levels of loneliness (Kuyper & Fokkema, Reference Kuyper and Fokkema2010; Warner & Kelley-Moore, Reference Warner and Kelley-Moore2012). Cacioppo, Hughes, Waite, Hawkley & Thisted (Reference Cacioppo, Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Thisted2006) showed that loneliness and depressive symptoms could act synergistically to diminish well-being in older adults. That said, one has to keep in mind that the health–loneliness relationship is likely reciprocal: poor health might affect loneliness, and loneliness affects the health conditions of persons (Havens & Hall, Reference Havens and Hall2001; Lauder, Mummery, Jones, & Caperchione, Reference Lauder, Mummery, Jones and Caperchione2006). Based on these findings, we formulated hypothesis 2: poor health is associated with higher levels of loneliness.

Deprived Living Conditions

There is growing evidence of the links between deprived living conditions and loneliness. Socioeconomic status influences older persons’ ability to optimize and diversify social contacts during the life course, affecting loneliness in late adulthood (Ajrouch, Blandon, & Antonucci, Reference Ajrouch, Blandon and Antonucci2005; Fokkema, De Jong Gierveld, & Dykstra, Reference Fokkema, De Jong Gierveld and Dykstra2012; Savikko, Routasalo, Tilvis, Strandberg, & Pitkala, Reference Savikko, Routasalo, Tilvis, Strandberg and Pitkala2005). For example, Broese van Groenou and van Tilburg (Reference Broese Van Groenou and Van Tilburg2003) found that education level is associated with access to resources among older people. Persons with lower levels of education are less likely to be employed and, if employed, more likely to be employed in low-pay, no-benefit jobs. They also have lower levels of financial capability which in turn will affect negatively their economic security, well-being, and prosperity (Arrowsmith & Pignal, Reference Arrowsmith and Pignal2010).

Loneliness may be particularly affected by late-life changes in resources that result in having insufficient means to make ends meet, and that go beyond one’s capacities to manage alone (De Jong Gierveld et al., Reference De Jong Gierveld, Van Tilburg, Dykstra, Vangelisti and Perlman2006). Research in rural Canada found evidence of loneliness among longtime residents of newly affluent communities who were no longer able to take part in activities and organizations and for whom constrained financial circumstances led to truncated social connections (Keating, Eales, & Phillips, Reference Keating, Eales and Phillips2013). Such situations may become more commonplace given rising income inequality in Canada, widening gaps in home ownership, and heightened risk of poverty among older people (Statistics Canada, 2011). These changes in living environments led us to formulate hypothesis 3: low educational achievement, lack of home ownership, decreased financial resources, and unmet needs are related with higher levels of loneliness.

Social Network/Social Engagement

A core set of factors related to loneliness are the personal network of kin and non-kin relationships, and a person’s involvement in community organizations. Research has shown that the size, composition, and support exchanges within the social network are strongly associated with levels of loneliness among older adults (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2003). Hagestad (Reference Hagestad, Lancaster, Altmann, Rossi and Sherrod1987) described the socially integrative role of families, arguing that communication and historical conversations across generations help maintain continuity across life phases and strengthen one’s sense of belonging. Adult children are an important source of companionship, closeness, and sharing, particularly for those who live alone (Dykstra, Reference Dykstra1993; Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2003). Based on these findings, we formulated hypothesis 4: frequency of contact with relatives, and especially the identification of relatives as close or intimate family members, are associated with lower levels of loneliness.

The importance of non-kin, including friends and neighbours, for alleviating loneliness also is well documented (Blieszner & Adams, Reference Blieszner and Adams1992; Cacioppo & Patrick, Reference Cacioppo and Patrick2009). The joy of spending time together, the compassion evident in keeping up with personal ups and downs, and the exchange of ideas are main mechanisms in this process (Victor et al., Reference Victor, Scambler, Bond and Bowling2000). Close friends can step in and function as confidants and in doing so help alleviate loneliness, in particular for never-partnered or childless adults (Dykstra, Reference Dykstra1993; Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2003). Thus, we formulated hypothesis 5: the frequency of contacts with friends and other non-kin, and especially the identification of friends as close, are negatively associated with loneliness.

Involvement in organizations, including church and community organizations, bring people together and is a means of forming new friendships and acquaintances (Fast & De Jong Gierveld, Reference Fast, De Jong Gierveld and Keating2008; Fraser, Clayton, Sickler, & Taylor, Reference Fraser, Clayton, Sickler and Taylor2009; Rozanova, Dosman, & De Jong Gierveld, Reference Rozanova, Dosman, De Jong Gierveld and Keating2008; Väänänen, Buunk, Kivimäki, Pentti, & Vahtera, Reference Väänänen, Buunk, Kivimäki, Pentti and Vahtera2005). Moreover, researchers have found that as mutual concern for the other’s well-being and shared feelings of community embeddedness increase, the risk of loneliness decreases (Thomése, Van Tilburg, & Knipscheer, Reference Thomése, Van Tilburg and Knipscheer2003; Brown, Consedine, & Magai, Reference Brown, Consedine and Magai2005). In this context, we formulated hypothesis 6: participation in community organizations is negatively related to loneliness.

Evaluations/Satisfaction

Perceived quality of the social network has been shown to be more important in explaining loneliness than the size of the social network per se (Victor et al., Reference Victor, Scambler, Bond and Bowling2000). Routasalo, Savikko, Tilvis, Strandberg, and Pitkala (Reference Routasalo, Savikko, Tilvis, Strandberg and Pitkala2006) showed that dissatisfaction with contacts with children or friends was a more powerful predictor of loneliness than the actual frequency of contacts with children and friends. Hawkley et al. (Reference Hawkley, Hughes, Waite, Masi, Thisted and Cacioppo2008) further showed that being satisfied with network relationships had an additional protective effect independent of network size. Thus, we developed hypothesis 7: satisfaction with communication with relatives and with friends will be negatively related to loneliness.

Finally, we hypothesized that perceived balance of personal resources and constraints can influence loneliness. Edwards, Cable, Williamson, Lambert, and Shipp (Reference Edwards, Cable, Williamson, Lambert and Shipp2006) have suggested that there is much to be learned about how people combine beliefs about themselves and their environment into a sense of fit between the demands of the environment and personal resources. Life satisfaction might be considered a proxy for “demands–abilities fit” (p. 804). Thus, we proposed hypothesis 8: life satisfaction is negatively related to loneliness.

Methods

Data

Data were drawn from the public-use microdata file of Statistics Canada’s General Social Survey, Cycle 22 (GSS-22), Social Networks. The GSS sample was selected using random-digit dialing, and data were collected via computer-assisted telephone interviewing. Interviews with 20,401 Canadian men and women aged 15 or older (excluding full-time residents of institutions and residents of the northern territories) took place from February through November 2008. The response rate was 57.3 per cent. Content relevant to the current study included information on respondents’ living arrangements, contact with friends and relatives, civic participation, well-being (including loneliness), and demographic and socioeconomic background characteristics.

For purposes of this study of the determinants of loneliness among Canadian older adults, we selected 3,799 respondents aged 65 and older who had provided information to the interviewers on loneliness.

Operational Measures

GSS-22 included the short form of the De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale which we used as the dependent variable in the current study. The scale comprises 6 items, 3 of which are indicators of emotional loneliness and 3 are indicators of social loneliness (De Jong Gierveld & Kamphuis, Reference De Jong Gierveld and Kamphuis1985; De Jong Gierveld & Van Tilburg, Reference De Jong Gierveld and Van Tilburg1999, Reference De Jong Gierveld and Van Tilburg2006, Reference De Jong Gierveld and Van Tilburg2010). Total scores on the loneliness scale range from 0 (not lonely) to 6 (extremely lonely). The scale has been used extensively and found to be reliable and valid as a unidimensional measure of loneliness among older adults. (Dykstra & Fokkema, Reference Dykstra and Fokkema2007; Grygiel, Humenny, Rebisz, Świtaj, & Sikorska, Reference Grygiel, Humenny, Rebisz, Świtaj and Sikorska2013; Leung, De Jong Gierveld, & Lam, Reference Leung, De Jong Gierveld and Lam2008; Penning et al., Reference Penning, Liu and Chou2014; Pinquart & Sörensen, Reference Pinquart and Sörensen2001b).

Independent variables included personal characteristics (sex, age, health, and marital status), deprived living conditions (low level of educational attainment, lack of home ownership, diminished financial condition, and unmet needs), social network characteristics (number and contact with relatives, close relatives, friends and close friends, religious attendance, membership in community organizations), and evaluation of network quality (satisfaction with frequency of communication with relatives and friends, life satisfaction).

Personal Characteristics

Sex is a dichotomous variable (0 = male; 1 = female) whereas age is entered as a continuous variable. Health indicators included self-reported health and mental health. Both are operationalized as dichotomous variables (0 = fair or poor; 1 = excellent, very good, or good). Marital status is operationalized as a set of dummy variables: married or common law (0 = no; 1 = yes; the reference category in multivariate analyses); widowed (0 = no; 1 = yes); divorced or separated (0 = no; 1 = yes); never married (0 = no; 1 = yes).

Deprived Living Conditions

For purposes of this study, we operationalized education in the multivariate analysis as a dichotomous variable (0 = high school completion or more; 1 = less than high school completion). Home ownership was used as a proxy measure of respondents’ wealth (0 = yes; 1 = no). We also employed a variable indicating whether a recent life change had made the respondent’s financial situation worse or much worse (0 = no; 1 = yes). Finally, an indicator of unmet needs was created using a series of questions asking about changes that impacted the respondent’s life (personal, family, or work-related) experienced in the 12 months preceding the survey. Respondents also were asked whether help or resources were needed, and received or available, to cope with the most impactful change. Unmet needs was coded as one if the respondent reported that they had experienced impactful changes with which they needed help and that the help needed was unavailable; otherwise it was coded as zero.

Social Network Characteristics

We used two indicators of kin relationships: (a) number of close relatives in the respondent’s network; and (b) whether the respondent had any face-to-face and/or telephone contact with relatives in the past week. Friend relationships were similarly measured as (a) number of close friends in the respondent’s network; and (b) whether the respondent had any face-to-face and/or telephone contact with friends in the past week. Religious participation was measured as a dichotomous variable indicating whether the respondent attended church at least a few times each year (0 = no; 1 = yes), while community participation was a continuous variable measured as the number of community organizations in which respondents were actively involved.

Evaluations of Network Quality

While respondents were not asked explicitly to evaluate the quality of their social networks or contacts, they were asked how satisfied they were with the frequency with which they were in contact with members of their network of relatives and friends. These two variables were used as dichotomous variables in the multivariate analyses, indicating whether the respondent was satisfied or very satisfied (0 = yes; 1 = no). Finally, we used the answers to the question about how satisfied the respondent was with life currently as a proxy indicator of the respondent’s sense of fit between environmental demands and personal resources. This variable had 10 response categories, ranging from very dissatisfied to very satisfied. We entered the variable in the multivariate analyses as a dichotomous variable: 0 = scores 7 to 10 (satisfied), and 1 = scores 1 to 6 (not satisfied).

Analyses

We estimated weighted frequencies for categorical variables, and means and standard deviations for continuous variables, and these are reported in Table 1. We used hierarchical regression models to examine the unique contribution to variation in loneliness scores of each of the four sets of explanatory variables. Blocks of variables were entered into the models in the following order: (1) personal characteristics; (2) quality of living conditions; (3) social network characteristics; and (4) evaluation of network quality.

Results

Sample Description

Descriptive statistics for the dependent and independent variables can be found in Table 1. Loneliness scores range from 0 (not lonely) to 6 (extremely lonely) with a mean score for the sample of 1.27 (SD = 1.32). For the sample used for the current study, Cronbach’s alpha across the 6 items was .64.

Table 1: Sample description of the General Social Survey, Cycle 22, Social Networks –Respondents aged 65 or older

The sample was fairly evenly split between men and women (44.8% and 55.2% respectively). About one-third (32%) were aged 65–69, another one-quarter (24.4%) were aged 70–74, 19.6% were aged 75–79, and almost one-quarter (23.9%) were age 80 or older. Almost three-quarters (74.8%) of our respondents reported their health as good, very good, or excellent, and 93.3% reported their mental health as good, very good, or excellent. The majority of respondents were either married (65%) or widowed (24%) while small proportions were separated/divorced or never been married (7.3% and 3.7% respectively).

Almost two-thirds of respondents had completed at least high school, but more than one-third (36%) had no high school diploma or equivalent. Slightly more than one-fifth (20.8%) of respondents lived in rental housing rather than owning their own home. Only 8.8 per cent of our sample reported that their financial situation had worsened as the result of a recent life event, and an even smaller proportion (5.3%) reported having any related unmet needs.

Respondents had fairly sizable social networks comprising an average of 7.6 (SD = 11.4) close relatives and 6.9 (SD = 13.0) close friends, but 21.2 per cent and 26.9 per cent reported that they had had no in-person or voice contact with any of their family members or friends during the past week, respectively. More than one-third (38.4%) of respondents were not regular churchgoers, and they reported belonging to an average of 1.2 (SD = 1.38) community organizations.

Despite the sizable minority of respondents who reported no contact with their family members and friends in the past week, they seemed, on average, to be content with the amount of contact they had. Only 6.8 per cent and 9 per cent said they were dissatisfied or very dissatisfied with the frequency with which they had face-to-face or telephone contact with their family members and friends respectively. They were, by and large, quite happy with life overall at the time of the survey: only 14.2 per cent said they were dissatisfied or very dissatisfied.

Determinants of Loneliness

Here, we present the unstandardized and standardized coefficients, B and β, for the hierarchical regression analysis on loneliness (see Table 2) and discuss support for our hypotheses within the context of the existing literature. Each of the four blocks of variables – personal characteristics, deprived living conditions, social networks/social engagement, and satisfaction – contributed significantly to the variance in loneliness, as indicated in Table 2.

Table 2: Outcomes of hierarchical regression analysis on loneliness; Canadian men and women aged 65 and older

Source: GGS, Cycle 22

Findings for Model 1 in Table 2 illustrate the relationship between personal characteristics and loneliness. All of the personal characteristic variables were significantly related to loneliness with the exception of age. Those in poor physical and poor mental health were lonelier than those in excellent or good health. Those who were widowed, divorced or separated, or never married were significantly lonelier than those who were married, and men were significantly lonelier than women. The variables included in Model 1 explained 7 per cent of the variance in loneliness.

Model 2 illustrates the relationship between deprived living conditions and loneliness. Having less than a high school education, not owning a house, having experienced a negative change in finances, and having unmet needs were significantly related to loneliness. Adding characteristics of deprived living conditions to the model reduced the effect on loneliness of being never married, suggesting that differences in living conditions account for part of the association between being never married and loneliness. Variables included in Model 2 explained 9 per cent of the variance in loneliness.

In Model 3, social network/social engagement resources were added to the variables representing personal characteristics and deprived living conditions. All of the variables in this block were significant with the exception of religious attendance. Having a small number of close relatives and a small number of close friends both were significantly and positively related to loneliness. Respondents reporting less than weekly contact with relatives and less than weekly contact with friends were significantly more lonely. Belonging to fewer organizations was significantly and positively related to level of loneliness, but religious attendance was not. Once variables representing social network characteristics were added to the model, one dimension of deprived living conditions, owning one’s own home, became non-significant. Variables included in Model 3 explained 12 per cent of the variance in the loneliness.

Model 4 represents the full model with the addition of satisfaction variables. All of the satisfaction variables were significantly associated with loneliness. The only variables that dropped out of this final model were from the social networks/social engagement block of variables. Neither weekly contact with relatives nor number of close friends remained significant, suggesting that the influence of these variables on loneliness was at least partly explained by differences in satisfaction with these facets of the social network. The final model explained 19 per cent of the variances in loneliness.

With respect to our hypotheses, we found that patterns of significance across the four models provided information on the strength of support for our hypotheses and for our proposed conceptual framework.

The entry of personal characteristics as the first block of variables allowed us to track their explanatory value across models as blocks of variables related to other resources and to satisfaction were entered. Support for hypothesis 1 was mixed. Marital status was important although being never married was not significant in the final model, suggesting that change in marital status is a more important correlate of loneliness than is being without a partner. Gender was significant, but in the opposite direction to what was hypothesized. Being male was significantly related to loneliness across all models, suggesting a vulnerability that is not explained by access to personal, social, or contextual resources. Notably, age was not significantly associated with loneliness when other personal characteristics were taken into account. As predicted, both physical and mental health were negatively associated with loneliness (hypothesis 2).

Deprived living conditions also were significant in the final model with the exception of home ownership, providing partial support to hypothesis 3. Lower levels of education may be a proxy for lack of access to resources that provide for a comfortable living situation. In turn, losses related to financial adequacy and unmet needs indicate that transitions in living conditions are important precursors of loneliness.

Despite the fact that social networks and social engagement have comprised the core concepts in understanding loneliness in prior research, only 3 of the 6 variables in this block were significant in the final model. Hypothesized relationships between loneliness and number of close relatives and contact with relatives (hypothesis 4) operated differently than relationships with friends (hypothesis 5). In the final model, having fewer close relatives (but not recent contact with relatives) was significantly related to loneliness. In contrast, recent contact with friends (but not having fewer close friends) was significant. Perhaps because kin contact is more obligatory, loneliness is reduced only if frequency of contact is deemed satisfying, while having close kin may provide a sense that support is available if needed (Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith and Layton2010). In contrast, friendships are discretionary and hence choosing to have contact with friends is evidence of the strength of the relationship.

Hypothesis 6, that organizational involvement is negatively related to loneliness, also received only partial support. Organizational membership was significantly associated with loneliness, but church attendance was not significant across any of the models.

Finally, all of the satisfaction variables were negatively associated with loneliness. Support for hypothesis 7 (that satisfaction with communication with relatives and friends is negatively associated with loneliness) was congruent with the longstanding assumption in the loneliness literature that loneliness is an outcome of the use of “relationship standards” (Perlman & Peplau, Reference Perlman, Peplau, Gilmour and Duck1981) to evaluate kin and non-kin relationships. Additional findings that life satisfaction was negatively associated with loneliness (hypothesis 8) supported Edwards et al.’s (Reference Edwards, Cable, Williamson, Lambert and Shipp2006) contention that it is necessary to explore the broader context of person-resources fit in order to better understand quality of life of older persons. The separate contribution of life satisfaction to loneliness was an indication that resources and constraints in addition to those afforded by social network engagement were important precursors to loneliness.

Discussion

Results from this study provide insight into the contributions of personal and social resources and living conditions, and also afford insight into the evaluation of these resources to our understanding of loneliness. We highlight themes from our findings, commenting on the relative advantages and limitations of the expanded ecological model of loneliness used here.

For the most part, examining personal characteristics as an explicit element of the loneliness model confirmed previous findings about the importance of resources afforded by good health and marriage in protecting against loneliness. But this approach also highlighted two possible anomalies that warrant further investigation. The first is that age was not significant in any of the models. This finding seems at odds with previous research showing that people are more likely to have smaller social networks in advanced age (Carstensen, Reference Carstensen1995) and to be less engaged in the community (Fast & De Jong Gierveld, Reference Fast, De Jong Gierveld and Keating2008 ). However, other researchers have challenged the assumption that age is the main reason for such losses. For example, Van Tilburg and Broese van Groenou (Reference Van Tilburg and Broese van Groenou2002) found that chronic health problems or disabilities were more important than age in determining engagement in networks of kin and non-kin, and in voluntary activities. We add to this body of literature by showing that once such personal and social engagement characteristics are accounted for, age is not associated with loneliness. Challenging assumptions of age as the main driver of quality of later life continues to be important.

Gender also was significantly associated with loneliness, but men rather than women were lonelier, a finding that contradicts much of the previous research (Aartsen & Jylhä, Reference Aartsen and Jylhä2011; Steptoe, Shankar, Demakakos, & Wardle, Reference Steptoe, Shankar, Demakakos and Wardle2013), although it is in line with research by Hawkley et al. (Reference Hawkley, Hughes, Waite, Masi, Thisted and Cacioppo2008). This raises a question as to whether social gerontologists have taken for granted assumptions that aging is gendered but have not engaged sufficiently with debates about how men’s experiences differ from those of women. The so-called “double jeopardy” of being old and female has resulted in much research on how women’s aging is more problematic than men’s (Krekula, Reference Krekula2007) but not enough about factors that influence gendered identities in later life, nor about dimensions of aging that are difficult for men (McMullin & Cairney, Reference McMullin and Cairney2004).

Findings of significant relationships between transitions and loneliness point to the importance of recent empirical advancements in longitudinal and panel research that allow for examination of more-dynamic loneliness frameworks. Our findings suggest that some transitions may be more important in understanding loneliness than has been previously discussed. The relevant aspect of our findings related to deprived living conditions was that changes towards worse financial situations and unmet needs were associated with loneliness.

Overall findings that social resources of kin and friends are negatively related to loneliness supports core assumptions that these relationships are important in later life. Kin and friend networks operate differently in relation to loneliness. Family contact did not add to the explanatory value of the model, a reminder of the ambivalent aspects of family relationships (Giarrusso, Silverstein, Gans, & Bengtson, Reference Giarrusso, Silverstein, Gans, Bengtson, Johnson, Bengtson, Coleman and Kirkwood2005; Lüscher & Pillemer, Reference Lüscher and Pillemer1998). Findings that marriage protects against loneliness, but that frequency of contact with other kin does not, suggest that there is still much to learn about the nuances of family interaction and ambivalence in later life (Choudhry, Reference Choudhry2001; Connidis & McMullin, Reference Connidis and McMullin2002; De Jong Gierveld et al., Reference De Jong Gierveld, Broese van Groenou, Hoogendoorn and Smit2009).

In the introduction to this article, we posed a question about whether loneliness among older Canadians is distinct from loneliness among older adults in other parts of the world. A striking feature of our findings is the relatively low scores on loneliness. When compared to findings from the Generations and Gender Surveys of seven different countries (France, Germany, Netherlands, Russia, Bulgaria, Georgia, Japan) also using the De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale (De Jong Gierveld & Van Tilburg, Reference De Jong Gierveld and Van Tilburg2010), Canada scored lowest of all of the countries. In comparing Canadian older adults to their Czech peers, Rokach (Reference Rokach2007) observed that older adults in Canada were less likely to attribute loneliness to personal inadequacies. It may be that the cultural history of Canada, a nation of immigrants with traditions of self-reliance and independence that may be intensified by vast distances and isolation, contributes to a sense of connectedness despite the objective appearance of hardships (Keating, Swindle, & Fletcher, Reference Keating, Swindle and Fletcher2011).

The conceptual framework for this study built on earlier frameworks of loneliness by adding two sets of resources – personal characteristics and deprived living conditions – to resources afforded by social networks. Life satisfaction was added to satisfaction with social networks to capture broader evaluations of person-context fit. Each of these sets of resources and evaluations contributed to the final loneliness model.

There is a growing body of literature on the importance of transitions in personal resources in understanding loneliness. While there is considerable research showing the importance of loss of a marital relationship through widowhood or divorce (Aartsen & Jylhä, Reference Aartsen and Jylhä2011; Dykstra, Van Tilburg, & De Jong Gierveld, Reference Dykstra, Van Tilburg and De Jong Gierveld2005; Victor & Bowling, Reference Victor and Bowling2012), other transitions in marital roles, such as chronic illness of a spouse or placement of a spouse in residential care, warrant consideration. Conceptual work of Boss (Reference Boss2004) on ambiguous loss might inform this issue. For example, does the loss of a spousal relationship resulting from dementia contribute to loneliness in ways similar to other pathways to relationship loss?

Shortcomings in the data file preclude further examination of relationship issues that may influence loneliness. There is no structural information about children (e.g., numbers, ages, geographical distance, or frequency of contact), nor about supportive exchanges between parents and children. Nor is information about quality of relationships with children, spouse, and siblings available. These data gaps limit our understanding of what contributes to relationship satisfaction and ultimately to loneliness.

Transitions related to living conditions in addition to those found in this study also may be important. A number of examples of such changes have not been considered in relation to loneliness. For example, moves into supportive housing may result in disruption of existing social relationships (Adams, Sanders, & Auth, Reference Adams, Sanders and Auth2004) and thus increase the likelihood of loneliness. Other transitions in living situations may result from neighbourhood, rather than individual, changes. In research on older adults living in inner cities, Smith, Sim, Scharf, and Phillipson (Reference Smith, Sim, Scharf and Phillipson2004) found that older people who were longstanding residents of their communities became disconnected from their neighbourhoods as a result of a shifting ethnic mix. Keating et al. (Reference Keating, Eales and Phillips2013) found that longstanding residents of a newly affluent retirement community were no longer able to afford to attend social activities that previously provided venues to bring them into contact with friends.

In sum, a relatively small proportion of older Canadians report high levels of loneliness. Further research on this group who retain their satisfaction with connection to others as they navigate late-life transitions may help inform our understanding of how older adults re-calibrate the fit between their changing personal resources and environmental contexts.

References

Aartsen, M. J., & Jylhä, M. (2011). Onset of loneliness in older adults: Results of a 28 year prospective study. European Journal of Ageing, 8(1), 3138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, K. B., Sanders, S., & Auth, E. A. (2004). Loneliness and depression in independent living retirement communities: Risk and resilience factors. Aging and Mental Health, 8(6), 475485.Google Scholar
Ajrouch, K., Akiyama, H., & Antonucci, T. C. (2007). Cohort differences in social relations among the elderly. In Wahl, H.-W., Tesch-Roemer, C. & Hoff, A. (Eds.), New Dynamics in old age: Individual, environmental and societal perspectives (pp. 4363). Amityville, NY: Baywood.Google Scholar
Ajrouch, K., Blandon, A. Y., & Antonucci, T. C. (2005). Social networks among men and women: The effects of age and socioeconomic status. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 60B, S311S317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, K. R., Blieszner, R., & Roberto, K. A. (2000). Families in the middle and later years: a review and critique of research in the 1990s. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 911926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arrowsmith, S., & Pignal, G. (2010). Initial Findings from the 2009 Canadian financial Capability Survey. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Task Force on Financial Literacy.Google Scholar
Blieszner, R., & Adams, R. G. (1992). Adult friendship. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Boss, P. (2004). Ambiguous loss research, theory, and practice: Reflections after 9/11. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66, 551566.Google Scholar
Broese Van Groenou, M. I., & Van Tilburg, T. G. van (2003). Network size and support in old age: Differentials by socio-economic status in childhood and adulthood. Ageing and Society, 23(5), 625645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, W. M., Consedine, N. S., & Magai, C. (2005). Altruism relates to health in an ethnically diverse sample of older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 60B, P143P152.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 140151.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, W. (2009). Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 151156.Google Scholar
Choudhry, U. K. (2001). Uprooting and resettlement experiences of South Asian immigrant women. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 23, 376393.Google Scholar
Connidis, I. A., & McMullin, J. A. (2002). Sociological ambivalence and family ties: A critical perspective. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 558567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J. (1987). Developing and testing a model of loneliness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 119128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J., Broese van Groenou, M., Hoogendoorn, A. W., & Smit, J. H. (2009). Quality of marriages in later life and emotional and social loneliness. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Social Sciences, 64B, 497506.Google Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J., & Kamphuis, F. (1985). The development of a Rasch-type loneliness scale. Applied Psychological Measurement, 9, 289299.Google Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J., & Tesch-Römer, C. (2012). Loneliness in old age in Eastern and Western European societies: Theoretical perspectives. European Journal of Ageing, 9(4), 285295.Google Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J., & Van Tilburg, T. G. (1999). Manual of the Loneliness Scale. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: VU University.Google Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J., & Van Tilburg, T. (2006). A six-item scale for overall, emotional and social loneliness: confirmative tests on new survey data. Research on Aging, 28, 582598.Google Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J., Van Tilburg, T., & Dykstra, P. A. (2006). Loneliness and social isolation. In Vangelisti, A. & Perlman, D. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of personal relationships (pp. 485500).New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
De Jong Gierveld, J., & Van Tilburg, T. (2010). The De Jong Gierveld short scales for emotional and social loneliness: Tested on data from seven countries in the UN Generations and Gender surveys. European Journal of Ageing, 7(2), 121130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dykstra, P. A. (1993). The differential availability of relationships and the provision and effectiveness of support to older adults. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10(3), 355370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dykstra, P. A., & De Jong Gierveld, J. (2004). Gender and marital-history differences in emotional and social loneliness among Dutch older adults. Canadian Journal on Aging, 23(2), 141155.Google Scholar
Dykstra, P. A., & Fokkema, T. (2007). Social and emotional loneliness among divorced and married men and women: Comparing the deficit and cognitive perspectives. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29(1), 112.Google Scholar
Dykstra, P. A., Van Tilburg, T., & De Jong Gierveld, J. (2005). Changes in older adult loneliness: Results from a seven-year longitudinal study. Research on Aging, 27, 725747.Google Scholar
Fast, J. E., & De Jong Gierveld, J. (2008). Ageing, disability and participation. In Keating, N. C. (Ed.), Rural Ageing: A good place to grow old? (pp. 6373). Bristol, United Kingdom: The Policy Press.Google Scholar
Edwards, J. R., Cable, D. M., Williamson, I. O., Lambert, L. S., & Shipp, A. J. (2006). The phenomenology of fit: Linking the person and environment to the subjective experience of person-environment fit. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 802827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fokkema, T., De Jong Gierveld, J., & Dykstra, P. A. (2012). Cross-national differences in older adult loneliness. The Journal of Psychology, 146(12), 201228.Google Scholar
Fraser, J., Clayton, S., Sickler, J., & Taylor, A. (2009). Belonging to the zoo: Retired volunteers, conservation activism and collective identity. Ageing & Society, 29, 351368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giarrusso, R., Silverstein, M., Gans, D., & Bengtson, V. L. (2005). Ageing parents and adult children: New perspectives on intergenerational relationships. In Johnson, M. L., Bengtson, V. L., Coleman, P. G., & Kirkwood, T. B. L.. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of age and ageing (pp. 413421). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grygiel, P., Humenny, G., Rebisz, S., Świtaj, P., & Sikorska, J. (2013). Validating the Polish adaptation of the 11-item De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 29(2), 129139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagestad, G. O. (1987). Parent-child relations in later life: Trends and gaps in past research. In Lancaster, J. B., Altmann, J., Rossi, A. S., & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Parenting across the life span; Biosocial dimensions (pp. 405433). New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Havens, B., & Hall, M. (2001). Social isolation, loneliness, and the health of older adults. Indian Journal of Gerontology, 14, 144153.Google Scholar
Hawkley, L. C., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Masi, C. M., Thisted, R. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: The Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. Journal of Gerontology, Social Sciences, 63B(6), S375S384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLOS Medicine 7(7), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hortulanus, R., Machielse, A., & Meeuwesen, L. (2006). Social Isolation in Modern Society. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, N., Eales, J., & Phillips, J. (2013). Age-friendly rural communities: Conceptualizing ‘best-fit’. Canadian Journal on Aging, 32(4), 319332.Google Scholar
Keating, N., Swindle, J., & Fletcher, S. (2011). Aging in rural Canada: A retrospective and review, Canadian Journal on Aging, 30(3), 323338.Google Scholar
Korporaal, M., Broese van Groenou, M., & Van Tilburg, T. G. (2008). Effects of own and spousal disability on loneliness among older adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 20(3), 306325.Google Scholar
Krekula, C. (2007). The intersection of age and gender: Reworking gender theory and social gerontology. Current Sociology, 55(2), 155171.Google Scholar
Kuyper, L., & Fokkema, T. (2010). Loneliness among older lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults: the role of minority stress. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(5), 11711180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lauder, W., Mummery, K., Jones, M., & Caperchione, C. (2006). A comparison of health behaviours in lonely and non-lonely populations. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 11(2), 233245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawton, M.P., & Nahemow, L. (1973). Ecology and the aging process. In Eisdorfer, C. & Lawton, M. P. (Eds.), Psychology of Adult Development and Aging (pp. 657668). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Leung, G. T. Y., De Jong Gierveld, J., & Lam, L. C. W. (2008). Validation of the Chinese translation of the 6-item De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale in elderly Chinese. International Psychogeriatrics, 20(6), 12621272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüscher, K., & Pillemer, K. (1998). Intergenerational ambivalence: A new approach to the study of parent-child relations in later life. Journal of Marriage and Family, 60(2), 413425.Google Scholar
McMullin, J., & Cairney, J. (2004). Self-esteem and the intersection of age, class, and gender. Journal of Aging Studies, 18, 7590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penning, M. J., Liu, G., & Chou, P. H. B. (2014). Measuring loneliness among middle-aged and older adults: The UCLA and de Jong Gierveld loneliness scales. Social Indicators Research., 118, 11471166. doi10.1007/s11205-013-0461-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1981). Toward a social psychology of loneliness. In Gilmour, R. & Duck, S. (Eds.), Personal relationships 3: Personal relationships in disorder. (pp. 3143). London, United Kingdom: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pinquart, M. (2003). Loneliness in married, widowed, divorced, and never-married older adults. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 3153.Google Scholar
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2001a). Gender differences in self-concept and psychological well-being in old age: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology B, 56(4), 195213.Google Scholar
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2001b). Influences on loneliness in older adults: A meta-analysis. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23(4), 245266.Google Scholar
Rokach, A. (2007). The effect of age and culture on the causes of loneliness. Social Behavior and Personality, 35, 169186.Google Scholar
Routasalo, P. E., Savikko, N., Tilvis, R. S., Strandberg, T. E., & Pitkala, K. H. (2006). Social contacts and their relationships to loneliness among aged people: A population-based study. Gerontology, 52(3), 181187.Google Scholar
Rozanova, J., Dosman, D., & De Jong Gierveld, J. (2008). Participation in rural contexts: community matters. In Keating, N. C. (Ed.), Rural Ageing: A good place to grow old? (pp. 7586). Bristol, UK: Policy Press.Google Scholar
Savikko, N., Routasalo, P. E., Tilvis, R. S., Strandberg, T. E., & Pitkala, K. H. (2005). Predictors and subjective causes of loneliness in an aged population. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 41, 223233.Google Scholar
Smith, A. E., Sim, J., Scharf, T., & Phillipson, C. (2004). Determinants of quality of life amongst older people in deprived neighbourhoods. Ageing and Society, 24, 793814.Google Scholar
Statistics Canada (2011). Canada Year Book 2011. Families, households and housing. Retrieved 30, Sept 2011 fromhttp://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/11-402-x/2011000/chap/fam/fam-eng.htm.Google Scholar
Steptoe, A., Shankar, A., Demakakos, P., & Wardle, J. (2013). Social isolation, loneliness, and all-cause mortality in older men and women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 110(15), 57975801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomése, F., Van Tilburg, T., & Knipscheer, C. P. M. (2003). Continuation of exchange with neighbors in later life: The importance of the neighborhood context. Personal Relationships, 10, 535550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Väänänen, A., Buunk, B. P., Kivimäki, M., Pentti, J., & Vahtera, J. (2005). When it is better to give than to receive: Long-term health effects of perceived reciprocity in support exchange. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(2), 176193.Google Scholar
Van Tilburg, T., & Broese van Groenou, M. (2002). Network and health changes among older Dutch adults. Journal of Social Issues, 58(4), 697713.Google Scholar
Van Tilburg, T., Havens, B., & De Jong Gierveld, J. (2004). Loneliness among older adults in the Netherlands, Italy, and Canada: A multifaceted comparison. Canadian Journal on Aging, 23(2), 169180.Google Scholar
Victor, C., Scambler, S., Bond, J., & Bowling, A. (2000). Being alone in later life: Loneliness, social isolation and living alone. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 10(4), 407417.Google Scholar
Victor, C. R., & Bowling, A. (2012). A longitudinal analysis of loneliness among older people in Great Britain. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 146(3), 313331.Google Scholar
Wahl, H.-W., Iwarsson, S., & Oswald, F. (2012). Aging well and the environment: Toward an integrative model and research agenda for the future. The Gerontologist, 52(3), 306316.Google Scholar
Waite, L. J., & Lehrer, E. L. (2003). The benefits from marriage and religion in the United States: A comparative analysis. Population and Development Review, 29(2), 255275.Google Scholar
Warner, D. F., & Kelley-Moore, J. (2012). The social context of disablement among older adults: Does marital quality matter for loneliness? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 53(1), 5066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Z., & Penning, M. J. (2015). Immigration and loneliness in later life. Ageing & Society, 35(1), 6495). doi:10.1017/S0144686X13000470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1: Sample description of the General Social Survey, Cycle 22, Social Networks –Respondents aged 65 or older

Figure 1

Table 2: Outcomes of hierarchical regression analysis on loneliness; Canadian men and women aged 65 and older