Informed consent has passed through three stages. The first paternalistic stage lasted for many centuries: The doctor's diagnosis and healing arts were kept secret, and informing patients was regarded as professionally and ethically wrong. Second came the legal stage, when the right of patients to make informed decisions concerning their own treatment was imposed by the courts and reluctantly tolerated by medical professionals. The third informed consent stage emerged more recently: the general therapy stage. The therapeutic benefits of informed consent have been well established, and informed consent is widely recognized as an important element in sound medical practice. When patients are effectively informed and can exert knowledgeable control over their own treatment decisions and therapy processes, that enhances recovery,1
As shown by Brody DS, Miller SM, Lerman CE, Smith DG, Caputo GC. Patient perception of involvement in medical care: Relationship to illness attitudes and outcomes. Journal of General Internal Medicine 1989;4:506–11; Mahler HIM, Kulik JA. Preferences for health care involvement, perceived control and surgical recovery: A prospective study. Social Science and Medicine 1990;31:743–51.
Kamen-Siegel L, Rodin J, Seligman MEP, Dwyer J. Explanatory style and cell-mediated immunity in elderly men and women. Health Psychology 1991;10:229–35; Rodin J. Aging and health: Effects of the sense of control. Science 1986;233:1271–6.
Arnstein P, Caudill M, Mandle C, Norris R, Beasley R. Self-efficacy as a mediator of the relationship between pain intensity, disability and depression in chronic pain patients. Pain 1999;80:483–91; Bowers KS. Pain, anxiety, and perceived control. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1968;32:596–602; Hill HF, Mackie AM, Coda BA, Iverson K, Chapman CR. Patient controlled analgesic administration: A comparison of steady-state morphine infusions with bolus doses. Cancer 1991;67:873–82.
Joekes K, Van Elderen T, Schreurs K. Self-efficacy and overprotection are related to quality of life, psychological well-being and self-management in cardiac patients. Journal of Health Psychology 2007;12:4–16; Mirowsky J, Ross CE. Control or defense? Depression and sense of control over good and bad outcomes. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 1990;31:71–86; Taylor SE. Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist 1983;38:1161–73.
Among the many studies are Arras RE, Ogletree RJ, Welshimer KJ. Health-promoting behaviors in men age 45 and above. International Journal of Men's Health 2006;5:65–79; Kaplan RM, Atkins CJ, Reinsch S. Specific efficacy expectations mediate excerise compliance in patients with COPD. Health Psychology 1984;3:223–42; Wallston KA. Psychological control and its impact in the management of rheumatological disorders. Bailliere's Clinical Rheumatology 1993;7:281–95; Williams KE, Bond MJ. The roles of self-efficacy, outcome expectancies and social support in the self-care behaviours of diabetics. Psychology, Health & Medicine 2002;7:127–41.
The widespread ethical and therapeutic acceptance of informed consent is a positive development, but there are dangers. Like many positive therapeutic processes, informed consent can cause harm if administered carelessly. Informed consent is a valuable element of medical therapy, and good medical therapy is not a one-size-fits-all procedure. The question of what a “reasonable person” would want to know is no more helpful than asking whether a generic “reasonable person” would benefit from penicillin. To gain the full benefits of informed consent and avoid potential harms, conscientious medical professionals must move beyond the general therapy stage of informed consent to an individual therapy level of informed consent, in which—like other elements of good medical therapy—informed consent is tailored to the individual patient. Conscientious medical professionals are already accustomed to offering information in a vocabulary and at a level of discourse appropriate to the patient's background and understanding. The fourth stage of individual therapy informed consent makes additional requirements on the informed consent process. Those requirements entail examining some important psychological characteristics of individual patients, along three distinct but related dimensions.
First, physicians must determine what coping style6
Described by Krohne HW. Individual differences in coping. In: Zeidner M, Endler NS, eds. Handbook of Coping: Theory, Research, Applications. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons; 1996:391–409; Miller SM. Monitoring versus blunting styles of coping with cancer influence the information patients want and need about their disease: Implications for cancer screening and management. Cancer 2006;76:167–77.
Andrew JM. Recovery from surgery with and without preparatory instruction for three coping styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1970;15:223–6.
See note 6, Miller 2006; Miller SE, Mangan CE. Interaction effects of information and coping style in adapting to gynaecologic stress: Should the doctor tell all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1983;45:223–36.
Dealing effectively with patient coping styles is an important but challenging process. It is complicated by the fact that the blunting style involves problems of its own. Monitors are more likely to take positive steps toward health promotion and disease detection,9
Van Zuuren FJ, Dooper R. Coping style and self-reported health promotion and disease detection behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology 1999;4:81–9.
Olff M, Brosschot JF, Godaert G. Coping styles and health. Personality and Individual Differences 1993;15:81–90.
Crisson JE, Keefe FJ. The relationship of locus of control to pain coping strategies and psychological distress in chronic pain patients. Pain 1988;35:147–54.
Studies include Bombardier CH, D'Amico C, Jordan JS. The relationship of appraisal and coping to chronic illness adjustment. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1990;28:297–304; Carver CS, Scheier M. Vigilant and aviodant coping in two patient groups. In: Krohne HW, ed. Attention and Avoidance: Strategies in Coping with Aversiveness. Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber; 1993:295–319; see note 10, Olff et al., 1993.
Described in Feifel H, Strack S, Nagy VT. Coping strategies and associated features of medically ill patients. Psychosomatic Medicine 1987;49:616–25; Suls J, Fletcher B. The relative efficacy of avoidant and nonavoidant coping strategies: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology 1985;4:249–88.
Examples include Bar-Tal Y, Spitzer A. The effect on coping of monitoring, blunting, and the ability to achieve cognitive structure. Journal of Psychology 1999;133:395–412; Ben-Zur H. Monitoring/blunting and social support: Associations with coping and affect. International Journal of Stress Management 2002;9:357–73.
Individualized informed consent requires screening patients to determine whether they employ a blunting style of coping: a coping style that makes some kinds of information potentially harmful to the patient. The best informed consent process with a blunting patient will be very different from the process that would occur with a patient who is an active monitor.15
As noted in Miller SM, Fang CY, Diefenbach MA, Bales C. Tailoring psychosocial intervention to the individual's health information processing style: The influence of monitoring versus blunting in cancer risk and disease. In: Baum A, Andersen B, eds. Psychosocial Interventions in Cancer. Washington, DC: APA, 2001:342–62.
Careless use of informed consent, without adequate attention to the individual needs of the particular patient, may undermine a patient's preferred coping style. That is the first problem with general therapy “one-size-fits-all” informed consent, and the first reason to move toward a model of individual therapy informed consent. The second danger concerns the locus of control16
Described in Rotter JB. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs 1966;80(whole no. 609); Rotter JB. Some problems and misconceptions related to the construct of internal vs. external control of reinforcement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1975;43:56–67.
It is very important that the physician recognize the locus of control of her patient when tailoring the informed consent process to fit the therapeutic needs of the individual patient. If a strongly external patient feels that medical professionals are placing the decisionmaking weight on his shoulders, though he can't effectively exert control, that will be very frightening. Furthermore, it is important to go further with patients who are external, and determine whether they believe control of events is set by fate or chance or instead by powerful others. In the right circumstances—when patients are confident of the expertise and good will of their physicians—externals who believe in control by powerful others fare almost as well as internals. It is obviously of great importance that such a patient regard his physician as strong, highly competent, and committed to the patient's welfare. This does not mean that the physician must pretend to knowledge of nonexistent medical certainties, much less that the physician should make false promises or cover up risks; but with externals who believe that control is in the hands of powerful others, the physician is justified in offering a stronger and more confident recommendation that “in my considered opinion, and with my many years of experience with cases like yours,” this specific course of treatment is best.
When dealing with external patients who believe that control rests with powerful others, Howard Brody's transparency standard17
Brody H. Transparency: Informed consent in primary care. Hastings Center Report 1989;19:506–11.
The first danger from the general therapy informed consent model is the danger of undercutting the patient's preferred coping method; second is the danger of imposing a frightening decisionmaking burden on an external patient who believes that he cannot control such vital decisions. The third danger concerns strongly internal patients, who believe that what happens is really up to them and under their control. Patients with an internal locus of control generally do better—in recovery, positive health behavior, avoidance of depression—than do externals. There are, however, some important exceptions. The problems emerge for internal patients who have a weak sense of perceived self-efficacy. Self-efficacy18
See Bandura A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavior change. Psychological Review 1977;84:191–215; Bandura A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist 1982;37:122–47; Bandura A. Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: W.H. Freeman; 1997.
Making competent choices for myself is satisfying and healthy, but making important choices while feeling painfully inadequate is stressful and even terrifying. Thus the third danger of general therapy informed consent is the danger to an internal patient who—because of her weak sense of self-efficacy—is wrongly characterized as an external, and thus “chooses” not to exercise the full range of control that (with adequate support and information and confidence) the patient prefers to exercise and would benefit from exercising.19
As noted in Pucheu S, Consoli SM, D'Auzac C, Francais P, Issad B. Do health causal attributions and coping strategies act as moderators of quality of life in peritoneal dialysis patients? Journal of Psychosomatic Research 2004;56:317–22.
The fourth danger is closely connected with the third. It is unfortunate to be deprived of desired effective control because of one's sense of incompetence and low self-efficacy; it is still worse to feel that one is in control and that what happens is unavoidably in one's own hands, but one is incompetent to exercise that control intelligently and effectively.20
As described by Litt MD. Cognitive mediators of stressful experience: Self-efficacy and perceived control. Cognitive Therapy and Research 1988;12:241–60.
It is essential to provide patients with sufficient information, in a form they can understand, but it is also important to keep in mind that even when patients have such information they may not feel confident to make decisions. Strengthening or restoring a patient's sense of health self-efficacy is not always an easy task: Anyone who has tried to tutor a bright and capable student who lacks an adequate sense of mathematical self-efficacy will appreciate the challenges. If the patient's perceived self-efficacy can be strengthened, then the patient's autonomy as well as the patient's therapeutic success is significantly enhanced. As patients gain more information and experience success in making decisions and managing their own care, they may gradually gain a stronger sense of health self-efficacy.21
A sample of such experimental findings are Bandura A, Schunk DH. Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1981;41:586–98; Davis AHT, Carrieri-Kohlman V, Janson SL, Gold WM, Stulbart MS. Effects of treatment on two types of self-efficacy in people with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 2006;32:60–70; Lorig K, Holman H. Self-management education: History, definition, outcomes and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 2003;26:1–7; Wright C, Barlow J, Turner A, Bancroft G. Self-management training for people with chronic disease: An exploratory study. British Journal of Health Psychology 2003;8:465–76.
Informed consent is a basic requirement for dealing ethically with patients: a requirement mandated by the obligation to honor the autonomous choice-making capacities of patients. But informed consent is also a positive element of good therapy; as such, it should be handled with caution and tailored to the individual patient. Individual therapy informed consent requires medical professionals to recognize the salient psychological characteristics that are key determinants of appropriate informed consent practices (coping style, locus of control, and level of self-efficacy) and shape decisions concerning informed consent procedures to the relevant characteristics of individual patients. None of that justifies forgoing or compromising the patient's basic right of informed consent. But honoring that right—and achieving the best ethical and therapeutic results—requires honoring the individuality of the patient.
Determining the relevant psychological characteristics of patients is a significant task, but by no means an impossible one. There are relatively short tests available that can be given to patients, perhaps as part of their admission profile. For coping style, brief available tests include the Threatening Medical Situations Inventory (TMSI)22
Van Zuuren FJ, de Groot KI, Mulder NL, Muris P. Coping with medical threat: An evaluation of the threatening medical situations inventory (TMSI). Personality & Individual Differences 1996;21:21–31.
Miller SM. Monitoring and blunting: Validation of a questionnaire to assess styles of information seeking under threat. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 1987;52:345–53; Miller SM, Combs C, Kruus L. Tuning in and tuning out: Confronting the effects of confrontation. In: Krohne HW, ed. Attention and Avoidance: Strategies in Coping with Aversiveness. Seattle: Hogrefe and Huber; 1993:51–69.
Watkins LO, Weaver L, Odegaard V. Preparation for cardiac catherization: Tailoring the content of instruction to coping style. Heart and Lung 1986;15:382–9.
Wallston KA, Wallston BS. Health locus of control scales. In: Lefcourt H, ed. Research with a Locus of Control Construct. New York: Academic Press; 1981:189–243; Wallston KA, Wallston BS, DeVellis R. Development of the multidimensional health locus of control (MHLC) scales. Health Education Monographs 1978;6:160–70.
Sherer M, Maddux JE, Mercadante B, Prentice-Dunn S, Jacobs B, Rogers RW. The self-efficacy scale: Construction and validation. Psychological Reports 1982;51:663–71.
Patients with an internal locus of control, a monitoring style of coping, and a strong sense of self-efficacy are the patients most likely to benefit from a rich, full, informative discussion of prognosis, treatment options, and likely outcomes. Such patients are also most likely to make good use of the opportunity to exercise choice and control in selecting a program of treatment and following that treatment program as full partners with their health professionals. At the other end of the spectrum is the patient who favors a blunting style of coping, has an external (even fatalistic) locus of control, and suffers from a weak sense of self-efficacy—perhaps an especially weak sense of health self-efficacy. We might well conclude that the latter patient enjoys considerably less autonomy than does the former. It would still be wrong, both ethically and therapeutically, to treat the latter patient paternalistically. First, paternalistic treatment violates the patient's autonomy. A patient with a weak sense of self-efficacy and an external locus of control has only minimal autonomy, true enough; but respect for autonomy is not limited to those with the richest autonomy resources. Second, such paternalistic treatment will solidify the problematic perspectives. Medical professionals do not have the right to change the personalities of people against their wishes, even for their own good; but neither should they adopt policies that exacerbate problematic psychological characteristics. A weak sense of control, combined with weak self-efficacy, causes problems: It is associated with depression, compromised immune function, slower recovery, and poor health behavior. Treating such patients paternalistically—not respecting them as persons who have their own values and are still autonomous (even if not as robustly autonomous as some others)—is likely to entrench those unfortunate characteristics and make a bad situation worse. When physicians recognize the relevant psychological characteristics of their patients, that makes it possible for patients to gain the knowledge that is most appropriate for their needs and interests and enables patients to make judgments—or at the very least, freely acquiesce in the considered judgment of a respected and trusted physician—and thereby exercise genuine informed consent; and respect for limited autonomy may be one way of enlarging and strengthening autonomy. Thus even patients with very weak psychological resources can and should practice giving informed consent. Third, involving the patient of limited autonomy resources in a rudimentary and nonthreatening exercise of autonomy may be a means of preventing that patient from sliding into deeper learned helplessness,27
Seligman MEP. Helplessness: On Depression, Development, and Death. New York: W.H. Freeman; 1975.
Informed consent is fundamentally the recognition of the autonomous patient's right to make her own decisions concerning medical treatment. But informed consent is also a positive therapeutic resource when used correctly and a significant source of potential harm when handled carelessly. Having recognized both the ethical and therapeutic importance of informed consent, the medical community must now invest the care and thought required to gain the benefits and avoid the perils of informed consent. Those in health psychology have an opportunity to make relevant psychological research readily accessible to practicing physicians and to devise tests that can be easily administered and adequately understood by physicians who are learning to deal with the vitally important psychological dimensions of treatment that is therapeutically sound and ethically legitimate.