Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T16:11:59.478Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Response of Aphidius colemani to aphid sex pheromone varies depending on plant synergy and prior experience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2015

G.M. Fernández-Grandon*
Affiliation:
Centre for Biological Sciences, University of Southampton, Southampton, Hampshire, UK, SO17 1BJ
G.M. Poppy
Affiliation:
Centre for Biological Sciences, University of Southampton, Southampton, Hampshire, UK, SO17 1BJ
*
*Author for correspondence Phone: +44 (0)1634 88 3057 E-mail: mandelagrandon@yahoo.co.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A critical stage in the success of a parasitoid is the ability to locate a host within its habitat. It is hypothesized that a series of olfactory cues may be involved in altering the parasitoid's movement patterns at this stage of foraging. This paper focuses specifically on host habitat location and host location and the olfactory stimuli necessary to mediate the transition between these stages. Firstly, we confirm the ability of the parasitoid Aphidius colemani to detect the aphid sex pheromone at an electrophysiological level. Following this we investigate the effect of the sex pheromone component (4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactone on the movement patterns of A. colemani and its retention within an area. The key findings of this work are that A. colemani is able to detect the sex pheromone components, that parasitoid retention is increased by a synergy of nepetalactone and other host-associated cues and that foraging patterns are augmented by the presence of nepetalactone or experience associated with nepetalactone.

Type
Research Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Introduction

One of the main challenges facing a parasitoid is locating a host in a stimulus-rich environment. For parasitoids, like many other insects, olfaction plays an important role in host location and host-habitat location (Wäschke et al., Reference Wäschke, Hardge, Hancock, Hilker, Obermaier and Meiners2014). Having an acute sense of smell is critical for the success of a parasitoid in locating its host but means that any given environment will appear populated with a wide range of stimuli. Part of the challenge for the foraging parasitoid will be to distinguish the odours which relate to host presence and adjust their behaviour accordingly to minimize the energy expenditure in locating the host.

The process of host location in a multitrophic environment is frequently viewed as a series of stages. Divisions of the stages of host selection are often defined either by a hierarchy of cues, or the order in which they are encountered (Alphen & Vet, Reference Alphen, Vet, Waage and Greathead1986; Wellings, Reference Wellings, Bailey and Ridsdill-Smith1991). One of the most widely adopted categorization systems is that proposed by Vinson (Reference Vinson1976). Vinson describes five distinct stages in host selection: host habitat location, host location, host acceptance, host suitability and regulation. Our focus in this study is the transition between the initial two stages: host habitat location and host location.

To understand the transition between host habitat location and host location we must also consider the reliability–detectability theory (Vet & Dicke, Reference Vet and Dicke1992). This theory identifies that while odours more distantly associated with the host may be more abundant, those most closely related with the host, while more discrete, will offer greater reliability. We speculate that less reliable host habitat odours (plant volatiles) will be the principal cue used in host habitat location but more reliable host cues will be required to initiate host location. For this reasons we selected the use of the aphid sex pheromone in this study. Aphid sex pheromones from a range of species have been shown to comprise (1R,4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactol and (4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactone (for full list see Hardie & Minks, Reference Hardie and Minks1999). In a natural environment female oviparae will produce the pheromone from glands on the metathoracic tibia (Marsh, Reference Marsh1975) and wave their legs to increase exposure of the glands for the male aphids to detect (Pettersson, Reference Pettersson1970). The odour is detected by a range of aphid parasitoids (see Powell & Pickett, Reference Powell and Pickett2003) in addition to lacewings (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Sheng, Chen, Aldrich and Chauhan2006) and ladybirds (Leroy et al., Reference Leroy, Schillings, Farmakidis, Heuskin, Lognay, Verheggen, Brostaux, Haubruge and Francis2012) which predate on aphids. Although aphid populations are often asexual, it is hypothesized that the strong response seen by parasitoids is because the critical timing at which aphid populations produce the pheromone which may be the final chance for the parasitoid to find an overwintering host (Hardie et al., Reference Hardie, Nottingham, Powell and Wadhams1991; Wadhams et al., Reference Wadhams, Birkett, Powell, Woodcock, Chadwick and Goode1999).

Integral to parasitoid foraging success is the learning plasticity displayed throughout their life cycle. Although odour preferences may be acquired in the early stages of the parasitoid life cycle they are frequently adjusted by experiences of associative learning which the adult will gain through successful oviposition (Storeck et al., Reference Storeck, Poppy, Emden and Powell2000). Odours which already exist in the wasp's chemical catalogue may elicit an increased responsiveness (Turlings et al., Reference Turlings, Wäckers, Vet, Lewis, Tumlinson, Papaj and Lewis1993) such as seen in alpha conditioning. It has previously been hypothesized that nepetalactone, which is considered a highly reliable cue, will not be subject to alpha conditioning (Glinwood et al., Reference Glinwood, Du and Powell1999). Using a stimulus to which the parasitoid potentially already holds an innate response offers an exciting opportunity to explore the extent of associative learning and address the reliability–detectability hypothesis with regard to changes in foraging behaviour. Experience in this study is provided via associative learning with the sex pheromone which was not present in any previous stage of the individual's life cycle.

The high reliability of aphid sex pheromone makes it a good candidate for the study of behavioural changes during foraging but it could also have application in biological control systems. Work has previously shown that the spatial distribution of aphid parasitoids is altered by the presence of nepetalactone (Powell et al., Reference Powell, A'Hara, Harling, Holl, Northing, Thomas and Walters2004). Although the compounds are detected by asexual aphids (Fernández-Grandon et al., Reference Fernández-Grandon, Woodcock and Poppy2013), there is no evidence that it would alter their behaviour or population dynamics. Laboratory bioassays have shown an innate behavioural response to nepetalactone for the parasitoid Aphidius ervi (Poppy et al., Reference Poppy, Powell and Pennacchio1997). The prevalence of this behavioural response is supported by field work by Glinwood et al., (Reference Glinwood, Powell and Tripathi1998) which has shown that parasitism by Praon volucre and Aphidius rhopalosiphi can be increased around vials of nepetalactone. Despite being one of the most commonly used parasitoids in the biological control of aphids, it is not known whether the aphid parasitoid Aphidius colemani Viereck (Hymenoptera: Aphidiinae) responds to components of the sex pheromone.

Aphidius colemani is a generalist parasitoid frequently employed in the control of a range of glasshouse pests. It is a favourable control agent due to its short life cycle, high reproductive potential, ability to parasitize several aphid species and the relative ease with which they can be mass reared. As members of Hymenoptera they display a high aptitude for learning, this ability to learn combined with their need to oviposit in a suitable host to ensure reproduction make them an ideal candidate for studying foraging behaviours. A study by Ameixa & Kindlmann (Reference Ameixa and Kindlmann2012) has shown that the behaviour of the parasitoid can be altered by Nepeta cataria oil, which contains the sex pheromone components, however no confirmation of a response to the specific compounds was provided.

The aims of this research were to determine the ability of A. colemani to detect pheromone components, if there are synergies with the pheromone which alter their behaviour and if the pheromone plays a role in transitioning from host habitat location to host location.

To achieve this we implemented electroantennography to test A. colemani response the compounds (1R,4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactol and (4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactone which comprise the aphid sex pheromone. Following this we explored, through retention of the parasitoid, if there are any synergies with the component nepetalactone and plant or host volatiles. Finally, using tracking software, we identified the exact locomotory response of the parasitoid in various conditions to reveal changes associated with host foraging.

Materials and methods

Insects

Peach–potato aphids, Myzus persicae (Sulzer) (Hemiptera: Aphididae), were reared on Chinese cabbage Brassica rapa spp. pekinensis cv. Wong Bok as asexual virginoparae morphs. The generalist parasitoid A. colemani were maintained on these M. persicae colonies for use in electrophysiological work. While reproducing asexually, M. persicae do not produce any sex pheromone (Hardie et al., Reference Hardie, Holyoak, Nicholas, Nottingham, Pickett, Wadhams and Woodcock1990), therefore any treatments used in this study represent the parasitoids’ first encounter with the aphid sex pheromone. For all behavioural assays, A. colemani were provided by Koppert, B.V. (Netherlands) from a different host/plant rearing background (K. Bolckmans, personal communication). Aphidius colemani were used directly from supply meaning they had no experience on our host/plant system prior to the assays.

Electrophysiology

Electroantennogram (EAG) recordings from female A. colemani were made using Ag–AgCl glass electrodes filled with saline solution (composition as in Maddrell, Reference Maddrell1969 but without glucose). The parasitoid was anaesthetized by chilling after which the head was excised and the most distal antenna tips removed. The excised head was placed within the indifferent electrode, with the antennae protruding. Both antennae were inserted into the recording electrode. The signals were passed through a high impedance amplifier (UN-06, Syntech, Netherlands) and analysed using a customized software package (Syntech). The delivery system, which employed a filter paper in a disposable Pasteur pipette cartridge, has been described previously (Wadhams et al., Reference Wadhams, Angst and Blight1982). The stimulus (2 s duration) was delivered into a purified air stream (1 l min−1) flowing continuously over the preparation.

Samples (10 μl) of the nepetalactone and nepetalactol, diluted in hexane (50 μg ml−1, 1 and 10 mg ml−1), were applied to filter paper strips and the solvent was allowed to evaporate (30 s) before the strip was placed in a disposable Pasteur pipette cartridge. Hexane was used as a control stimulus. Fresh cartridges were prepared immediately prior to use. EAG responses to the two sex pheromone components were standardized to the response to hexane, applied at the beginning and end of each run, following the methodology used by Park et al. (Reference Park, Zhu, Harris, Ochieng and Baker2001) to account for signal deterioration. A total of five individuals were tested, each representing an independent replicate.

On-leaf retention time

In a natural environment specific chemical compounds are not found in isolation and normally form only a part of the total blend an insect will detect at a given time. It is likely that the response of the parasitoid will depend on a combination of these odours, vibrational and visual cues that it receives. The on-leaf retention time assay allowed an evaluation of latency to leaving the leaf under different conditions. This assay allowed us to investigate potential synergies between plant volatiles, aphid associated odours and nepetalactone in addition to the standardized visual stimulus of a leaf surface.

Leaves were excised from 3-week-old Chinese cabbage. Each leaf was placed in a small plastic ‘Blackman box’ measuring 7.5 × 4.4 × 2.2 cm3. These boxes allow for the leaf stem to pass into another area where it is kept moist by a damp sponge, allowing a greatly improved longevity for excised leaves (for full description see Blackman, Reference Blackman1971). The box was rested in a shallow basin of water, allowing the leaf to receive moisture through a sponge in the base. Treatments described as empty had no leaf in the box and no odour stimuli present. Those described as previously infested had ten aphids (3rd instar to adult) placed on the leaf using a size 0 paintbrush and allowed to feed. After feeding for 16 h, when the experiment was to be conducted, the aphids were gently removed from the leaves but any exuviae and honeydew was allowed to remain. The treatment involving only aphid-related cues was achieved by allowing the aphids to feed on the leaf for 3 days, after which time they were removed (though honeydew and exuviae remained) and the leaf was allowed 24 h with no aphid attack to allow volatile production to diminish (Du et al., Reference Du, Poppy and Powell1996, Guerrieri et al., Reference Guerrieri, Pennacchio and Tremblay1997). The systemically damaged plants were created by allowing 20 aphids, contained within a clip cage, to feed on a leaf on the plant for 72 h. A different leaf from the same plant was then used in the assay.

A female A. colemani naïve to the sex pheromone and the plant/host complex was placed on the centre of the leaf surface and latency to leaving the area was recorded. It was important that a replicate for all treatments in a set could be completed within a day to reduce temporal and cohort behavioural variation. For this reason the treatments were split into two distinct sets with the first covering a broader range of potential cues and the second focusing on those showing the greatest parasitoid retention. In Set 1, the parasitoid was allowed 15 min before the assay was completed; in Set 2 each parasitoid was allowed 20 min. The extended time allowance was used because the odours present in Set 2 were considered to be of greater reliability therefore increasing the time before the parasitoid would leave the area. If the parasitoid did stay longer as a result of these odours, 15 min would not have been sufficient to distinguish treatments. The assay was stopped if the parasitoid walked or flew out of the Blackman box. The time of leaving the leaf or central area, when no leaf was present, was also recorded giving an accurate measure of retention time, or latency of period before leaving the leaf/area. Treatments using nepetalactone had the compound introduced via a 10 μl microcapillary (Drummond Scientific Co., USA) affixed to the side of the Blackman box. The standard concentration of nepetalactone used was 1 mg ml−1 and the high concentration was 10 mg ml−1 providing an approximate release rate of 0.3 and 3 μg min−1, respectively. In both sets of the assay 35 replicates were completed, each representing a separate individual leaf, Blackman box and female aged 2–4 days old. The bioassay room was maintained at 20 ± 1°C.

Motivation assay

The motivation assay arena was constructed of a Petri dish 9 cm in diameter (Sterilin Limited, UK) with a 2 cm × 2 cm square removed from the centre. The central opening was covered with gauze and a 3 cm diameter Petri dish was affixed to the underside. One centimetre to the side of the square, a circle was cut of approximately 0.5 cm diameter which provided an entry point for the parasitoid from below the assay arena (fig. 1). The larger upper dish formed the assay arena, whereas the smaller dish on the underside was the odour source chamber. The parasitoids were provided with a wick of 20% honey solution and kept in a glass vial in the controlled environment 30 min before the start of the experiment to allow them to acclimate. A glass covering prevented parasitoids from flying out of the arena but still allowed for a recording of the assay to be made from above using a TK-C1360B colour camera (JVC). Experiments were conducted during the most active part of the insect's photophase (08.00–12.00) under ~1.7 kilolux of lighting using a lamp with a diffusion light filter. The bioassay room was maintained at 20 ± 1°C. The lid of a vial containing a parasitoid was placed in the 0.5 cm diameter hole in the base of the arena, once the lid was affixed to the entry point the recording commenced. The behaviour of the parasitoid was recorded for 8 min with the intention of identifying differences in movement speeds and frequency of turning to indicate changes in foraging behaviour. Particular differences in behaviour which may indicate a switch to foraging would be a reduction in locomotion and increase in turning. Our null hypothesis was that none of the treatments used would alter the movement speed or pattern of the parasitoids when compared with values seen in the control group.

Fig. 1. Motivation assay arena. A 9 cm diameter Petri dish forms the upper area of the arena which is rested on a 3 cm diameter Petri dish (1). Odour moves passively into the arena through the 2 × 2 cm gauze-covered opening (2). Aphidius colemani are introduced via the 0.5 cm circular opening which accommodates a small insect storage vial (3). The smaller Petri dish is used (4) to contain the treatment while preventing the parasitoid from contacting the source. A glass covering is placed over the apparatus to prevent escape of the insects while allowing recording.

Recordings were not analysed in real time but were recorded onto DVD and Ethovision tracking software (v 3.1 Noldus Information Technology) was applied later during playback to track the movement of the insect within the arena. Oviposition experience was provided by giving each parasitoid 5 min to freely oviposit in a Petri dish on a leaf containing ten aphids of mixed instars. At least one oviposition attempt had to be observed for the wasp to be used in the study. All parasitoids were naïve in that they emerged entirely in an isolated laboratory environment and therefore had no previous adult encounter with aphids or the sex pheromone prior to the assays. A full listing of the treatments used can be seen in table 1. Pre-treatments were administered immediately before commencing the assay in a separate Petri dish. Treatments were applied only 30 s before the wasp entered the arena. The still-air nature of this assay and passive movement of the odour into the arena allowed odour saturation. This was intentional as the design was not aimed at testing the response to locating the odour source but behaviours when the parasitoid is in close proximity to the odour source. All replicates were completely independent with a new wasp, leaf and treatment applied. All equipment was cleaned between each replicate.

Table 1. Treatment sets used in motivation assay.

Treatments in the arena were presented from a chamber below, odours passively entered the arena but contact with the parasitoid was prevented by gauze. Nepetalactone was administered on filter paper and aphid-infested leaf describes a Brassica rapa leaf with ten Myzus persicae feeding. N represents the number of independent replicates completed for each treatment.

Statistical analysis

Electrophysiology

Antennal response to the aphid sex pheromone components was compared with the hexane control using a Mann–Whitney rank sum test conducted using Minitab software (Minitab 16, 2010).

Retention time

Data were analysed from both sets of experiments using one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's tests in Minitab (Minitab 16, 2010). Thirty-five replicates were completed for each treatment, with each individual parasitoid and leaf representing an independent replicate.

Motivation assay

Differences between treatments and the control group were identified using Kruskal–Wallis tests with Minitab statistical software (Minitab 16, 2010). Due to the repeated analysis, the Bonferroni correction was applied post hoc.

Results

Electrophysiology

EAG was conducted using (4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactone and (1R,4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactol comparing these treatments against hexane as a control (fig. 2). EAG responses of the parasitoid antenna indicated that the 0.05 mg ml−1 of both isomers failed to elicit a response within the antenna. However, a concentration of 1 mg ml−1 the nepetalactone did elicit an electrophysiological response significantly greater than the control (T 4 = 4.09, P = 0.026) which was also seen in the higher concentration of 10 mg ml−1 (T 4 = 12.6, P < 0.001). A response to nepetalactol was only seen in the highest concentration of 10 mg ml−1 (T 4 = 3.72, P = 0.02).

Fig. 2. Electroantennogram response of Aphidius colemani to nepetalactone and nepetalactol. All responses are standardized to the baseline hexane response shown as 100%. Five independent replicates were completed with each individual compared to the full complement of treatments. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001 when compared with the baseline.

On-leaf retention time

Retention of A. colemani in a specific area was tested using a range of treatments. This approach allowed the identification of any synergies or additive effects of the cues available. A comparison of the retention time between treatments also reveals the hierarchy of cues which may be utilized by the foraging wasp.

In the first set of trials five treatments were tested, each with a maximum running time of 15 min. The negative control (an empty arena) was found to have a significantly lower retention time than all treatments containing leaf material (42 ± 12 s) (F 4,159 = 15.55, P < 0.001). Between treatments containing a leaf there was no distinction between leaves alone, a previously infested leaf or nepetalactone presented alongside the leaf (fig. 3). The only treatment showing a greater retention time than the leaf alone was a previously aphid-infested leaf with nepetalactone.

Fig. 3. On-leaf retention time Set 1. Differences in the retention time of the parasitoid Aphidius colemani within the arena for Set 1 where treatments are Nothing = empty arena, Lf + n = nepetalactone alongside an intact leaf, Lf = leaf, Prev. Inf. Lf = previously infested leaf, Prev. Inf. Lf + n = previously infested leaf with nepetalactone. Those not sharing a letter are significantly different (F 4,159 = 15.55, P < 0.001). Thirty-five replicates were completed for each treatment.

In the second set of retention time assays, the maximum permitted time was increased to 20 min. A significant difference was observed between treatments (F 4,160 = 21.42, P < 0.001). As with Set 1, the negative control shows a significantly lower retention time than all other treatments (fig. 4). Retention time for a high concentration of nepetalactone (10 mg ml−1) alongside a leaf resulted in a retention time equal to that of the systemically damaged plant leaf alongside nepetalactone. A leaf containing aphid-related cues alongside nepetalactone led the wasp to spend more than three times as long in the area than on the leaf with a high concentration of nepetalactone or a systemically damaged leaf and a more than six times longer than the negative control. This value was comparable with the previously infested leaf alongside nepetalactone which, as with Set 1, led to the parasitoid being retained in the area for the longest period of time, significantly longer than all treatments except that containing aphid-related cues with nepetalactone.

Fig. 4. On-leaf retention time Set 2. Differences in the retention time of the parasitoid Aphidius colemani within the arena for Set 2 where treatments are Nothing = empty arena, Lf + n = 10 mg ml−1 nepetalactone alongside an intact leaf, Syst.+n = nepetalactone alongside systemically damaged leaf, ARC + n = aphid-related cues on leaf with nepetalactone, Prev. Inf. Lf + n = previously infested Brassica rapa leaf with nepetalactone. Those not sharing a letter are significantly different (F 4,160 = 21.42, P < 0.001). Thirty-five replicates were repeated for each treatment.

Motivation assay

The motivation assay was conducted to determine behavioural changes induced by exposure to nepetalactone and/or successful oviposition experience. It was found that learning experience with nepetalactone led to a decreased velocity (H = 7.32, P = 0.007) (table 2). Naïve parasitoids (those without oviposition experience) exposed to a high concentration of nepetalactone showed both a decrease in velocity (H = 11.78, P = 0.001) and an increase in the mean turn angle during movement (H = 12.78, P < 0.001). A trend was observed that meander varied with treatment in a fashion concurrent to that seen for increasing turn angle and decreased velocity. The greatest disparity is seen in a comparison of treatments with the high concentration of nepetalactone leading to meander of more than twice the value (−653.9 ± 113.75) of the stimulus-free control (−294.28 ± 72.15) however with the sample variance no significant difference was observed between these treatments (H = 5.19, P = 0.023, not significant following Bonferroni correction).

Table 2. Movement patterns of female Aphidius colemani recorded for 8 min using a series of treatments.

All parasitoids had no previous encounter with the host–plant complex or the sex pheromone nepetalactone prior to the bioassay. Pre-treatments were those administered directly before commencing the assay, while treatments in the arena were presented from below with the wasp unable to contact them. *P < 0.05 when compared with the control of no pre-treatment and no treatment in arena.

Discussion

Electroantennography confirmed that the aphid sex pheromone components were both sufficient to elicit a response in A. colemani. This was expected as an electrophysiological response has previously been shown to be detected by Aphidius rhopalosiphi, Aphidius ervi, Aphidius matricariae, Diaeretiella rapae, Praon volucre (Wadhams et al., Reference Wadhams, Birkett, Powell, Woodcock, Chadwick and Goode1999) and Aphidius gifuensis (Dong et al., Reference Dong, Zhang, Fang and Zhang2008). Its effect on parasitoid behaviour as a kairomone is established for a range of parasitoid species (for more details see Powell & Pickett, Reference Powell and Pickett2003). Although both nepetalactol and nepetalactone are sufficient in eliciting an electrophysiological response, it is nepetalactone to which a response was seen at a lower concentration, which may offer a greater opportunity to exploit parasitoid behaviour. In exploring the potential use for nepetalactone in the field it has to be recognized that it may not be a simple case of attracting natural enemies to an area. It has already been noted that differences in behavioural response to nepetalactone may exist depending on the extent to which the parasitoid is a specialist or a generalist (Glinwood et al., Reference Glinwood, Powell and Tripathi1998; Wadhams et al., Reference Wadhams, Birkett, Powell, Woodcock, Chadwick and Goode1999). Previous work has suggested that generalists may have a higher threshold for the pheromone, our work supports these findings but takes a wider view in recognizing that related-host and plant cues may be important for the insect to reach, what is not necessarily a threshold relating to one compound, but rather the reliability of the signal as a whole. The subtle behavioural changes seen in this series of experiments (increased retention, altered foraging patterns) may also explain why the application of nepetalactone in field can reduce aphid populations and alter spatial dynamics but is not consistently observed to attract wasps into the area (Powell et al., Reference Powell, A'Hara, Harling, Holl, Northing, Thomas and Walters2004).

Although nepetalactone alone was not found to increase the retention time of the parasitoids, a synergy was found to exist when presented with a previously infested leaf or when aphid-related cues are presented alongside the pheromone. Reliability–detectability theory (Vet & Dicke, Reference Vet and Dicke1992) recognizes the trade-off that exists in a natural environment with reliability and detectability. It is implicit in this theory that for the foraging individual a host-related signal of lesser detectability will provide greater reliability in the presence of that host. In a multitrophic context, a plant may benefit from advertising to parasitoids the presence of a pest feeding on it (Loon et al., Reference Loon, Boer and Dicke2000; Hoballah & Turlings, Reference Hoballah and Turlings2001). This broadcasting of the signal is in contrast to pest species where selection pressures exist for them to remain discreet and avoid predation or parasitism. As a consequence of this, and their greater biomass, plant odours are often more frequently available within the environment and may initially be utilized by the parasitoid to locate the habitat of the aphid, however, once the habitat is located parasitoids may require more host-specific cues to encourage search of that area. These changes in foraging behaviour are ultimately linked to the optimization of the foraging strategy employed by the parasitoid. By evaluating the cues associated with host presence, the parasitoid is able to reduce the energy expenditure involved in locating new sources of hosts. This study supports this hypothesis by demonstrating that more directly host-related cues are necessary to increase the retention of the natural enemy. Reception of the pheromones provides accurate temporal information regarding the presence of the host; conversely aphid-related cues provide exact spatial evidence pertaining to the location of hosts but may not provide evidence of when it was occupied. Whether or not this is the evolutionary rationale for such a response, the combination is sufficient to cause a substantial increase in the retention time of A. colemani. This may also be indicative of the natural enemy having a preference for the multimodal reception of host cues. While a difference is recognized between the control and any treatment involving leaves, this may not entirely be a consequence of volatiles as we recognize that the visual cue may also have affected the retention of the wasp. The on-leaf retention assay outlined may be considered a new and simple method of evaluating the retention time of insects with the caveat that visual stimuli also form part of this response.

It has previously been found with the hymenopteran parasitoid Pseudeucoila bochei that following a host encounter the female wasp will continue searching for 6–8 min before reverting to a general foraging pattern (Luck et al., Reference Luck, van Lenteren, Twine, Juenen and Unruh1979). This time is also similar to that used in tracking studies of the more closely related Aphidius uzbekistanicus (Micha & Wyss, Reference Micha and Wyss1996). The temporal scale for these changes is also an important consideration for our assays. It was following this timescale that we chose an 8 min recording for the motivation assays. Work by Hassell & Southwood (Reference Hassell and Southwood1978) has shown that invertebrate foraging behaviour at a local scale is characterized primarily by a decreased rate of movement (orthokinesis) but an increased rate of turning (klinokinesis). It was found that this foraging pattern would cease if the insect failed to find prey and it would revert to its previous pattern of searching (Hassell & May, Reference Hassell and May1974). In concurrence with these descriptions, it was found that A. colemani velocity was reduced following successful oviposition experience with nepetalactone. Although orthokinesis was reduced this did not correspond to any significant increase in the klinokinesis. It is speculated that with reduced error such a difference may have been observed as the values for meander (an indication of klinokinesis) are more than double that seen for the control.

The description of behavioural changes provided by our study and that of older work (Hassell & Southwood, Reference Hassell and Southwood1978) can also be framed in the context of modern foraging theory which frequently examines foraging in a larger spatial scale. Lévy flight foraging theory represents an optimal method of foraging and expects that selection pressure in a natural environment will have led to a selection for behaviours which reflect this (Viswanathan et al., Reference Viswanathan, Raposo and Da Luz2008). The Lévy flights described in the theory are ostensibly random but distinct periods of isotropic movements originally described by Mandelbrot (Reference Mandelbrot1983). These Lévy flights are thought to occur during periods in which the foraging organism has little reliable information regarding the desired resource (in our case the aphid host) and will allow them to efficiently explore a greater range of patches. These flights are interspersed by periods of foraging in a Brownian motion. In these terms, we hypothesize that the initiation of Brownian motion from a Lévy flight pattern will be achieved through identifying signals relating to a host, signals of greater reliability will elicit a more exaggerated or rapid transition to the Brownian motion. This transition is characterized by increasing meander and a decrease in the total distance moved in a given time.

It is speculated that this reintroduction of nepetalactone was sufficient to elicit a foraging response in the parasitoid which it associated with successful location of the host. This behaviour is also compliant with the optimal foraging theory and will potentially reduce the time spent by the parasitoid foraging for another host habitat. It is noted that the stimulus used was the aphid sex pheromone, to which it is shown in this paper A. colemani has an innate response. In this context it is feasible that the choice of odour was irrelevant and may simply be a demonstration of classic or ‘Pavlovian’ conditioning. In classical conditioning a stimulus elicits behavioural or physiological change as an unrelated stimulus is linked to a recognized stimulus, in this series of experiments this is shown not to be the case as nepetalactone is also active in altering A. colemani behavioural patterns when presented in isolation. Results from the analysis of retention and for movement patterns of A. colemani are not only concurrent with searching patterns previously described (Hassell & May, Reference Hassell and May1974; Hassell & Southwood, Reference Hassell and Southwood1978) but also support hypotheses proposed in reliability–detectability theory, optimal foraging theory and models of host location. From these experiments we provide evidence that a transition in foraging behaviour is characterized by reduced orthokinesis (to ensure the wasp does not leave the habitat) and increased klinokinesis (to increase the probability of encountering the host in the environment). The initiation of this change in foraging strategy is likely to be linked to the reliability of the stimuli that it receives. During the habitat location, a parasitoid will be reliant on cues of greater detectability but as it approaches the habitat it will also begin to receive more discrete stimuli which provide greater reliability of host presence. Once in this foraging mode the wasp will be retained for a greater time period, increasing its ‘give-up time’ (Croze, Reference Croze1970) to reduce energy expenditure on repeating the initial habitat location stage.

This work specifically focused on the behavioural response of the female A. colemani, primarily because of their importance in biological control. However, it is recognized that an interesting question is left unanswered of how the male parasitoids respond to this odour; although aphids are not a host for male A. colemani it may still be possible that they exploit the sex pheromone as a kairomone, to increase the likelihood of encountering females. It has been observed Psyttalia concolor Braconid males will readily exploit the host pheromones to raise their chances of encountering receptive females (Benelli et al., Reference Benelli, Carpita, Simoncini, Raspi and Canale2014). An interesting avenue for future work would be to examine the same behaviours in male Aphidiinae parasitoids where we might expect a similar foraging pattern to be observed.

In conclusion, for the first time we have identified the electrophysiological response of the aphid parasitoid A. colemani to the aphid sex pheromones (4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactone and (1R,4aS,7S,7aR)-nepetalactol. We also identified that a synergy exists with nepetalactone and other host cues causing an increased retention of the parasitoid A. colemani in an area. The behavioural patterns of A. colemani were also shown to be altered by learning or through the presence of nepetalactone, changes which are thought to reflect a transition of the parasitoid from general habitat search to specific host searching.

Acknowledgements

Koppert B.V. for provision of the parasitoids used in this study, Sarah E.J. Arnold for useful comments on the manuscript. Christine M. Woodcock assisted in conducting EAG experiments. G.M. Fernández-Grandon was supported by a BBSRC CASE Studentship with AgriSense BCS Ltd.

References

Alphen, J.V. & Vet, L.E.M. (1986) An evolutionary approach to host finding and selection. in Waage, J., Greathead, D. (Eds) Insect Parasitoids. 13th Symposium of the Royal Entomological Society of London 18–19 September 1985, at the Department of Physics Lecture Theatre, Imperial College, London, Academic Press, 23–61.Google Scholar
Ameixa, O.M.C.C. & Kindlmann, P. (2012) Effect of synthetic and plant-extracted aphid pheromones on the behaviour of Aphidius colemani . Journal of Applied Entomology 136, 292301.Google Scholar
Benelli, G., Carpita, A., Simoncini, S., Raspi, A. & Canale, A. (2014) For sex and more: attraction of the tephritid parasitoid Psyttalia concolor (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) to male sex pheromone of the olive fruit fly, Bactrocera oleae . Journal of Pest Science 87, 449457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackman, R.L. (1971) Variation in the photoperiodic response within natural populations of Myzus persicae (Sulz.). Bulletin of Entomological Research 60, 533546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croze, H. (1970) Searching image in carrion crows: hunting strategy in a predator and some anti-predator devices in camouflaged prey. Parey Berlin 5, 185.Google Scholar
Dong, W.X., Zhang, F., Fang, Y.L. & Zhang, Z.N. (2008) Electroantennogram responses of aphid parasitoid Aphidius gifuensis to aphid pheromones and host–plant volatiles. Chinese Journal of Ecology 27, 591595.Google Scholar
Du, Y.J., Poppy, G.M. & Powell, W. (1996) Relative importance of semiochemicals from first and second trophic levels in host foraging behavior of Aphidius ervi . Journal of Chemical Ecology 22, 15911605.Google Scholar
Fernández-Grandon, G.M., Woodcock, C.M. & Poppy, G.M. (2013) Do asexual morphs of the peach–potato aphid, Myzus persicae, utilise the aphid sex pheromone? Behavioural and electrophysiological responses of M. persicae virginoparae to (4aS, 7S, 7aR)-nepetalactone and its effect on aphid performance. Bulletin of Entomological Research 103, 466472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glinwood, R.T., Powell, W. & Tripathi, C.P.M. (1998) Increased parasitization of aphids on trap plants alongside vials releasing synthetic aphid sex pheromone and effective range of the pheromone. Biocontrol Science and Technology 8, 607614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glinwood, R.T., Du, Y.J. & Powell, W. (1999) Responses to aphid sex pheromones by the pea aphid parasitoids Aphidius ervi and Aphidius eadyi . Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 92, 227232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guerrieri, E., Pennacchio, F. & Tremblay, E. (1997) Effect of adult experience on in-flight orientation to plant and plant–host complex volatiles in Aphidius ervi Haliday (Hymenoptera, Braconidae). Biological Control 10, 159165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardie, J. & Minks, A.K. (1999) Pheromones of non-Lepidopteran Insects Associated with Agricultural Plants. Wallingford, CAB International.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardie, J., Holyoak, M., Nicholas, J., Nottingham, S.F., Pickett, J.A., Wadhams, L.J. & Woodcock, C.M. (1990) Aphid sex pheromone components: age-dependent release by females and species-specific male response. Chemoecology 1, 6368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardie, J., Nottingham, S.F., Powell, W. & Wadhams, L.J. (1991) Synthetic aphid sex pheromone lures female parasitoids. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 61, 9799.Google Scholar
Hassell, M.P. & May, R.M. (1974) Aggregation of predators and insect parasites and its effect on stability. The Journal of Animal Ecology 43, 567594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassell, M. & Southwood, T.R.E. (1978) Foraging strategies of insects. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 9, 7598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoballah, M.E.F. & Turlings, T.C.J. (2001) Experimental evidence that plants under caterpillar attack may benefit from attracting parasitoids. Evolutionary Ecology Research 3, 553565.Google Scholar
Leroy, P.D., Schillings, T., Farmakidis, J., Heuskin, S., Lognay, G., Verheggen, F.J., Brostaux, Y., Haubruge, E. & Francis, F. (2012) Testing semiochemicals from aphid, plant and conspecific: attraction of Harmonia axyridis . Insect Science 19, 372382.Google Scholar
Loon, J.J.V., Boer, J.G. & Dicke, M. (2000) Parasitoid-plant mutualism: parasitoid attack of herbivore increases plant reproduction. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 97, 219227.Google Scholar
Luck, R.F., van Lenteren, J.C., Twine, P.H., Juenen, L. & Unruh, T. (1979) Prey or host searching behavior that leads to a sigmoid functional response in invertebrate predators and parasitoids. Researches on Population Ecology 20, 257264.Google Scholar
Maddrell, S.H.P. (1969) Secretion by the Malpighian tubules of Rhodnius. The movements of ions and water. Journal of Experimental Biology 51, 7197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandelbrot, B.B. (1983) The Fractal Geometry of Nature. Henry Holt and Company, New York City, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, D. (1975) Responses of male aphids to the female sex pheromone in Megoura viciae Buckton. Journal of Entomology Series A, General Entomology 50, 4364.Google Scholar
Micha, S.G. & Wyss, U. (1996) Aphid alarm pheromone (E)-β-farnesene: a host finding kairomone for the aphid primary parasitoid Aphidius uzbekistanicus (Hymenoptera: Aphidiinae). Chemoecology 7, 132139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, K.C., Zhu, J., Harris, J., Ochieng, S.A. & Baker, T.C. (2001) Electroantennogram responses of a parasitic wasp, Microplitis croceipes, to host-related volatile and anthropogenic compounds. Physiological Entomology 26, 6977.Google Scholar
Pettersson, J. (1970) An aphid sex attractant. Insect Systematics and Evolution 1, 6373.Google Scholar
Poppy, G.M., Powell, W. & Pennacchio, F. (1997) Aphid parasitoid responses to semiochemicals—genetic, conditioned or learnt? Entomophaga 42, 193199.Google Scholar
Powell, W. & Pickett, J.A. (2003) Manipulation of parasitoids for aphid pest management: progress and prospects. Pest Management Science 59, 149155.Google Scholar
Powell, W., A'Hara, S., Harling, R., Holl, J., Northing, P., Thomas, C.F.G. & Walters, K.F.A. (2004) Managing biodiversity in field margins to enhance integrated pest control in arable crops (3-D farming project). HGCA Project Report 356, 8096.Google Scholar
Storeck, A., Poppy, G.M., Emden, H.V. & Powell, W. (2000) The role of plant chemical cues in determining host preference in the generalist aphid parasitoid Aphidius colemani . Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 97, 4146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turlings, T.C., Wäckers, F.L., Vet, L.E., Lewis, W.J. & Tumlinson, J.H. (1993) Learning of host-finding cues by hymenopterous parasitoids. pp. 5178 in Papaj, D.R. & Lewis, A.C. (Eds) Insect Learning. USA, Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vet, L.E. & Dicke, M. (1992) Ecology of infochemical use by natural enemies in a tritrophic context. Annual Review of Entomology 37, 141172.Google Scholar
Vinson, S.B. (1976) Host selection by insect parasitoids. Annual Review of Entomology 21, 109133.Google Scholar
Viswanathan, G.M., Raposo, E.P. & Da Luz, M.G.E. (2008) Lévy flights and superdiffusion in the context of biological encounters and random searches. Physics of Life Reviews 5, 133150.Google Scholar
Wadhams, L.J., Angst, M.E. & Blight, M.M. (1982) Responses of the olfactory receptors of Scolytus scolytus (F.)(Coleoptera: Scolytidae) to the stereoisomers of 4-methyl-3-heptanol. Journal of Chemical Ecology 8, 477492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadhams, L.J., Birkett, M.A., Powell, W. & Woodcock, C.M. (1999) Aphids, predators, and parasitoids. pp. 6067 in Chadwick, D. & Goode, J. (Eds) Insect–Plant Interactions and Induced Plant Defences. Novartis Foundation Symposium 223, Chichester, Wiley.Google Scholar
Wäschke, N., Hardge, K., Hancock, C., Hilker, M., Obermaier, E. & Meiners, T. (2014) Habitats as complex odour environments: how does plant diversity affect herbivore and parasitoid orientation? PLoS ONE 9, e85152.Google Scholar
Wellings, P.W. (1991) Host location and oviposition on animals. pp. 75107 in Bailey, W.J. & Ridsdill-Smith, J. (Eds) Reproductive Behavior of Insects: Individuals and Populations. London, Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Zhang, Q.H., Sheng, M., Chen, G., Aldrich, J.R. & Chauhan, K.R. (2006) Iridodial: a powerful attractant for the green lacewing, Chrysopa septempunctata (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae). Naturwissenschaften 93, 461465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Motivation assay arena. A 9 cm diameter Petri dish forms the upper area of the arena which is rested on a 3 cm diameter Petri dish (1). Odour moves passively into the arena through the 2 × 2 cm gauze-covered opening (2). Aphidius colemani are introduced via the 0.5 cm circular opening which accommodates a small insect storage vial (3). The smaller Petri dish is used (4) to contain the treatment while preventing the parasitoid from contacting the source. A glass covering is placed over the apparatus to prevent escape of the insects while allowing recording.

Figure 1

Table 1. Treatment sets used in motivation assay.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Electroantennogram response of Aphidius colemani to nepetalactone and nepetalactol. All responses are standardized to the baseline hexane response shown as 100%. Five independent replicates were completed with each individual compared to the full complement of treatments. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001 when compared with the baseline.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. On-leaf retention time Set 1. Differences in the retention time of the parasitoid Aphidius colemani within the arena for Set 1 where treatments are Nothing = empty arena, Lf + n = nepetalactone alongside an intact leaf, Lf = leaf, Prev. Inf. Lf = previously infested leaf, Prev. Inf. Lf + n = previously infested leaf with nepetalactone. Those not sharing a letter are significantly different (F4,159 = 15.55, P < 0.001). Thirty-five replicates were completed for each treatment.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. On-leaf retention time Set 2. Differences in the retention time of the parasitoid Aphidius colemani within the arena for Set 2 where treatments are Nothing = empty arena, Lf + n = 10 mg ml−1 nepetalactone alongside an intact leaf, Syst.+n = nepetalactone alongside systemically damaged leaf, ARC + n = aphid-related cues on leaf with nepetalactone, Prev. Inf. Lf + n = previously infested Brassica rapa leaf with nepetalactone. Those not sharing a letter are significantly different (F4,160 = 21.42, P < 0.001). Thirty-five replicates were repeated for each treatment.

Figure 5

Table 2. Movement patterns of female Aphidius colemani recorded for 8 min using a series of treatments.