Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T18:49:34.191Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Health and Voting in Young Adulthood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2017

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Do changes in health lead to changes in the probability of voting? Using two longitudinal datasets, this article looks at the impact of three measures of health – physical health, mental health and overall well-being – on voting trajectories in young adulthood. The results show that self-rated health is associated with a lower probability of voting in one’s first election, depression is related to a decline in turnout over time and physical limitations are unrelated to voting. Some familial resources from childhood are also found to condition when the health–participation effect manifests.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2017 

Why do some individuals become lifelong voters, while others do not? A typical answer focuses on resources and the degree to which young adults have access to the time, money and civic skills for participation.Footnote 1 Recent research on voting behavior points to a fourth resource: health. If participation requires that individuals possess some physical capacity and mental well-being, then population health inequities may lead to a health–participation gap. Indeed, the voting gap across health status is consistently estimated to be upwards of 10 percentage points,Footnote 2 rivaling other noteworthy disparities in electoral participation. This association is important not just because it pinpoints a source of political inequality, but because health inequalities in turnout may also have significant political and electoral consequencesFootnote 3 due to health-based disparities in partisanshipFootnote 4 and public opinion.Footnote 5

While research on the health–participation gap is in its infancy, research on the adult consequences of childhood and adolescent health has accumulated in the fields of sociology psychology, public health and economics. It is now well established that poor health early in life is deleterious to success on an array of adult outcomes such as educational attainment, socioeconomic status and health.Footnote 6 One recent article aptly described this effect as the ‘long shadow cast by childhood physical and mental problems on adult life’.Footnote 7 We expand this work into the political realm by asking whether the ‘long shadow’ extends to adult political life.

Using the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1997 (NLSY), we find that poor adolescent health generally retards the upward trajectory of voting, but that certain childhood resources can reverse this effect in complex and unexpected ways. Specifically, we show that self-rated health status (SRHS) is associated with an individual’s initial probability of voting but not the voting trajectory, depression has little influence on the initial probability of voting but is associated with changes in voting over time, and physical limitations are unrelated to voting in young adulthood. We further show that the parents’ educational level mitigates the effect of self-rated health on voting for some young adults and exacerbates it for others, while the parents’ economic standing does not change the nature of the health–turnout linkage. Importantly, we demonstrate the robustness of some claims and the precariousness of others by replicating our findings using the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID).

This study contributes to our understanding of political behavior in several ways. First, it affirms and extends the idea developed in recent scholarship that health is important to political participation. Participation in politics requires a functioning and healthy (or at least not unhealthy) body. While previous research on the participatory consequences of health has generally focused on only one aspect of health,Footnote 8 we show that health influences turnout differently depending on the ailment. Health is multidimensional, and our understanding about how health is related to political behavior must take this into account.

Secondly, our findings – that certain health conditions may interrupt the traditional voting trajectory of young adults – challenges the dominant assumption in the turnout literature that the likelihood of voting increases in the first few elections but flattens soon after.Footnote 9 We show that chronic health conditions not only affect the initial likelihood of voting, but may also rob individuals of the typical increase in voting during their early 20s. It remains to be seen whether these same individuals ‘catch up’ to their healthy counterparts later in life. This finding, along with more recent research showing that the development of a voting habit is bumpier than previously suggested,Footnote 10 requires scholars to question commonly held views about how young adults transition to being active participants.

Thirdly, we show that the resources for participation – health, education and income – must be considered in tandem. Studies of voting behavior typically model turnout as a function of an individual’s resources on the basis that more resources unequivocally lead to a higher propensity to participate. We find, however, that some sets of resources can activate participation while other sets can debilitate it. For example, the prior experience of poor health can increase participation among now-healthy individuals who come from educated families. This effect – in which prior poor health is associated with higher turnout – suggests that poor–rich combinations of resources may sometimes motivate participation by placing past grievances (that is, prior poor health) in a resource-rich environment (that is, now-excellent health, educated family). In contrast, we show that coming from a well-educated family can reduce participation in the face of declining health. Although we are not able to replicate these findings, we believe that they call for new theorizing on how sets of resources, rather than the individual resources themselves, may motivate or depress participation.

Finally, our research is notable in that it employs underutilized longitudinal datasets including the NLSY and the PSID to understand political participation. The majority of work on the health–participation gap uses cross-sectional surveys with adult samples. While cross-sectional data are useful for certain analytic purposes, they do little to distinguish between factors that influence an individual’s long-term propensity to vote from those that have temporary, short-term effects that are associated with recent personal or political conditions.Footnote 11 The inability to ‘sort out’ causal ordering and the potential endogeneity issues associated with cross-sectional analyses is particularly problematic for understanding the relationship between health and turnout, because health may have both developmental and contemporaneous components.Footnote 12 By employing longitudinal data and a developmental design we can better pinpoint both the short- and long-term effects of poor health across childhood, adolescence and young adulthood.

A DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACH TO VOTER TURNOUT

Political scientists have long viewed voting as a habit that develops over the life course,Footnote 13 with the bulk of this development occurring in young adulthood. The developmental trajectory has three components including a starting level, which is the probability that citizens vote in their first eligible election, growth, which is the period in which individuals gain resources and experiences that are relevant to participation, and inertia, which is ‘the propensity for citizens to settle into habits of voting or nonvoting’.Footnote 14 As Plutzer explains, young citizens must decide whether to vote in their first eligible election; this decision is determined in large part from parental, demographic and personal factors in adolescence.Footnote 15 First-time voters are likely to become habitual voters quickly after the first election, while non-voters are likely to remain non-voters in subsequent elections. Of the non-voters who become habitual voters, the factors that determined their initial probability of voting become less important.

Plutzer finds that the resources attained from parents have a strong influence on whether an individual voters in her or her first election, but are largely unrelated to growth thereafter.Footnote 16 This makes sense considering how costly it is to vote for the first time. Young citizens have the most barriers to voting: they are unfamiliar with the registration process, they may not know the location of their polling place or have a good understanding of key issues, they have few friends who vote and they are least likely to be targeted by political groups.Footnote 17 Surprisingly, research shows that the adoption of adult roles, such as leaving home, attending college or buying a house, has little bearing on turnout in ‘the first seven years of the political life cycle’.Footnote 18 Turnout in young adulthood thus seems to be influenced more by the resources and socialization experiences of childhood and adolescence than by the adult experiences that would be expected to draw individuals into politics.

Inertial states are different than starting values or growth. Because the costs of voting are constant and resources generally increase with age, many nonvoters eventually become habitual voters. The difference across individuals is the speed at which they do so. For instance, Plutzer shows that college-educated individuals make the transition to habitual voters quicker than those without a college degree.Footnote 19 And, while major life events – such as job loss or divorce – are likely to cause temporary disruptions in turnout, these effects are more detrimental for habitual voters than for non-voters.

Using a developmental approach allows scholars to assess the determinants of turnout more precisely by (1) looking at how factors originating in adolescence or young adulthood impact starting levels compared to growth, (2) modeling variations in how quickly individuals make the transition from non-voters to habitual voters, and (3) allowing major life events to temporarily disrupt the inertia of habitual voters. The developmental model, combined with a theoretical understanding of how health imposes additional costs to voting, is also well suited to generate hypotheses for how different facets of health are related to voter turnout.

THE ROLE OF HEALTH IN THE DEVELOPMENTAL MODEL OF VOTER TURNOUT

We discuss the effect of three dimensions of health – physical health, mental health and overall well-being – over an individual’s life and in terms of the resources and motivation required to participate, thereby placing the role of health firmly in the theoretical frames offered by the developmental and resource models of participation. There are two reasons for focusing on these three dimensions of health. First, research on the health–participation gap has consistently shown the importance of these dimensions of health, but has often done so in an isolated fashion. Because one of our goals is to reconcile past research and provide a more comprehensive understanding of the health–participation link, it makes the most sense to build on previous research. Secondly, these dimensions of health are the most commonly available in survey data.

Physical Health and Turnout

We define physical health as ‘the general working of one’s body’Footnote 20 and measure it as the presence (or absence) of a chronic condition that limits daily activities. Conditions that limit physical mobility are particularly detrimental to political participation,Footnote 21 since they impose additional physical costs to voting. Absentee ballotsFootnote 22 and improved street conditionsFootnote 23 help overcome some of these physical limitations for senior citizen participation, but individuals are generally less likely to vote if they find it more physically difficult to do so.Footnote 24 Prior research on limitations focuses on disabilities such as blindness, hearing impairment and immobility, but overlooks those that emerge from other physical health conditions earlier in life such as cancer, diabetes, asthma and high blood pressure. Disability and chronic conditions differ in several important respects; most notably, disability does not imply poor physical health.Footnote 25 An individual with a hearing impairment or who is in a wheelchair may consider themselves to be in perfect health otherwise, or may be diagnosed as healthy along a range of health dimensions. However, the logic of physical limitations should apply equally to disabilities and chronic conditions when looking at their effects on political participation.

Living with chronic conditions in a physical world that privileges able-bodied individuals is part of a managed process: individuals establish routines, redesign their environment and living conditions to be more accommodating, and establish networks of social support to manage the condition and its subsequent limitations. In this sense, we expect that physical limitations primarily affect the inertia underpinning voting as opposed to the starting levels or growth. The onset of a chronic condition will be disruptive until the condition and its limitations are sufficiently managed and accommodated in everyday life. That is, among individuals who have already developed the habit of voting, a negative change in physical health will likely cause a temporary disruption in their voting trajectory, much like a job loss or divorce causes temporary disruptions.

Like other disruptive factors, such as widowhoodFootnote 26 or withdrawal from the labor market,Footnote 27 we expect that the negative effect of physical limitations on turnout will be considerably less evident among young citizens than their older counterparts. We expect this for three reasons. First, younger citizens are much less likely to have formed voting habits that can be disrupted by the onset of a limiting chronic condition. Secondly, the onset of the chronic conditions considered here, such as diabetes, high blood pressure and cancer, often occur later in life, making them less prevalent among younger citizens. In our dataset, for instance, nearly 60 per cent of young adults cited asthma as their chronic condition. While asthma is certainly an affliction that limits physical activity, it may not be as significant as those caused by the types of conditions that are more typical in late adulthood. Thirdly, research has identified generational differences in how individuals understand their disability; young citizens, unlike their older counterparts, are much more likely to identify with and rally around their disability, which in turn leads to smaller differences in political participation between those with and without a disability.Footnote 28 This type of generational change also seems possible with chronic conditions like cancer and diabetes, which have recently been subject to organizational efforts to raise awareness, reduce stigma around the disease and empower those affected.Footnote 29 Because we are looking at samples of young adults who came of age in the late 1990s and early 2000s, it seems less likely that a large dampening effect would be observed.Footnote 30

Mental Health and Turnout

For mental health, we focus on depression because it is associated with political participation, it is one of the leading burdens of disease, and relevant to the larger population due to its high prevalence. Depression is a mood disorder characterized by a loss of energy, low self-esteem, concentration problems, changes in eating and sleeping patterns, and thoughts of suicide.Footnote 31 We measure depression in terms of symptomology. Using a gradient of symptoms is better because it captures individuals along a depressed mood spectrum, including respondents with clinically significant but non-major symptoms.Footnote 32 As Ojeda explains, people with a depressed mood are less likely to vote because of a loss in motivation, decreased cognitive abilities and reduced somatic capacity, all of which are required for participation.Footnote 33

Depression is much more likely than physical limitations to disrupt all aspects of voting, because it imposes larger costs. Whereas voters who are physically limited may still have the motivation and cognitive resources to turn out, depression attacks not only the physical capacity to vote, but also the psychological factors that impact the decision to vote. Depression impairs the cognitive abilities and executive functioning that underpin the civic skills required for participation. If voting requires ‘mind, body and heart’, voters with physical limitations lack the body while voters experiencing a bout of depression are affected in all three domains.

There are also more opportunities for citizens with physical limitations to be integrated into society compared to those with mental health issues. The disability rights movement is nearly a half-century old, and efforts like the Voting Accessibility for the Elderly and Handicapped Act of 1984 and the Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990 have helped make the political process more open to those with physical disabilities.Footnote 34 However, disability was only redefined to include depression in 2008.Footnote 35 While research on the economic and social consequences of depression has flourished,Footnote 36 there is little electoral policy to make voting easier for people with mental disorders; in some cases, policy excludes such individuals.Footnote 37

Overall Well-Being and Turnout

We capture overall well-being using a SRHS question asking respondents ‘In general, how is your health?’ with five allowable responses of excellent, very good, good, fair and poor. Because SRHS is empirically related to objective measures of health,Footnote 38 scholars argue that it is ideally suited to test theories of political behavior.Footnote 39 Some even claim that ‘an individual’s health status cannot be assessed without’ SRHS and that this single item captures ‘an irreplaceable dimension of health status’.Footnote 40

While poor health is associated with lower levels of turnout, we expect SRHS to have a substantial effect on starting levels and a lesser effect on growth and inertia. As stated above, factors that originate in adolescence and enable citizens to overcome the high costs of first-time voting are the most likely to differentiate individuals in their starting levels of turnout. Unlike other ailments that develop as people age or fluctuate quite substantially over the life course, SRHS originates in early childhood,Footnote 41 is transmitted across generationsFootnote 42 and is highly correlated with socioeconomic status,Footnote 43 much like political voice and power.Footnote 44 Consequently, we expect that SRHS will largely differentiate individuals in their initial starting levels of turnout but have little effect on voting rates over time, much like parental education.Footnote 45

FAMILY RESOURCES AND THE HEALTH TURNOUT LINKAGE

The transition to adulthood, including the development of a voting habit and an interest in politics more broadly, is smoothed by the presence of resources; adolescents from well-to-do families are better equipped to confront the challenges that come with independent living and thinking, while their less well-off counterparts face a bumpier path.Footnote 46 The role of health on political participation must be considered in this context. Thus while poor health negatively affects turnout, it is also worth considering how family resources can mitigate or exacerbate the health–participation gap. We consider two resources in particular – parental education and family income. Parental education is an important predictor of political participation,Footnote 47 and income is an important predictor of health status.Footnote 48

Parental education is a well-known and well-studied predictor of turnout. Children from families with educated parents are, on average, according to the results of a recent meta-analysis of voter turnout, more likely to participate later in life than those from less well-educated families.Footnote 49 Educated parents are likely to have established interest in politics that they can transmit to their children through direct teaching, inculcation and role modeling. In this sense, we expect parental education to mitigate the effects of health in much the same way that we expect income to. However, we also believe that there is a second pathway through which parental education can have mitigating effects: by helping adolescents and young adults learn how to manage their health. Education has consistently been shown to improve health behaviors.Footnote 50 In this vein, we expect that educated parents will better understand insurance plans and know how to navigate the healthcare system. These resources and knowledge can be provided to offspring, helping to reduce the burden of poor health. We therefore expect that the effect of health on turnout will be lower among children who grow up in better-educated families both because parental education prepares children for the political process and assists them in dealing with health problems.

Perhaps one of the best-established findings in the study of political behavior is that the rich are more likely to participate than the poor.Footnote 51 Income also affects health outcomes, as sociologists have long noted.Footnote 52 These findings together would point to a spurious health–turnout linkage were it not for evidence showing a persistent health effect even after controlling for economic status.Footnote 53 Still, the nature of the relationship raises interesting questions about how income might condition the effect of health. The rich are more likely to participate than the poor because they are more likely to be better educated (thereby acquiring the requisite civic skills for participation), are more likely to develop friendship networks largely composed of voters (raising the possibility that they will be recruited into politics) and are more likely to be contacted by an election campaign seeking donations and votes (thereby raising the probability that they will be mobilized to participate).Footnote 54 It therefore seems likely that the skills, recruitment and mobilization that come with rising income levels will offset the negative effects of health. That is, income will have a compensatory effect where good health is lacking, thus shrinking the degree to which poor health limits participation.

These two hypotheses – both of which posit that the health–participation gap will be biggest where family resources are lowest – are part of a larger narrative about cumulative disadvantage and political participation. Research elsewhere shows that resources beget more resources, and can therefore widen the participation gap between the haves and the have-nots.Footnote 55 While we expect health to contribute to cumulative disadvantage, we do not believe that this effect will manifest the same across all dimensions of health. We expect that the effect of physical limitations will be more disruptive and prevalent later in life. Thus, physical health will be untainted by the presence or absence of other resources in adolescence. Depression and SRHS, however, should exhibit a cumulative disadvantaging effect. We expect to see the greatest cumulative disadvantage in initial levels of participation when family resources are most proximate to turnout.

To summarize, we anticipate that poor health will generally dampen levels of turnout; however, how poor health impacts turnout depends on the indicator under consideration and on what other family resources are present. We hypothesize that starting levels of turnout are affected by SRHS and depression but less so by physical limitations, and that these effects on initial turnout are exacerbated by low levels of parental education and family income. We also expect depression to affect the growth period, hindering individuals’ movement from non-voter to voter status; during this period, we expect to see a small level of cumulative disadvantage that carries over from the low levels of family resources available during adolescence. Finally, we hypothesize that physical limitations will a temporarily disrupt voting among habitual voters.

DATA AND METHODS

We use the 1997 National Longitudinal Study of Youth (NLSY97) to study the effect of physical limitations, depression and SRHS on an individual’s developmental trajectory of voter turnout. Collected by the Bureau of Labor Statistics, the NLSY97 is a longitudinal dataset that surveyed a nationally representative sample of approximately 9,000 youths annually (1997 to present) when they were 12 to 16 years old. The NLSY97 contains detailed information on a wide range of topics including relationships with parents, marital and fertility histories, employment activity, education, expectations, deviant behavior and health. In addition, parents were surveyed about the youths’ family background and history in 1997; in some instances, we use parental questions to control for adolescent factors. The NLSY97, in coordination with the American National Election Studies, asks about voter turnout in the 2004 and 2008 presidential elections and the 2006 and 2010 midterm elections.

Figure 1 shows the percentage of citizens who reported voting during each election year. As expected, respondents report higher levels of turnout in presidential years compared to midterm years. There is also descriptive evidence that levels of voter turnout increased over time; 57 per cent reported voting in the 2004 election compared to 63 per cent in 2008. Similarly, only 35 per cent reported voting in the 2006 midterm election compared to 43 per cent in 2010. This indicates that even though turnout drops significantly in midterm years, there is still a general pattern of growth over the four election cycles.

Fig. 1 Reported turnout in each election, NLSY97

Fundamentally, we are interested in looking at how the propensity to turn out changes over time, and exploring whether health is related to individual differences in these changes. Empirically, we use multi-level modeling as it allows us to partition the variance in voter turnout into two parts: within-individual change and inter-individual change. The first stage of the multilevel model, known as level-1, looks specifically at within-individual change and aims to describe each person’s pattern of change. The second stage of the multilevel model, known as level-2, looks at inter-individual differences in change and seeks to determine the relationship between predictors and the shape of each person’s individual trajectory. Although we model both types of variation, we are primarily interested in the latter.Footnote 56 About 40 per cent of the variance in voter turnout is at level-1, while 60 per cent is at level-2.

An important methodological feature of panel analyses is a sensible metric for time. We opt to use election year as the metric of time, with the 2004 election marking the first recorded experience in presidential electoral politics; thus we code the 2004 election as election 0, with 2006, 2008 and 2010 coded as 1, 2 and 3, respectively. Notably, because about 38 per cent of the sample was old enough to vote in the 2000 election and almost all respondents were eligible to vote in the 2002 midterm election, this temporal choice will necessarily add noise the analysis and make it more difficult to distinguish health effects on the initial probability of voting from changes in the trajectory of voting. Unfortunately, NLSY97 does not ask about voter turnout in the 2000 and 2002 elections. We therefore also estimate the basic models with only those respondents for whom 2004 was the first presidential election and find that the inferences are nearly identical to what is reported below.

More specifically, the probability of an individual, i, voting in election t, is estimated as:

$$\matrix{ {{\bf{Level}} \,{\bf{1}} } \hfill & {Pr\left( {{\rm Y}_{{{\rm ti}}} \,{\equals}\,1} \right){\equals}\,{\rm B}_{{0{\rm i}}} {\plus}{\rm B}_{{1{\rm i}}} \left( {{\rm Election}_{{{\rm ti}}} } \right){\plus}{\rm B}_{{2{\rm i}}} \left( {{\rm Midterm}_{{{\rm ti}}} } \right){\plus}{\rm B}_{{3{\rm i}}} \left( {{\rm Time}{\minus}{\rm Varying \,\,X}_{{{\rm ti}}} } \right){\plus}{\rm e}_{{{\rm ti}}} } \hfill \cr {{\bf{Level}} \,{\bf{2}} } \hfill & {{\rm B}_{{0{\rm i}}} \,{\equals}\,{\rm \gamma }_{{00}} {\plus}{\rm \gamma }_{{01}} \left( {{\rm Z}_{{\rm i}} } \right){\plus}{\rm U}_{{0{\rm i}}} } \hfill \cr {} \hfill & {{\rm B}_{{1{\rm i}}} \,{\equals}\,{\rm \gamma }_{{10}} {\plus}{\rm U}_{{1{\rm i}}} } \hfill \cr {} \hfill & {{\rm B}_{{2{\rm i}}} \,{\equals}\,{\rm \gamma }_{{20}} } \hfill \cr {} \hfill & {{\rm B}_{{3{\rm i}}} \,{\equals}\,{\rm \gamma }_{{30}} } \hfill \cr } $$

Variation among individuals is modeled in two ways. First, the intercept (B0i) captures differences in starting levels of turnout when citizens are, on average, 22 years old. The intercept, or the probability of voting in the 2004 election, is predicted by variable Z (the effect of variable Z is captured by γ01) and U0i, which is the unexplained variance in person i’s starting point. Variation is also captured by the slope (B1i), which estimates the developmental trajectory of turnout that occurs over time, in this case over four consecutive elections, and U1i, which is the unexplained variance in person i’s trajectory. Since turnout is typically lower in midterm elections, we also include a dummy variable to differentiate between the two types of elections, although we assume that this negative effect does not vary across respondents (for example, the midterm elections variable does not have a random effect).

Developmental theory suggests that variables determined in adolescence will have a greater impact on the initial starting point than on subsequent changes in turnout over time. Empirically, Plutzer finds that this is largely the case.Footnote 57 As a result, all of the control variables measured prior to 2004 are entered into the random intercept part of the model (for example, as variable Z in the above equation) and, therefore, can only influence the probability of initially turning out in the 2004 election. That is, these variables only contribute to understanding differences in the intercept across individuals and tell us little about changes over time.Footnote 58

In cases where the independent variable is time varying (for example, marital status), we include both the 2004 value (for example, as variable Z in the above equation) as well as the change in that variable, shown as B3i in the equation above. Including both the initial starting value and the time-varying variable allows us to model systemic variation in both the initial probability of turnout and changes in turnout.Footnote 59 For example, we are able to determine whether married citizens had higher levels of turnout in 2004 compared to unmarried citizens (for example, if marital status differentiates starting levels or the intercept) as well as whether getting married increased turnout across the same time period (for example, affected the voting trajectory or slope over time). Model specification in this manner allows us to explore not only how aspects of health impact turnout in the 2004 election, but also subsequent growth by accessing how changing levels of health impact changes in the probability of turning out.Footnote 60

Measuring Physical Health, Mental Health and Overall Well-Being

In 2002 and 2008, respondents listed up to six chronic conditions and were subsequently asked whether any of these conditions limited their daily activities. This variable is recoded into a binary indicator where a 1 indicates that the respondent is limited to any extent by at least one of these conditions (mean=0.20). This variable measures physical limitations. Respondents are also asked a series of questions in all four election years about their mental health, and more specifically, their level of depression. We combine these questions into a standardized scale (mean=−0.01, SD=0.59), with a high degree of reliability, as indicated by Cronbach’s alpha (r=0.79).Footnote 61 Finally, we measure overall health using a self-rated health question that ranges from poor (3) to excellent/very good (0) with higher values indicating poorer health (mean=0.47, SD=0.68). Thus for all measures of health, higher values correspond to poorer health levels.Footnote 62 Question wording for the health measures is reported in Appendix Table A.1, and the corresponding descriptive statistics are reported in Appendix Table A.2.

Control Variables

It is important to control for other variables that influence the propensity to vote over time. Demographic characteristics include race (measured as two indicator variables for black and other, with white as the reference category), gender (female=1), and age (centered at 22).Footnote 63 One important parental influence on young adult turnout is education.Footnote 64 Consequently, we include a composite index of parental education by combining the highest degree earned by the respondent’s mother and father; this index has a high degree of reliability, as indicated by Cronbach’s alpha (r=0.93). We also include a measure of parent-reported family income in 1996, the year in which parents were asked about the family’s economic status.

Plutzer finds that high school achievement matters mostly indirectly through knowledge and political engagement; that is, students who are high achievers in high school have a higher propensity to vote in their first election primarily because they are more engaged in politics.Footnote 65 We control for adolescent political engagement indirectly by including a measure of high school GPA in 1999, which captures high school achievement. Adult education, and particularly college education, also contributes to a head start and a more rapid increase in voter turnout. We include a time-varying measure of the highest degree earned by the respondent (centered at twelve years) to account for the additional resources respondents gather through higher education.

Other variables tap into important life events that may temporarily disrupt political activity, including marital status (measured by two dummy variables indicating whether the respondent was married or divorced/separated; never married/widowed is the omitted category), parenthood (measured as the number of children living in the household) and residential migration (measured by a dummy variable indicating whether the respondent has moved since the prior interview). Finally, we capture political engagement directly by including a time-varying measure of the extent to which respondents follow what is going on in government, which ranges from 0 (hardly at all) to 3 (most of the time).

Unless otherwise noted, all predictors that are numeric scales (for example, depression or parental education) are mean centered, while variables with arbitrary scales (for example, SRHS) and dummy variables remain in their natural metric (see Appendix Table A.2 for descriptive information on all of the variables). This allows an intuitive interpretation of the intercept, which is the propensity to turn out in the 2004 election. Linear mixed models are estimated using the lmer function in R in order to examine the overall pattern of individual differences in voter turnout across election years. Restricted maximum likelihood is used to report model parameters and to assess the significance of random effects.

RESULTS: THE EFFECT OF PHYSICAL HEALTH, DEPRESSION AND SRHS ON TURNOUT

The baseline model reported in Table 1 largely corroborates previous research.Footnote 66 Parental education is strongly related to turnout among offspring in the 2004 election. Respondent education is also important to the initial starting level of turnout in the 2004 election as well as to change. Young adults who are politically interested are more likely to turn out in the 2004 election, and as political interest increases, so does the probability of voting. The election variable is statistically significant, indicating that turnout generally increases over time, although the probability of turning out decreases substantially in midterm elections. Finally, we find that residential mobility is associated with turnout. As a reference, we note that the constant is the probability of turning out in the 2004 election (estimated to be about 15 per cent) when all other variables are 0, which corresponds to never married, white, male, aged 22 years old with twelve years of education whose parents have an average level of education and who does not follow what is going on in politics.

Table 1 Self-rated Health Status and Depression Affect Different Parts of the Voting Habit

Note: data come from the National Longitudinal Study of Youth 1997. Cell entries are coefficients from a multi-level logistic regression; standard errors are listed in parentheses. **p<0.05, *p<0.10

We now turn to the effects of health, which are presented in Models 1–4 in Table 1. Past research shows that individuals with physical limitations are less likely to turn out,Footnote 67 as are those with depressive symptomsFootnote 68 and poor SRHS.Footnote 69 Our results suggest that inferences about the effect of health on political participation need to be qualified, particularly when applied to young adults. As shown in Model 1 of Table 1, we find that physical health during adolescence is not related to voter turnout in the 2004 election or change thereafter, even when other health indicators are absent from the model.Footnote 70 The developmental model suggests that the effect of physical health will be greatest for habitual voters, and therefore negligible for young voters. The estimates presented in Models 1 and 4 in Table 1 are consistent with that argument.

Unlike physical health, depression is associated with lower rates of turnout over time, as is shown in Model 2 of Table 1. Figure 2 illustrates these effects by plotting the trajectory in the predicted probabilities of voting for individuals who had average levels of depression in 2004, but who differed in terms of how their depressive symptoms changed over time.Footnote 71 As seen in Figure 2, both types of individuals had a low probability of voting in the 2004 election (13 per cent). Yet individuals who experienced a decline in depression nearly doubled their probability of turning out in the next presidential election (24 per cent), while those whose symptoms worsened over time saw no growth in their propensity to turn out in the 2008 election (15 per cent). By the 2010 election, these two types of individuals differ in their probability of voting by a four-point gap even though starting levels in 2004 are the same – a not insubstantial change given the overall low level of turnout among young adults. An otherwise larger improvement or worsening of depressive symptoms would further widen this gap. Fluctuations in depression over the life course are not uncommon. While almost half of respondents had changes in their symptoms between 2004 and 2010 within a standard deviation, about one-third of respondents saw their depressive symptoms decline by over a standard deviation, while another one-fifth saw their symptoms rise by more than one standard deviation.

Fig. 2 Predicted probabilities of voting across levels of depression and change in depression

Finally, the results in Model 3 show that SRHS affects starting turnout levels in 2004, but has little effect thereafter, much like the effect of parental education. The predicted probability of turning out in the 2004 election for young adults who report being in excellent health is 15 per cent, which is larger than the 9 per cent of young adults who report being in poor health. While this effect is small, it is comparable in size to the difference in turnout between an individual who follows what is going on in the government ‘some of the time’ (24 per cent predicted probability of voting in 2004) compared to ‘hardly at all’ (15 per cent predicted probability of voting in 2004).

We replicate the findings from Table 1 using the PSID and report these results in Appendix Table B.1. The results of the PSID analysis reveal that SRHS is negatively associated with the initial probability of voting, while depression negatively affects growth in voting over time. The similarity between the NLSY and PSID results affirms our conclusion that health affects turnout, but it depends on what aspect of health and turnout are considered. Nevertheless, two differences in the substantive effects are worth noting. First, the NLSY model tends to underestimate participation levels compared to the PSID model. This difference means that the effect sizes of depression and SRHS observed in the NLSY model are probably smaller than their actual effect on turnout in the population. Secondly, while the NLSY model shows that increases in depression over the course of young adulthood can stunt the development of a voting habit, this effect was not as strongly observed in the PSID. The voting trajectory of those with depression is flattened in the PSID relative to those with consistently average or improved levels of depression, but it is not flat as in the NLSY.

Interactions with Parental Education

While the findings from Table 1 show that different aspects of health affect the development of a voting habit at different points in the process, they tell us nothing about how this process interacts with other childhood and adolescent resources that are important to political participation. In this section we examine how parental education conditions the effects of health on voting by including interaction terms between parental education and each measure of health. We report these results in Table 2. Model 1 shows that physical limitations do not affect political participation at any value of parental education. This comports with our hypothesis that physical limitations matter later in life, and therefore should not reveal an effect regardless of parental education.

Table 2 The Effect of Self-rated Health Status on Voting is Conditional on Parental Education

Note: data come from the National Longitudinal Study of Youth 1997. Cell entries are coefficients from a multi-level logistic regression; standard errors are listed in parentheses. **p<0.05, *p<0.10

Next we turn to depression. The results of Model 2 in Table 2 reveal that the effects of depression documented earlier hold regardless of parental education levels. As we noted before, depression does not affect the starting level of turnout but does shape the voting trajectory. This finding does not follow our hypothesis of cumulative disadvantage, in which those from disadvantaged backgrounds would be hit hardest by depression because they would not have other resources to compensate for the lack of motivation and physical fatigue induced by depression.

Perhaps the most interesting and surprising results are those related to how parental education conditions the effect of SRHS. Model 3 of Table 2 reveals that the interaction term for each measure of SRHS is statistically significant and in the opposite direction from the other, suggesting that changes in health over time can have opposing effects on turnout depending on parental education. This finding holds in Model 4 after controlling for physical limitations and depression. For ease of interpretation, Figure 3 plots the probability of voting in 2008 for four illustrative cases in which health either changed or stayed the same between 2004 and 2008 – poor to poor, poor to excellent, excellent to poor, and excellent to excellent – across all the potential values of parental education.

Fig. 3 Predicted probabilities of voting across levels of SRHS and parental education

Two noticeable patterns emerge from this graph. First, the probability of voting increases with parental education when health is consistent over time: those who are always in excellent health are more likely to participate than those who are always in poor health. Secondly, the effect of changes in SRHS over time is conditional upon parental education. Those who transitioned from poor health to excellent health are the least likely to vote when parental education is low, but the most likely when parental education is high. The exact opposite is true for those who transition from excellent health to poor health. These results do not comport with a story about cumulative disadvantage as we originally imagined, but instead suggest that a lack of resources can mobilize turnout under the auspices of a grievance. We elaborate on this possibility in the discussion, but do so cautiously given that the results do not replicate in our analysis of the PSID (reported in Appendix Table B.2).

Interactions with Family Income

Next we turn to the conditional effect of family income. As shown in Table 3, none of the interaction terms are statistically significant. These findings thus mirror those reported in Table 1 and do not change our understanding of health, except to the extent that the lack of statistically significant interaction terms indicates that the effects of health on participation are present regardless of economic background. The replication of these results using the PSID is reported in Appendix Table B.3.

Table 3 Family Income Does Not Condition the Health-turnout Linkage

Note: data come from the National Longitudinal Study of Youth 1997. Cell entries are coefficients from a multi-level logistic regression; standard errors are listed in parentheses. **p<0.05, *p<0.10

CONCLUSIONS

Our findings advance research on voter turnout more broadly and the health–participation gap more specifically. Regarding turnout, the results of this article challenge the commonly held view that the probability of voting uniformly increases throughout young adulthood. We show that certain conditions – in this case, depression – dampen the probability of voting in spite of all the changes in young adulthood, such as education, mobilization and familiarization with the political process – that would typically lead to a positive voting trajectory during this period. Because of the age limitations of our data, it remains to be seen whether the negative voting trajectory that results from depression is temporary or permanent, but even the possibility that it is permanent should be of concern, given the habitual nature of voting.

This study also contributes to the larger research agenda on health and politics. Recent research, much of it conducted in isolation, finds that various facets of adult health are associated with political participation. We tie together and strengthen the inferences of these disparate findings using a developmental framework that incorporates multiple dimensions of adolescent and young adult health. Doing so allows us to distinguish the effect of health on starting levels of turnout from the voting trajectory. We find that: (1) self-rated health reduces the initial probability of voting but not the voting trajectory, (2) depression slows (and sometimes even reverses) an individual’s voting trajectory and (3) physical limitations do not have a statistically significant effect on either the initial probability of voting or a citizen’s trajectory in young adulthood; however, this does not rule out the possibility of an effect later in life.

That these findings were replicable across datasets bolsters the strength of our results and more generally solidifies the idea that health affects participation. Importantly, the findings have larger implications for social and political processes beyond voter turnout. In particular, they contribute to emerging scholarship on the ‘new inequality paradigm’, which emphasizes the role of interdisciplinary research in highlighting and documenting the breadth and depth of social, economic and political inequalities around the world.Footnote 72 With respect to health and politics, research elsewhere suggests that the diminution of political voice due to health inequalities has political consequencesFootnote 73 that may reinforce initial inequalities in health.Footnote 74 If this is the case, then knowing when and how health inequalities lead to inequality in political voice is important for breaking this cycle. Our findings suggest that breaking the health–participation linkage cannot be done by treating health as a monolith – in which individuals are simply characterized as healthy or unhealthy – but requires recognizing the multidimensionality of health. Indeed, severing the health–participation linkage will require diverse efforts: encouraging participation among young adults with poor-rated health, mobilizing individuals with depression and potentially findings ways to facilitate participation among older citizens with physical limitations.

As research on health and politics continues to grow, we see three important lines of research. First, scholars should continue exploring the effect of health on political behavior by considering other forms of participation and political attitudes. The majority of the literature focuses on voter turnout. Voting is arguably one of the easiest forms of political participation, and may therefore be less affected by health disparities than other forms of participation, such as working in campaigns or in the community, contacting government officials and contributing money.Footnote 75 In addition to alternative forms of participation, scholars should also consider other types of health indicators. For example, research on the mental health–participation linkage has thus far focused primarily on depression. Other forms of mental health should also be considered – potentially including anxiety, externalizing and substance-related behaviors, and personality disorders. Examining several measures of mental health will illuminate which aspects matter most for participation. This is also the case with specific chronic conditions. Gollust and Rahn show that both cancer and heart disease affect voting in different ways.Footnote 76 Sund and colleagues show that mental health disorders, alcoholism and neurodegenerative brain diseases reduce the probability of voting, while cancer, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and asthma do the opposite.Footnote 77 Future research should continue to theoretically and empirically distinguish between when specific chronic conditions are expected to depress turnout and when they become motivating factors for participation (for example, participation and advocacy among women who have, or have had, breast cancer).

A second line of research would focus on the policy consequences of health-based inequalities in participation. Are individuals in poor health under-represented compared to their healthy counterparts? Research shows a direct link between inequalities in turnout and policy responsiveness, yet the majority of this research concentrates on class-based politics.Footnote 78 Since voters are better represented than non-voters, itFootnote 79 stands to reason that healthy individuals may also enjoy better responsiveness. Preliminary evidence is suggestive. For instance, Pacheco finds that healthy individuals are better represented by their congressional representatives compared to unhealthy citizens; this positive relationship is particularly true for individuals who are affluent as well as when citizens are represented by Republicans.Footnote 80 Determining the extent of under-representation and its policy consequences are important for identifying the magnitude of the problem.

Finally, having firmly established a health–participation link, scholars should turn their attention to examining how the under-representation of individuals in poor health reinforces both poor health and health-based inequalities in participation. Institutions and policies shape how individuals experience health and navigate the health system, which raises the possibility that the health–participation link is self-reinforcing. Evaluating how access to (and the quality of) healthcare is shaped by healthcare policy and then experienced differently across racial, gender and class identities could be important in understanding how institutions and policies can close the health–participation gap. For example, recent changes in the state of healthcare due to the Affordable Care Act (ACA), as well as challenges and changes to the ACA, means that the healthcare landscape is constantly changing. Evaluating the policy consequences of the ACA, as well as state-level variation in its implementation, will reveal the extent to which access to and the quality of healthcare can magnify the political voice of individuals in poor health. These suggestions are forward looking, but should in no way obscure the noteworthy contributions of the current study. To reiterate, we add to a growing body of research on health and participation through a more detailed analysis of longitudinal data. We show that health politics research must consider the multidimensionality of health as well as the habit of voting to fully understand what the health–participation gap represents.

Footnotes

*

Institute for Research in the Social Sciences, Stanford University (email: cojeda@stanford.edu); Department of Political Science, University of Iowa (email: julianna-pacheco@uiowa.edu). Data replication sets are available in Harvard Dataverse at: doi:10.7910/DVN/ZTJWUF and online appendices are available at https://doi.org/doi:10.1017/S0007123417000151.

1 Verba, Schlozman, and Brady Reference Verba, Schlozman and Brady1995.

4 Pacheco and Fletcher Reference Pacheco and Fletcher2015.

5 Roberts and Booske Reference Robert and Booske2011.

7 Goodman, Joyce, and Smith Reference Goodman, Joyce and Smith2011, 6032.

8 For an exception, see Denny and Doyle Reference Denny and Doyle2007; Ojeda Reference Ojeda2015.

9 Miller and Shanks 1996.

10 Bhatti, Hansen, and Wass Reference Bhatti, Hansen and Wass2012.

12 Pacheco and Fletcher Reference Pacheco and Fletcher2015.

13 Aldrich, Montgomery, and Wood 2010; Coppock and Green Reference Coppock and Green2015; Gerber, Green, and Shacher Reference Gerber, Green and Shachar2003; Milbrath Reference Milbrath1965; Plutzer Reference Plutzer2002.

14 Plutzer Reference Plutzer2002, 41.

17 Rosenstone and Hanson Reference Rosenstone and John Mark1993.

18 Highton and Wolfinger 2001, 202.

21 E.g., Schur and Kruse Reference Schur and Kruse2000.

22 Alvarez, Levin, and Sinclair Reference Alvarez, Levin and Sinclair2012.

26 Hobbes, Christakis, and Fowler Reference Hobbes, Christakis and Fowler2014.

27 Bhatti and Hansen Reference Bhatti and Hansen2012.

28 Schur, Shields, and Schriner Reference Schur, Shields and Schriner2005.

30 There are two distinct, but interrelated issues related to chronic diseases/illness/conditions: empowerment and limitations. The literature on identity politics suggests that chronic disease may lead some people to identify as ‘survivors’, which may increase empowerment and participation. Others, however, may not adopt such an identity and instead be hindered by the physical limitations of the condition. We focus mostly on the latter in this article.

31 APA 2000.

32 Lavretsky and Kumar Reference Lavretsky and Kumar2002.

34 Alvarez, Levin, and Sinclair Reference Alvarez, Levin and Sinclair2012.

35 Benfer Reference Benfer2009. Although mental health problems were classified as a disability in 2008, disability research has measured mental disability for much longer, using the question ‘Because of a physical, mental, or emotional condition, does anyone have serious difficulty concentrating, remembering, or making decisions?’ (Schur et al. Reference Schur, Shields, Kruse and Schriner2002). While this question taps into the concept of mental health, the broadness of the question makes it ambiguous about which aspect of mental health matters. While all ‘no’ responses to this question will be equivalent, all ‘yes’ answers are not equivalent and can mean quite different things. For example, an affirmative response is more likely among individuals with depression, individuals with concussions, senior citizens (i.e., aging), individuals with anxiety, individuals with learning disabilities (which themselves can be quite diverse), individuals with substance-related disorders, and so forth. Therefore past research that uses this measure tells us little about how or why mental health matters. By focusing on depression here, we can begin to examine how different aspects of mental health affect participation.

36 E.g., Greenberg et al. 2003.

37 Schriner, Ochs, and Shields Reference Schriner, Ochs and Shields1997.

38 Bjorner, Fayers, and Idler Reference Bjorner, Fayers and Idler2005; Jylhä Reference Jylhä2009.

39 Pacheco and Fletcher Reference Pacheco and Fletcher2015.

40 Idler and Benyamini Reference Idler and Benyamini1997, 34.

42 National Research Council 2000.

44 Schlozman, Verba, and Brady Reference Schlozman, Verba and Brady2012.

45 An important caveat is that we do expect SRHS to impact the voting trajectories of older voters, as suggested by Pacheco and Fletcher (Reference Pacheco and Fletcher2015). This is primarily because what it means to be in poor health varies with age (Groot Reference Groot2000). SRHS is related to health trajectories across the lifespan (Ferraro Reference Ferraro2006), often reflecting how an individual’s health is changing and is therefore a useful measure to explore changes in political behavior that occur as people age. Unfortunately, testing such hypotheses is not possible in this article given the youthfulness of our sample.

47 Gigendel, Wass, and Valaste Reference Gigendel, Wass and Valaste2016; Plutzer Reference Plutzer2002.

49 Smets and van Ham Reference Smets and van Ham2013.

51 Rosenstone Reference Rosenstone1982; Smets and van Ham Reference Smets and van Ham2013; Verba, Scholzman, and Brady Reference Verba, Schlozman and Brady1995.

52 Adler and Newman Reference Adler and Newman2002.

53 Pacheco and Fletcher Reference Pacheco and Fletcher2015.

54 Soss and Jacobs Reference Soss and Jacobs2009.

55 Pacheco and Plutzer Reference Pacheco and Plutzer2008.

56 Singer and Willet Reference Singer and Willett2003.

57 Plutzer Reference Plutzer2002. The only variable that has a statistically significant impact on subsequent growth is parental strength of partisanship. Unfortunately, the NLSY97 does not ask parents about partisanship.

58 We can relax this assumption by interacting the time-invariant variables with the election variable or other time-varying variables. We do so in later analyses when exploring conditional effects by parental education and family income.

59 Singer and Willett Reference Singer and Willett2003.

60 Empirically, this is equivalent to including an interaction variable between the 2004 measures and the election variable (e.g., time variable). In both specifications, we are able to assess how early onset of poor health influences the typical growth in the propensity of turning out.

61 Although these measures do not perfectly reflect standard practice in the survey measurement of depression – the gold standard of which is the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale – they do broadly capture the constellation of symptoms used to diagnose depression according to the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistics Manual. Specifically, the measure falls short insofar as it fails capture all four subscales – negative feelings, positive feelings, interpersonal problems, and somatic problems – that are informally associated with survey measures of depression. However, that our measure is a noisy approximation of depressive symptoms means that our analysis will be hampered by measurement error thus making any effect of depression more difficult to detect.

62 The correlations among the health indicators are low, suggesting that each is measuring distinct concepts. The correlation between SRHS and depression is 0.27, the correlation between SRHS and limitations in 2002 is 0.09, and the correlation between depression and limitations in 2002 is 0.06.

63 We do not include a time varying measure of age since it is collinear with the election variable.

66 E.g., Plutzer Reference Plutzer2002.

70 This remains true even when we exclude asthma from our operationalization of physical limitations. Asthma comprises more than half of all cases of physical limitation, but may be expected to have the smallest effect on turnout, a combination that could obscure the effect of other important chronic conditions. We do not find this to be the case.

71 Recall that this estimate is predicted keeping all other variables at 0, which refers to a never married, white, male, aged 22 years old with twelve years of education whose parents have an average level of education and who does not follow what is going on in politics.

75 Verba, Nie, and Kim Reference Verba, Nie and Kim1978.

76 Gollust and Rahn Reference Gollust and Wendy2015.

78 E.g., Gilens Reference Gilens2009; Gilens and Page Reference Gilens and Page2014.

79 Griffin and Newman Reference Griffin and Newman2005.

References

Adler, Nancy E., and Newman, Katherine. 2002. Socioeconomic Disparities in Health: Pathways and Policies. Health Affairs 21 (2):6076.Google Scholar
Aldrich, John H., Montgomery, Jacob M., and Wood, Wendy. 2010. Turnout as Habit. Political Behavior 33 (4):535563.Google Scholar
Alvarez, Michael R., Levin, Ines, and Sinclair, J. Andrew. 2012. Making Voting Easier: Convenience Voting in the 2008 Presidential Election. Political Research Quarterly 65:248262.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Benfer, Emily A. 2009. The ADA Amendments Act: An Overview of Recent Changes to the Americans with Disabilities Act. Washington, DC: American Constitution Society for Law and Policy.Google Scholar
Berger, Michele Tracy. 2004. Workable Sisterhood: The Political Journey of Stigmatized Women with HIV/AIDS. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bhatti, Yosef, and Hansen, Kasper M.. 2012. Retiring from Voting: Turnout among Senior Voters. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion, and Parties 22 (4):479500.Google Scholar
Bhatti, Yosef, Hansen, Kasper M., and Wass, Hanna. 2012. The Relationship Between Age and Turnout: A Roller-Coaster Ride. Electoral Studies 31:588593.Google Scholar
Bjorner, Jakob B., Fayers, P. M., and Idler, E. L.. 2005. Self-rated Health. in Assessing Quality of Life, edited by Peter Fayers and Ron Hays, 309323. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Case, Anne, Fertig, Angela, and Paxson, Christina. 2005. The Lasting Impact of Childhood Health and Circumstance. Journal of Health Economics 24:365389.Google Scholar
Clarke, Philippa J, Ailshire, Jennifer A., Nieuwenhuijens, Els R., and de Kleijn-de Vrankrijker, Marijke W.. 2011. Participation Among Adults With Disability: The Role of the Urban Environment. Social Science & Medicine 72:16741684.Google Scholar
Coppock, Alexander, and Green, Donald P.. 2015. Is Voting Habit Forming? New Evidence from Experiments and Regression Discontinuities. American Journal of Political Science 60 (4):10441062.Google Scholar
Cowell, Alexander J. 2005. The Relationship Between Education and Health Behavior: Some Empirical Evidence. Health Economics 15 (2):125146.Google Scholar
Currie, Janet, Stabile, Mark, Manivong, Phongsack, and Roos, Leslie L.. 2009. Child Health and Young Adult Outcomes. Journal of Human Resources 45 (3):517548.Google Scholar
Denny, Kevin J., and Doyle, Orla M.. 2007. …‘Take Up Thy Bed, and Vote’ Measuring the Relationship Between Voting Behaviour and Indicators of Health. European Journal of Public Health 17 (4):400401.Google Scholar
Ferraro, Keneth F. 2006. Health and Aging. Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences 6:238256.Google Scholar
Gerber, Alan S., Green, Donald P., and Shachar, Ron. 2003. Voting May be Habit Forming: Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment. American Journal of Political Science 47 (3):540550.Google Scholar
Gigendel, Elisabeth, Wass, Hanna, and Valaste, Maria. 2016. Political Socialization and Voting: The Parent–Child Link in Turnout. Political Research Quarterly 60 (2):373383.Google Scholar
Gilens, Martin. 2009. Preference Gaps and Inequality in Representation. PS: Political Science & Politics 42 (2):335341.Google Scholar
Gilens, Martin, and Page, Benjamin I.. 2014. Testing Theories of American Politics: Elites, Interest groups, and Average Citizens. Perspectives on Politics 12 (3):564581.Google Scholar
Goodman, Alissa, Joyce, Robert, and Smith, James P.. 2011. The Long Shadow Cast by Childhood Physical and Mental Problems on Adult Life. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 108 (15):60326037.Google Scholar
Gollust, Sarah E., and Wendy, Rahn. 2015. The Bodies Politic: Chronic Health Conditions and Voter Turnout in the 2008 Election. Journal of Health Politics, Policy and Law 40 (6):11151155.Google Scholar
Greenberg, Paul E., Kessler, Ronald C., Birnbaum, Howard G., Leong, Stephanie A., Lowe, Sarah W., Berglund, Patricia A., and Corey-Lisle, Patricia K.. 2003. The Economic Burden of Depression in the United States: How Did It Change between 1990 and 2000? Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 64 (12):14651475.Google Scholar
Griffin, John D., and Newman, B.. 2005. Are Voters Better Represented? Journal of Politics 67 (4):12061227.Google Scholar
Groot, Wim. 2000. Adaptation and Scale of Reference Bias in Self-Assessments of Quality of Life. Journal of Health Economics 19 (3):403420.Google Scholar
Harrington, Richard, Fudge, Hazel, Rutter, Michael, Pickles, Andrew, and Hill, Jonathan. 1990. Adult Outcomes of Childhood and Adolescent Depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 47:465473.Google Scholar
Hobbes, William, Christakis, Nicholas, and Fowler, James. 2014. Widowhood Effects in Voter Participation. American Journal of Political Science 58 (1):116.Google Scholar
Idler, Ellen L., and Benyamini, Yael. 1997. Self-Rated Health and Mortality: A Review of Twenty-Seven Community Studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 38:2137.Google Scholar
Jylhä, Marja. 2009. What is Self-Rated Health and Why does it Predict Mortality? Towards a Unified Conceptual Model. Social Science & Medicine 69:307316.Google Scholar
King, Samantha. 2008. Pink Ribbons Inc: Breast Cancer Activism and the Politics of Philanthropy. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Lantz, Paula M., House, James S., Lepkowski, James M., Williams, David R., Mero, Richard P., and Chen, Jieming. 1998. Socioeconomic Factors, Health Behaviors, and Mortality: Results from a Nationally Representative Prospective Study of US Adults. Journal of American Medical Association 279:17031708.Google Scholar
Lavretsky, Helen, and Kumar, Anand. 2002. Clinically Significant Non-Major Depression: Old Concepts, New Insights. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 10 (3):239255.Google Scholar
Marmot, Michael. 2002. The Influence of Income on Health: Views of an Epidemiologist. Health Affairs 21 (2):3146.Google Scholar
Mattila, Mikko, Soderlund, Peter, Wass, Hanna, and Rapeli, Lauri. 2013. Healthy Voting: The Effect of Self-Reported Health on Turnout in 30 Countries. Electoral Studies 32 (4):886891.Google Scholar
Milbrath, Lester. 1965. Political Participation: How and Why Do People Get Involved in Politics? Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
National Research Council. 2000. From Neurons to Neighborhoods. The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Ojeda, Christopher. 2015. Depression and Political Participation. Social Science Quarterly 96 (5):11261243.Google Scholar
Ojeda, Christopher. 2017. “Replication Data for: Health and Voting in Young Adulthood”, doi: 10.7910/DVN/ZTJWUF, Harvard Dataverse, V1, UNF:6:65FxJZJPHdskSxLcvD+Q8w==.Google Scholar
Pacheco, Julianna. 2014. A Conditional Theory of Self-Interest and Symbolic Politics. Presented at the Annual Meetings of the Southern Political Science Association, New Orleans, LA, 3–7 January.Google Scholar
Pacheco, Julianna Sandell, and Plutzer, Eric. 2008. Political Participation and Cumulative Disadvantage: The Impact of Economic and Social Hardship on Young Citizens. Journal of Social Issues 64 (3):571593.Google Scholar
Pacheco, Julianna Sandell, and Fletcher, Jason. 2015. Incorporating Health into Studies of Political Behavior: Evidence for Turnout and Partisanship. Political Research Quarterly 68 (1):104116.Google Scholar
Palloni, Alberto. 2005. Reproducing Inequalities: Luck, Wallets, and the Enduring Effects of Childhood Health. Demography 43:587615.Google Scholar
Plutzer, Eric. 2002. Becoming a Habitual Voter: Inertia, Resources, and Growth in Young Adulthood. American Political Science Review 96 (1):4156.Google Scholar
Robert, Stephanie A, and Booske, BC. 2011. US Opinions on Health Determinants and Social Policy as Health Policy. American Journal of Public Health 101:16551663.Google Scholar
Rosenstone, Steve J. 1982. Economic Adversity and Voter Turnout. American Journal of Political Science 26 (1):2546.Google Scholar
Rosenstone, Steven J., and John Mark, Hansen. 1993. Mobilization, Participation, and Democracy in America. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Savage, Mike. 2016. Are We Seeing a New ‘Inequality Paradigm’ in Social Science? The London School of Economics British Politics and Policy Blog, 2 June.Google Scholar
Schlozman, Kay Lehman, Verba, Sidney, and Brady, Henry E.. 2012. The Unheavenly Chorus. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Schur, Lisa, and Kruse, Douglas. 2000. What Determines Voter Turnout? Lessons from Citizens with Disabilities. Social Science Quarterly 81 (2):571587.Google Scholar
Schur, Lisa, Shields, Todd, Kruse, Douglas, and Schriner, Kay. 2002. Enabling Democracy: Disability and Voter Turnout. Political Research Quarterly 55 (1):167190.Google Scholar
Schur, Lisa, Shields, Todd, and Schriner, Kay. 2005. Generational Cohorts, Group Membership, and Political Participation by People with Disabilities. Political Research Quarterly 58 (3):487496.Google Scholar
Schriner, Kay, Ochs, Lisa A., and Shields, Todd G.. 1997. The Last Suffrage Movement: Voting Rights for Persons with Cognitive and Emotional Disabilities. Publius 27 (3):7596.Google Scholar
Simon, J. G., De Boer, J. B., Joung, I. M. A., Bosma, H., and Mackenbach, J. P.. 2005. How is Your Health in General? A Qualitative Study on Self-assessed Health. European Journal of Public Health 15 (2):200208.Google Scholar
Singer, Judith D., and Willett, John B.. 2003. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurrence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smets, Kaat, and van Ham, Carolien. 2013. The Embarrassment of Riches? A Meta-Analysis of Individual-Level Research on Voter Turnout. Electoral Studies 32:344359.Google Scholar
Soderlund, Peter, and Rapeli, Lauri. 2015. In Sickness and in Health: Personal Health and Political Participation in the Nordic Countries. Politics and the Life Sciences 34 (1):2843.Google Scholar
Soss, Joe, and Jacobs, Lawrence R.. 2009. The Place of Inequality: Non-Participation in the American Polity. Political Science Quarterly 124 (1):95125.Google Scholar
Sund, Reijo, Lahtinen, Hannu, Wass, Hanna, Mattila, Mikko, and Martikainen, Pekka. 2016. How Voter Turnout Varies between Chronic Conditions? A Population-Based Register Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 71 (5):475479.Google Scholar
Verba, Sidney, Schlozman, Kay Lehman, and Brady, Henry. 1995. Voice and Equality: Civic Voluntarism in American Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Verba, Sidney, Nie, Norman H., and Kim, Jae-on. 1978. Participation and Political Equality: A Seven-Nation Comparison. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, Simon J. 1999. Is Anybody There? Critical Realism, Chronic Illness, and the Disability Debate. Sociology of Health & Illness 21 (6):797819.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Reported turnout in each election, NLSY97

Figure 1

Table 1 Self-rated Health Status and Depression Affect Different Parts of the Voting Habit

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Predicted probabilities of voting across levels of depression and change in depression

Figure 3

Table 2 The Effect of Self-rated Health Status on Voting is Conditional on Parental Education

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Predicted probabilities of voting across levels of SRHS and parental education

Figure 5

Table 3 Family Income Does Not Condition the Health-turnout Linkage

Supplementary material: PDF

Ojeda and Pacheco supplementary material

Appendices

Download Ojeda and Pacheco supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 532.4 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Ojeda and Pacheco Dataset

Link