Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Yow, W. Quin
and
Li, Xiaoqian
2015.
Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 6,
Issue. ,
Yang, Sujin
and
Yang, Hwajin
2016.
Bilingual effects on deployment of the attention system in linguistically and culturally homogeneous children and adults.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Vol. 146,
Issue. ,
p.
121.
Kousaie, Shanna
Chai, Xiaoqian J.
Sander, Kaija M.
and
Klein, Denise
2017.
Simultaneous learning of two languages from birth positively impacts intrinsic functional connectivity and cognitive control.
Brain and Cognition,
Vol. 117,
Issue. ,
p.
49.
Delcenserie, Audrey
and
Genesee, Fred
2017.
The effects of age of acquisition on verbal memory in bilinguals.
International Journal of Bilingualism,
Vol. 21,
Issue. 5,
p.
600.
PIERCE, LARA J.
GENESEE, FRED
DELCENSERIE, AUDREY
and
MORGAN, GARY
2017.
Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes.
Applied Psycholinguistics,
Vol. 38,
Issue. 6,
p.
1265.
Daller, Michael
and
Ongun, Zehra
2018.
The Threshold Hypothesis revisited: Bilingual lexical knowledge and non-verbal IQ development.
International Journal of Bilingualism,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 6,
p.
675.
De Cat, Cecile
Gusnanto, Arief
and
Serratrice, Ludovica
2018.
IDENTIFYING A THRESHOLD FOR THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ADVANTAGE IN BILINGUAL CHILDREN.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Vol. 40,
Issue. 1,
p.
119.
De Houwer, Annick
and
Ortega, Lourdes
2018.
The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism.
Cornips, Leonie
2018.
Bilingual Cognition and Language.
Vol. 54,
Issue. ,
p.
15.
Whitford, Veronica
and
Luk, Gigi
2019.
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond.
Vol. 57,
Issue. ,
p.
67.
Antoniou, Mark
2019.
The Advantages of Bilingualism Debate.
Annual Review of Linguistics,
Vol. 5,
Issue. 1,
p.
395.
SURRAIN, SARAH
and
LUK, GIGI
2019.
Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 2,
p.
401.
CHAMPOUX-LARSSON, MARIE-FRANCE
and
DYLMAN, ALEXANDRA S.
2019.
A prosodic bias, not an advantage, in bilinguals' interpretation of emotional prosody.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 2,
p.
416.
DeLuca, Vincent
2019.
Future Directions in Examining Neurological Adaptation to Bilingual Experiences.
Journal of Experimental Neuroscience,
Vol. 13,
Issue. ,
SABOURIN, LAURA
and
VĪNERTE, SANTA
2019.
Cognitive control among immersed bilinguals: Considering differences in linguistic and non-linguistic processing.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 3,
p.
590.
De Cat, Cécile
2020.
PREDICTING LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY IN BILINGUAL CHILDREN.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Vol. 42,
Issue. 2,
p.
279.
Jansen, Silke
Higuera del Moral, Sonja
Barzen, Jessica Stefanie
Reimann, Pia
and
Opolka, Markus
2021.
Demystifying Bilingualism.
p.
81.
Francot, Ryanne
Blom, Elma
Broekhuizen, Martine
and
Leseman, Paul
2021.
Profiles of bilingualism in early childhood: A person-centred Latent Profile Transition Approach.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
Vol. 24,
Issue. 3,
p.
569.
Kremin, Lena V.
and
Byers-Heinlein, Krista
2021.
Why not both? Rethinking categorical and continuous approaches to bilingualism.
International Journal of Bilingualism,
Vol. 25,
Issue. 6,
p.
1560.
Gade, Miriam
Declerck, Mathieu
Philipp, Andrea M.
Rey-Mermet, Alodie
and
Koch, Iring
2021.
Assessing the Evidence for Asymmetrical Switch Costs and Reversed
Language Dominance Effects – A Meta-Analysis.
Journal of Cognition,
Vol. 4,
Issue. 1,
In the keynote article, “Bilingualism and Cognition”, Valian (Reference Valian2014) has reviewed current research on comparing executive function (EF) in monolingual and bilingual individuals across the lifespan. The conclusion is that there are inconsistent EF advantages from bilingualism and all other cognitive challenging activities primarily because individual differences in these cognitive challenging experiences may collectively attribute to superior EF resulting in inconsistent EF benefit attributable to a single experience. In essence, variability in study participants’ experience and tasks contributes to the inconsistency in the behavioral outcomes observed in monolinguals and bilinguals. Notably, Valian suggests that monolinguals may also engage in other cognitively challenging activities, which have not been accounted for in individual studies, thereby resulting in improved EF similar in magnitude to that related to bilingual experience. Although it was not specified which cognitively challenging activity is more likely to be engaged by monolinguals more than by bilinguals, the question at heart is: is there an EF advantage that can be specifically attributed to bilingual experience? The review addressed in the keynote demonstrates seemingly inconsistent patterns of results. In this commentary, I would like to suggest that, in addition to task measurements, individual bilingual experience is dynamic and multifaceted. Moreover, bilingual experience varies in different communities. Consequently, one potential source of explanation for the inconsistent results in between-group EF performances is the characteristics of the bilinguals (and monolinguals) and their social environments included in these studies.
Who are the bilinguals (and monolinguals)? As much as researchers would like to have mutually exclusive groups including individuals with bilingual or monolingual experience, it may not be realistic because bilingualism and monolingualism may represent extremes of a multidimensional spectrum, such as history of language acquisition (e.g., Luk, De Sa & Bialystok, Reference Luk, de Sa and Bialystok2011), language usage (e.g, Luk & Bialystok, Reference Luk and Bialystok2013; Prior & Gollan, Reference Prior and Gollan2011), and second language proficiency (e.g., Mishra, Hilchey, Singh & Klein, Reference Mishra, Hilchey, Singh and Klein2012; Tse & Altarriba, Reference Tse and Altarriba2014). The exact nature of how these dimensions in bilingual experience interact with each other and components representing EF is still unclear. Therefore, it is imperative to consider participants’ characteristics when interpreting differences in behavioral outcomes. This consideration should apply to both bilingual and monolingual participants. Given that bilingualism is influenced by social interaction (Grosjean, Reference Grosjean1982; Lambert, Reference Lambert1967), it is also important to report the social environment pertaining to both the bilingual and monolingual participants (for an extensive global perspective on this topic, see Romaine, 2012). Although bilinguals may have a choice to retain usage of another language, this choice is likely driven by the social value of using a minority language. In summary, bilingual characteristics should be reported for both the intraindividual (dimensions of bilingualism) and interindividual (social values of bilingualism vs. monolingualism) domains.
Relevant to EF performance is the intensity and duration of bilingual experience engaged by the participants persistently. Cross-sectional between-group comparisons may introduce group differences unrelated to bilingual experience, as suggested by Valian (Reference Valian2014). One critical question is, how much bilingual experience is enough? This question can only be speculated in a longitudinal design or prospective cohort study. A recent study by Macnamara and Conway (Reference Macnamara and Conway2014) demonstrated that American Sign Language (ASL)–English interpretation students showed significant gains in cognitive control and working memory after two years of high demand of managing both ASL and English. These behavioral gains were specific to the experience of needing to manage two languages, but not related to preexisting cognitive abilities and other possible confounding factors. Findings from this study demonstrate that sufficient intensive bilingual management is necessary to enjoy cognitive enhancements. In a large-scale prospective longitudinal study involving 964 older adults, those who reported receiving longer duration of foreign language instruction before the age of 18 had a lower risk of mild cognitive impairment after controlling for age, sex and education (Wilson, Boyle, Yang, James & Bennett, Reference Wilson, Boyle, Yang, James and Bennett2014). Similar findings were also reported for those with music instruction. Together, these findings point to the importance of qualifying and quantifying bilingual experience when interpreting between-group comparisons.
Aside from the intensity of demand involved in managing two languages, second language proficiency also plays a role in relation to EF performance. Linck, Osthus, Koeth and Bunting (Reference Linck, Osthus, Koeth and Bunting2014) reported that second language proficiency (comprehension and production) was associated with working memory, in a meta-analysis involving 79 independent study samples of 3707 adult participants. The effect sizes were relatively larger in verbal working memory measures and executive control component, suggesting second language processing may recruit domain-general processes. In addition, Fernandez, Tartar, Padron and Acosta (Reference Fernandez, Tartar, Padron and Acosta2013) and Tse and Altarriba (Reference Tse and Altarriba2014) reported that second language proficiency correlated with N2 amplitude (an ERP component representing inhibition of motor response or response selection) and reaction time distributions respectively in the Simon task. Critically, these findings suggest the importance of considering second language proficiency when evaluating EF performance in bilinguals.
Recent neuroimaging research has suggested that overlapping brain regions showed activity between bilingual language control and domain-general cognitive control (e.g., Abutalebi, Della Rosa, Green, Hernandez, Scifo, Keim, Cappa & Costa, Reference Abutalebi, Della Rosa, Green, Hernandez, Scifo, Keim, Cappa and Costa2012; de Bruin, Roelofs, Dijkstra & Fitzpatrick, Reference de Bruin, Roelofs, Dijkstra and Fitzpatrick2014; for a meta-analysis, see Luk, Green, Abutalebi & Grady, Reference Luk, Green, Abutalebi and Grady2012). Although neuroimaging findings do not imply underlying cognitive mechanisms, when overlapping brain regions engage in bilingual language control and EF, it suggests that managing multiple languages is not a (neurologically) modular process. Complementing Valian's suggestions of focusing on specific EF components relevant to bilingualism and conducting detailed task analyses, I recommend examining bilingualism and cognition using multiple methodologies: such as testing specific hypothesis in large-scale cohort datasets, designing theory-driven behavioral tasks and adopting multimodal neuroimaging methods. Furthermore, bilingualism provides a unique window to examine the interaction between experience-expectant (e.g., maturational) and experience-dependent (e.g., interaction between an individual and the environment) mechanisms. More detailed report and analysis of the diversity in experience-dependent mechanisms is needed to advance research on bilingualism and cognition.