Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T07:55:59.360Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Towards a bioecological model of bilingual development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2016

ADRIANA WEISLEDER*
Affiliation:
New York University School of Medicine
*
Address for correspondence: Adriana Weisleder, Ph.D., New York University School of Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, 550 First Avenue, OBV A529, New York, NY 10016, U.S.A.adriana.weisleder@nyumc.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

At its best, the field of bilingualism is a dynamic discipline informed by diverse perspectives. Carroll's critical review – situated within a linguistics framework – contends that current research on bilingual exposure and language development suffers from a lack of theoretical clarity, and makes little contribution to our understanding of bilingual acquisition. Carroll's push (Carroll) towards greater precision in our thinking about the relation between input and outcomes is an important and welcome challenge. However, it is also critical to keep in mind the social context that motivates much of the current research on bilingual development, and to leave room for studies whose main goal is to provide answers to societally important questions about bilingual children's health and development.

Type
Peer Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

At its best, the field of bilingualism is a dynamic discipline informed by diverse perspectives. Carroll's critical review – situated within a linguistics framework – contends that current research on bilingual exposure and language development suffers from a lack of theoretical clarity, and makes little contribution to our understanding of bilingual acquisition. Carroll's push (Carroll) towards greater precision in our thinking about the relation between input and outcomes is an important and welcome challenge. However, it is also critical to keep in mind the social context that motivates much of the current research on bilingual development, and to leave room for studies whose main goal is to provide answers to societally important questions about bilingual children's health and development.

In many countries, policy discussions regarding bilingualism have focused on the potential for bilingual children to experience delays in language development relative to monolinguals (Hoff, Reference Hoff2013; Oller & Eilers, Reference Oller and Eilers2002). Research on language exposure and bilingual development has been essential in reframing these discussions, shifting the focus away from concerns about the ‘risks’ of bilingualism – which are unfounded – and focusing instead on the contexts that best support language development in multilingual children (McCabe, Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein, Cates, Golinkoff, Guerra, Hirsh-Pasek, Hoff, Kuchirko, Melzi, Mendelsohn, Páez & Song, Reference McCabe, Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein, Cates, Golinkoff, Guerra, Hirsh-Pasek, Hoff, Kuchirko, Melzi, Mendelsohn, Páez and Song2013). In this context, understanding the social and experiential factors that enable children to become proficient users of more than one language has emerged as a priority.

Linguistic and psychological theories have played an important role in guiding research into the conditions that support bilingual development. Conceptualized within a bioecological model (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, Reference Bronfenbrenner and Ceci1994), much of this work has centered on language exposure as a key aspect of children's ‘proximal’ experience, while also considering more ‘distal’ social and cultural factors that shape children's language-learning environments (Pearson, Reference Pearson2007; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder, Fernald, Arnon, Casillas, Kurumada and Estigarribia2014). Critically, studies have shown that the amount of exposure to each language has a proportional relation to various aspects of bilingual children's development in that language, including vocabulary (Place & Hoff, Reference Place and Hoff2011), speed of lexical processing (Hurtado, Grüter, Marchman, & Fernald, Reference Hurtado, Grüter, Marchman and Fernald2013), and some aspects of grammatical ability (Paradis, Tremblay & Crago, Reference Paradis, Tremblay, Crago, Paradis and Grüter2014; Thordardottir, Reference Thordardottir2015). Carroll highlights important limitations in the measures of language exposure used in these studies, which are based on parents’ reports of the relative amount of time children hear each language. Fortunately, novel methods are now emerging that facilitate direct observation of children's language exposure through collection of daylong naturalistic recordings, thus enabling more in-depth investigation of the quantity and quality of speech that children hear during a typical day. Importantly, recent studies using these methods confirm that amount of language exposure is related to vocabulary size and speed of lexical processing in both monolingual and bilingual children (Marchman, Martínez, Hurtado, Grüter & Fernald, in press; Weisleder & Fernald, Reference Weisleder and Fernald2013).

Beyond these methodological concerns, Carroll questions whether broad measures of exposure tell us anything meaningful about the input children use. This is a good reminder of the importance of using multiple methods and levels of analysis to approach the same question. While these studies don't tell us how learners use the input to acquire specific linguistic phenomena, they show that amount of exposure is an ecologically meaningful construct with considerable explanatory power, and suggest the involvement of learning mechanisms that are sensitive to frequencies in the input. In future research, it will be important to examine how specific aspects of language experience relate to different language outcomes. This research can help identify properties of language exposure that relate to language learning in the context of children's real-life experiences, and thus suggest new targets for investigation in experimental studies.

From a practical standpoint, this literature has made meaningful progress on key questions important to parents, educators, clinicians and policymakers. It has reaffirmed the idea that bilingualism itself does not place children at risk for language delay, yet it also points to the language environment as a potential source of the differences observed between the language abilities of bilingual and monolingual children. Because many bilingual children in the United States come from low-income households and are at risk for poor academic outcomes, it is imperative to continue studying the many sources of variability driving individual differences in language development, and to inform policies that strengthen learning outcomes for all children. Carroll's keynote underscores the importance of research that connects theories of language acquisition to questions about how social environments can support bilingual development.

References

Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101 (4), 568586.Google Scholar
Carroll, S. E. Exposure and input in bilingual development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-ses and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49, 14.Google Scholar
Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., Marchman, V., & Fernald, A. (2013). Relative language exposure, processing efficiency and vocabulary in Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17 (01), 189202.Google Scholar
Marchman, V., Martínez, L., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A. (in press). Caregiver talk to young Spanish-English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Cates, C. B., Golinkoff, R. M, Guerra, A. W., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Hoff, E., Kuchirko, Y., Melzi, G., Mendelsohn, A., Páez, M., & Song, L. (2013). SRCD Social Policy Report. Multilingual Children: Beyond Myths and Toward Best Practices (Vol. 27).Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children (Vol. 2). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., Tremblay, A., & Crago, M. (2014). French-English bilingual children's sensitivity to child-level and language-level input factors in morphosyntactic acquisition. In Paradis, J. & Grüter, T. (Eds.), Input and Experience in Bilingual Development (pp. 161180). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z. (2007). Social factors in childhood bilingualism in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28 (03), 399410.Google Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82 (6), 18341849.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E. (2015). The relationship between bilingual exposure and morphosyntactic development. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17 (2), 97114.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24 (11), 2143–52.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2014). Social environments shape children's language experiences, strengthening language processing and building vocabulary. In Arnon, I., Casillas, M., Kurumada, C., & Estigarribia, B. (Eds.), Language in Interaction. Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark (pp. 2949). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar