Hostname: page-component-69cd664f8f-k8xkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-13T10:25:44.115Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Digital Language Learning and SLA

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2021

ZhaoHong Han*
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Dr. ZhaoHong Han, Teachers College, Columbia University, Box 66, 525 W. 120th Street. New York, NY10027. Email: han@tc.columbia.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Peer Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Li and Lan (Reference Li and Lan2021) admirably broached a timely topic in second language learning: the role of technological mediation in learning and instruction. In particular, the authors sought to make a case for Digital Language Learning as a domain of research and practice in its own right. Their professed goal is ambitious:

Here we outline the approach of digital language learning (DLL) for L2 acquisition and representation, and provide a theoretical synthesis and analytical framework regarding DLL's current and future promises.

The authors argue:

Theoretically, the DLL approach serves as the basis for understanding differences between child language and adult L2 learning, and the effects of learning context and learner characteristics. Practically, findings from learner behaviors, cognitive and affective processing, and brain correlates can inform DLL-based language pedagogies. Because of its highly interdisciplinary nature, DLL can serve as an approach to integrate cognitive, social, affective, and neural dimensions of L2 learning with new and emerging technologies including VR, AI, and big data analytics.

To what extent the authors accomplished their goal and succeeded in advancing their case theoretically and practically is best left for individual readers to find on their own; but here, I would like to take up four issues.

First is whether it is true that “our theories and practices for the learning and teaching of second languages (L2) have lagged behind the pace of scientific advances and technological innovations”.

For me, the reverse seems truer. The field of study, second language acquisition (SLA), has been around for almost five decades, the field of foreign language teaching for centuries. Both have long predated technology, in spite of the breathtaking pace of development of the latter. The fact is that our knowledge of second language learning and teaching stems much more from the academic study of second language acquisition and from pedagogical practice than from recent “scientific advances and technological innovations”. Indeed, of the multiple fields of study cited in the Li and Lan article, there is a glaring void – the near absence of SLA research, especially the literature on instructed second language acquisition (ISLA), a thriving and productive domain of study spanning more than three decades and yielding a wealth of relevant insights into learning and intervention (see, e.g., Han & Nassaji, Reference Han and Nassaji2019; Norris & Ortega, Reference Norris and Ortega2000; Loewen, Reference Loewen2014; Loewen & Sato, Reference Loewen and Sato2017). The point here is not to suggest that DLL cannot or should not constitute an independent domain of inquiry, but to suggest the need to avoid much reinventing of the wheel when there is a relevant, available body of research to tap into, build on, and expand.

Relatedly, a second issue concerns the authors’ frequent reference to “traditional teaching”, “traditional learning methods”, “traditional classroom-based, translation-based, teacher-centered L2 learning”, “traditional teacher-student interactions”, “traditional approaches”, “traditional L2 learning”, “traditional methods”, “traditional classrooms”, “traditional L2 instruction” and the like, pitting DLL against them, treating traditional practices monolithically, and allowing a rather dated understanding of second language instruction to pervade. Even a cursory inspection of the ISLA literature suffices to indicate that the field has, for the most part, long ceased practicing “traditional classroom-based, translation-based, teacher-centered L2 learning”. Therefore, arguing the strengths of DLL over traditional teaching is neither convincing nor compelling. A more productive way of exploring the potential of DLL would be to make connections with current pedagogical practices, which, for the most part, are functionally oriented. The structural-to-functional shift debuted in the late 1960s and has since undergone substantive revision and refinement, with task-supported and task-based language teaching representing the state-of-the art – informed by an SLA-based understanding of learning as a socio-cognitive constructive process and of the need for differentiated instruction, catering to individual differences.

Relatedly, third, depicting Zoom teaching categorically, as follows, is inaccurate and biased.

Today's pandemic-induced online learning mode (e.g., through Zoom or Microsoft Teams) often lacks joint attention, contingent response, and reciprocal interaction between the students and the instructor. Sustained attention to the subject matter is difficult to maintain in such a setting.

The fact is that Zoom teaching is not necessarily devoid of social interaction, nor without “contingent response, joint attention, or reciprocal interaction between the students and the instructor”. Like any technology-mediated language teaching and learning, Zoom is only a mediating tool, to be wielded by a mediator, the instructor and (to some extent) the learner. Compared to GBLL, MALL, VR, and so on, Zoom arguably excels in allowing direct ‘human intervention’, thereby enabling human-human interaction, albeit at a distance. When it comes to social interaction, a theme Li and Lan expounded in their article, there is no substitute for human-human connection precisely on levels the authors defended: the social, the cognitive, the affective, and the neural. These connections are, at best, limited, rigid, and incomplete in human-machine interaction.

Fourth, the authors’ argument, via the social L2 learning hypothesis (Li & Jeong, Reference Li and Jeong2020), is that L2 acquisition should replicate the conditions surrounding L1 acquisition, but this is only partly supported by SLA research – insofar as current understanding of L2 acquisition favors a usage-based approach, semantic bootstrapping, and incidental learning. However, as decades of SLA research have amply reasoned and documented, L2 acquisition is fundamentally different from L1 acquisition (see, e.g., Bley-Vroman, Reference Bley-Vroman, Gass and Schachter1989; Clahsen & Felser, Reference Clahsen and Felser2006; N. Ellis, Reference Ellis2006; R. Ellis, Reference Ellis1985; Hulstijn, Reference Hulstijn2015; Schachter, Reference Schachter, Brown, Malmkjaer and Williams1996; Selinker, Reference Selinker1972; Sorace, Reference Sorace, Cornips and Corrigan2005). Instruction should, therefore, be designed and implemented so as to mitigate gaps between L1 and L2 learners (see, e.g., N. Ellis, Reference Ellis, Robinson and Ellis2008; Long, Reference Long2007).

In closing, the potential of DLL, which Li and Lan (Reference Li and Lan2021) set out to explore, looms large and real. Its realization rests on intimate collaboration between technology designers and second language acquisition professionals. It is at that interface where creative, innovative, and robust tools for language learning shall emerge, thereafter to be validated by theoretical and empirical research from multi-disciplinary perspectives.

Acknowledgements

I thank the editor for the invitation to contribute this commentary. Any errors are exclusively my own.

References

Bley-Vroman, R (1989) What is the logical problem of foreign language acquisition? In Gass, S and Schachter, J (eds), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 4164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H and Felser, C (2006) Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics 27(1), 107126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, NC (2006) Selective Attention and Transfer Phenomena in L2 Acquisition: Contingency, Cue Competition, Salience, Interference, Overshadowing, Blocking, and Perceptual Learning. Applied Linguistics 27(2), 164194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, NC (2008) Usage-based and form-focused language acquisition: The associative learning of constructions, learned-attention, and the limited L2 endstate. In Robinson, P and Ellis, NC (eds), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition. London: Routledge, pp. 372405.Google Scholar
Ellis, R (1985) The L1=L2 Hypothesis: A reconsideration. System 13, 924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, Z-H and Nassaji, H (2019) Introduction: A snapshot of thirty-five years of instructed second language acquisition. Language Teaching Research 23(4), 383402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, JH (2015) Language proficiency in native and non-native speakers: Theory and research Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, P and Jeong, H (2020) The social brain of learning language: Grounding second language learning in social interaction. npj Science of Learning 19. doi:10.1038/s41539-020-0068-7Google ScholarPubMed
Li, P and Lan, Y-J (2021) Digital Language Learning (DLL): Insights from Behavior, Cognition, and the Brain. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewen, S (2014) Introduction to instructed second language acquisition. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewen, S and Sato, M (Eds.). (2017) The Routledge handbook of instructed second language acquisition. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, MH (2007) Problems in SLA. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Norris, J and Ortega, L (2000) Effectiveness of L2 Instruction: A Research Synthesis and Quantitative Meta-analysis. Language Learning 50(3), 417528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, J (1996) Learning and triggering in adult L2 acquisition. In Brown, G, Malmkjaer, K and Williams, J (eds), Performance and competence in SLA. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press pp. 7088.Google Scholar
Selinker, L (1972) Interlanguage. IRAL 10(2), 209231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A (2005) Selective optionality in language development. In Cornips, L and Corrigan, K (eds), Syntax and variation reconciling the biological and the social. Amsterdam: Benjamins, pp. 111160.Google Scholar