Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T04:13:37.384Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cross-linguistic priming in bilinguals: Multidisciplinary perspectives on language processing, acquisition, and change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 October 2016

GERRIT JAN KOOTSTRA*
Affiliation:
Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands
PIETER MUYSKEN
Affiliation:
Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands
*
Address for correspondence: Gerrit Jan Kootstra, Windesheim University of Applied Sciences, Division of Human Movement and Education, Campus 2–6, 8017 CA Zwolle, the NetherlandsG.Kootstra.work@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

This special issue deals with cross-linguistic priming in bilinguals, and consists of six contributions, which were all presented during the workshop on cross-linguistic priming in bilinguals at Radboud University Nijmegen (NL) in September 2013 (http://crosslingprimingconf2013.wordpress.com).

Type
Introduction
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

This special issue deals with cross-linguistic priming in bilinguals, and consists of six contributions, which were all presented during the workshop on cross-linguistic priming in bilinguals at Radboud University Nijmegen (NL) in September 2013 (http://crosslingprimingconf2013.wordpress.com).

Priming is the influence of recent experience with language on current processing of language. For example, when language users have just heard a sentence with a specific syntactic structure, they have a tendency to re-use that syntactic structure in subsequent utterances (i.e., structural priming; see e.g., Pickering & Ferreira, Reference Pickering and Ferreira2008, for a review; see the JML special issue on New Approaches to Structural Priming [Dell & Ferreira, 2016], for recent insights). Priming plays an important role in the psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic literature. Not only is it used in experiments to gain insight into the representational nature of language comprehension and production (Bock, Reference Bock1986, Reference Bock1996; Garrod, Reference Garrod and Brown2006; Pickering & Branigan, Reference Pickering and Branigan1999); it is also proposed as a factor that facilitates fluency of language production (Ferreira & Bock, Reference Ferreira and Bock2006; MacDonald, Reference MacDonald2013; Schober, Reference Schober and Brown2006), that contributes to implicit language learning and predictive language processing (Chang, Dell & Bock, Reference Chang, Dell and Bock2006; Dell & Chang, Reference Dell and Chang2014), and that guides linguistic choices in spontaneous language use between conversation partners in social interaction (e.g., Bresnan, Cueni, Nikitina & Baayen, Reference Bresnan, Cueni, Nikitina, Baayen, Bouma, Krämer and Zwarts2007; Pickering & Garrod, Reference Pickering and Garrod2004; Schenkein, Reference Schenkein and Butterworth1980; Weiner & Labov, Reference Weiner and Labov1983). Thus, priming is a multifaceted phenomenon firmly grounded in cognition and social interaction.

Priming also plays a central role in bilingual language use. Cross-linguistic priming, i.e., the influence of recent exposure to one language on language processing in another language, has been investigated in many laboratory studies on language processing in bilinguals (e.g., Basnight-Brown & Altarriba, Reference Basnight-Brown and Altarriba2007; Bernolet, Hartsuiker & Pickering, Reference Bernolet, Hartsuiker and Pickering2012; Jiang, Reference Jiang1999; Hartsuiker, Pickering & Veltkamp, Reference Hartsuiker, Pickering and Veltkamp2004; Loebell & Bock, Reference Loebell and Bock2003; Zhao & Li, Reference Zhao and Li2013). The underlying idea here is that if priming takes place across languages, this is evidence of cross-language activation. By investigating the processing levels at which cross-linguistic priming can take place, more information is gained on the different processing levels at which cross-language activation can take place, and thus on the interactivity of the bilingual processing system in general, a central theme in the psycholinguistic study of bilingualism. Thus, for example, findings of cross-language structural priming have indicated that cross-language activation can take place at the syntactic level of processing, which has informed the development of models on cross-language interactions in bilingual language use beyond the single-word level (see e.g., Hartsuiker & Pickering, Reference Hartsuiker and Pickering2008, for more information).

In addition to serving as a laboratory measure of cross-language activation, cross-linguistic priming is also a real-life process that can be related to many results of language contact, such as cross-linguistic transfer in second language acquisition (Flett, Branigan & Pickering, Reference Flett, Branigan and Pickering2013; Jackson & Ruf, Reference Jackson and Ruf2016; Nitschke, Kidd & Serratrice, Reference Nitschke, Kidd and Serratrice2010; Nitschke, Serratrice & Kidd, Reference Nitschke, Serratrice and Kidd2014), code-switching (Kootstra, van Hell & Dijkstra, Reference Kootstra, van Hell and Dijkstra2010; Reference Kootstra, van Hell and Dijkstra2012; Fricke & Kootstra, Reference Fricke and Kootstra2016), and contact-induced language change (Loebell & Bock, Reference Loebell and Bock2003; Torres Cacoullos & Travis, Reference Torres Cacoullos and Travis2011, Reference Torres Cacoullos and Travis2015). This not only provides ecological validity to experimental studies, but also indicates that cross-linguistic priming is a key mechanism of bilingual language use with important functions, just as is the case with priming in monolingual language use (Ferreira & Bock, Reference Ferreira and Bock2006). The study of cross-linguistic priming in both experiments and corpora can thus create a link between issues that are traditionally studied in quite different sub-disciplines of bilingualism research, and often on the basis of different methodological paradigms and beliefs of what counts as evidence (cf. Gullberg, Indefrey & Muysken, Reference Gullberg, Indefrey, Muysken, Bullock and Toribio2009; Li Wei, Reference Wei and Moyer2008; Moyer, Reference Moyer, Wei and Moyer2008).

The goal of this special issue is to create this link. It brings together research on the role of cross-linguistic priming with respect to the cognitive mechanisms of language processing and research on the role of cross-linguistic priming in language variation and change. It provides a multidisciplinary overview of the state of the art of cross-linguistic priming research, in terms of both theoretical issues and methodological approaches, and it presents novel findings on the role of cross-linguistic priming with respect to various aspects of bilingual language processing, acquisition, and change. This way, we intend to provide a broad and up-to-date impression of the central role and great potential of cross-linguistic priming in bilingualism research.

The issue starts with two review papers (Hartsuiker & Bernolet, Reference Hartsuiker and Bernolet2017; Gries & Kootstra, Reference Gries and Kootstra2017). Hartsuiker and Bernolet (Reference Hartsuiker and Bernolet2017) focus on the experimental literature on structural priming across languages, which has informed an influential model on shared syntactic representations in bilinguals (e.g., Hartsuiker, Pickering & Veltkamp, Reference Hartsuiker, Pickering and Veltkamp2004; Hartsuiker & Pickering, Reference Hartsuiker and Pickering2008). After a review of literature, Hartsuiker and Bernolet present a re-analysis of data from Schoonbaert, Hartsuiker, and Pickering (Reference Schoonbaert, Hartsuiker and Pickering2007) to address the question how these shared syntactic representations develop as a function of proficiency. In doing so, they link their model on shared syntactic representations with the idea of priming as a form of implicit learning, in this case syntactic learning. Hartsuiker and Bernolet's model can serve as an important and testable model for future studies on cross-linguistic priming and second language acquisition (see e.g., Jackson & Ruf, Reference Jackson and Ruf2016).

The second review paper is by Gries and Kootstra (Reference Gries and Kootstra2017), who focus on corpus-based investigations of structural priming in real life. Structural priming in spontaneous language use was long thought to be difficult if not impossible to investigate in an internally valid way, but recent statistical techniques have made it possible to control for the many confounding variables that may influence linguistic choices in real life. Gries and Kootstra provide a selective review of the way corpus-based approaches to structural priming have developed and the kind of questions investigated on the basis of corpora. They end with a discussion of studies on priming in bilingual corpora, showing the potential insights that can be gained from the relatively recent advance in the development of bilingual corpora big enough for the quantitative study of linguistic choices in bilinguals.

The other four papers in this issue present empirical findings on various aspects of priming in bilinguals. Fernández, de Souza, and Carando (Reference Fernández, de Souza and Carando2017) bring together two experimental studies on the processing and primed production of contact-induced innovative language structures in Portuguese–English and Spanish–English bilinguals. They show how bilinguals can become tolerant to innovative structures in their first language on the basis of experience with such structures in their second language (via priming). Based on these results, they develop a preliminary hypothesis that cross-linguistic priming may well serve as a mechanism of contact-induced language change.

Jacob, Katsika, Family, and Allen (Reference Jacob, Katsika, Family and Allen2017) present two experiments examining the representational nature of cross-linguistic structural priming. As specified in Hartsuiker et al.’s model (e.g., Reference Hartsuiker, Pickering and Veltkamp2004; see also Hartsuiker and Bernolet, Reference Hartsuiker and Bernolet2017), cross-linguistic structural priming is explained in terms of shared syntactic representations in the bilingual mind. The question is, however, what kind of syntactic ‘sharedness’ between languages is necessary for priming to take place? Based on a systematic experimental investigation in German–English bilinguals, Jacob et al. argue that not only constituent order but also syntactic level of embedding needs to be shared between sentences for priming to occur – a finding that is explained with reference to hierarchical tree structures. An interesting aspect brought up by Jacob et al. is that syntactic level of embedding does not seem to constrain structural priming in monolingual discourse. Thus, perhaps, structural priming across languages is more constrained than structural priming within languages.

This difference between priming within and across languages is one of the issues explored in the paper by Travis, Torres Cacoullos, and Kidd (Reference Travis, Torres Cacoullos and Kidd2017). They analyzed a corpus of natural speech from Spanish–English bilinguals in New Mexico, focusing on a dependent variable that is different from the syntactic alternations central in ‘traditional’ priming studies, namely pronominal subject expression, which is optional in Spanish but not in English. They found that priming of the overt expression of the pronominal subject in Spanish occurred both within and across languages, but was weaker and shorter-lived across languages than within languages. They further show that priming effects depend on verb-specific usage patterns and subject continuity of the conversation. Especially this last factor is difficult to investigate in an experimental setting, thus showing the added value of corpus-based investigations of priming. Travis et al. make a convincing case for combining usage-based perspectives on language variation with psycholinguistic modeling.

The final paper is by Carrol and Conklin (Reference Carrol and Conklin2017), who focus on cross-linguistic priming in the processing of idioms in Chinese–English bilinguals. Based on two eye-tracking experiments in which bilinguals read English sentences that contained Chinese idioms (translated in English), they find that idioms modulate reading times, and that figurative meanings of the idioms are processed slower than literal meanings. Thus, this study shows how formulaic units in one language, which can perhaps be related to usage-based constructions as addressed in Travis et al.’s (Reference Travis, Torres Cacoullos and Kidd2017) paper, can prime language processing strategies in the other language. These findings provide important insights into the workings of the bilingual mental lexicon with respect to multi-word units. The study can also be seen as an example of how priming (in this case, cross-linguistic priming) can lead to linguistic expectations that result in predictive language processing (cf. Dell & Chang, Reference Dell and Chang2014).

All in all, this special issue covers reviews and novel findings on many aspects of cross-linguistic priming, based on both experimental and corpus-based methodologies, on various linguistic structures, and with bilingual speakers of various languages. Cross-linguistic priming can be seen as a beautiful experimental measure of cross-linguistic interaction that, at the same time, shapes linguistic choices in real-life discourse. It provides a unique mirror into the dynamics of bilingual language use, revealing the connection between cognitive mechanisms, learning processes, and communicative forces in spontaneous communication in bilinguals. The study of cross-linguistic priming may thus help in establishing stronger links between research on bilingual language production and comprehension, research on second language acquisition, and research on (contact-induced) language variation and change. We hope this special issue contributes to further establishing these links.

References

Basnight-Brown, D. M., & Altarriba, J. (2007). Differences in semantic and translation priming across languages: The role of language direction and language dominance. Memory & Cognition, 35, 953965. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193468.Google Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2012). Effects of phonological feedback on the selection of syntax: Evidence from between-language syntactic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 503516. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728911000162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355387. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90004-6.Google Scholar
Bock, J. K. (1996). Language production: Methods and methodologies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 395421. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03214545.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J., Cueni, A., Nikitina, T., & Baayen, R. H. (2007). Predicting the dative alternation. In: Bouma, G., Krämer, I., & Zwarts, J. (eds.), Cognitive foundations of interpretation (pp. 6994). Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Science.Google Scholar
Carrol, G., & Conklin, K. (2017). Cross language lexical priming extends to formulaic units: Evidence from eye-tracking suggests that this idea “has legs”. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728915000103.Google Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, J. K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113, 234272. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.113.2.234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell, G. S., & Chang, F. (2014). The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B., 369, 20120394. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0394.Google Scholar
Fernández, E. M., de Souza, R. A., & Carando, A. (2017). Bilingual innovations: Experimental evidence offers clues regarding the psycholinguistics of language change. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI: not yet available.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S., & Bock, J. K. (2006). The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 10111029. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690960600824609.Google Scholar
Flett, S., Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2013). Are non-native structural preferences affected by native language preferences? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 751760. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728912000594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fricke, M., & Kootstra, G. J. (2016). Primed codeswitching in spontaneous bilingual dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 181201. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.04.003.Google Scholar
Garrod, S. (2006). Psycholinguistic research methods. In: Brown, K. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (pp. 251257). Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b0-08-044854-2/04155-9.Google Scholar
Gries, S. Th., & Kootstra, G. J. (2017). Structural priming within and across languages: a corpus-based perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916001085 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M., Indefrey, P., & Muysken, P. (2009). Research techniques for the study of code-switching. In: Bullock, B. E. & Toribio, A. J. (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Code-switching (pp. 2139). Cambridge University Press. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511576331.003.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Bernolet, S. (2017). The development of shared syntax in second language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728915000164.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2008). Language integration in bilingual sentence production. Acta Psychologica, 128, 479489. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.08.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15, 409414. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00693.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, C. N., & Ruf, H. T. (2016). The priming of word order in second language German. Applied Psycholinguistics, Firstview article. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716416000205.Google Scholar
Jacob, G., Katsika, K., Family, N., & Allen, S. E. M. (2017). The role of constituent order and level of embedding in cross-linguistic structural priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728916000717.Google Scholar
Jiang, N. (1999). Testing processing explanations for the asymmetry in masked cross-language priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 5975. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728999000152.Google Scholar
Kootstra, G. J., van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2010). Syntactic alignment and shared word order in code-switched sentence production: Evidence from bilingual monologue and dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 210231. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.03.006.Google Scholar
Kootstra, G. J., van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2012). Priming of code-switches in sentences: The role of lexical repetition, cognates, and language proficiency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 797819. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s136672891100068x.Google Scholar
Li Wei (2008). Research perspectives on bilingualism and multilingualism. In: Wei, Li & Moyer, M. G. (eds.), The Blackwell Guide to Research Methods in Bilingualism and Multilingualism (pp. 317). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781444301120.ch1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loebell, H., & Bock, J. K. (2003). Structural priming across languages. Linguistics, 41, 791824. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ling.2003.026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M. C. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, article 226. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226.Google Scholar
Moyer, M. G. (2008). Research as practice: Linking theory, method, and data. In: Wei, Li & Moyer, M. G. (eds.), The Blackwell Guide to Research Methods in Bilingualism and Multilingualism (pp. 1832). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781444301120.ch2.Google Scholar
Nitschke, S., Kidd, E., & Serratrice, L. (2010). First language transfer and long-term structural priming in comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 94114. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690960902872793.Google Scholar
Nitschke, S., Serratrice, L., & Kidd, E. (2014). The effect of linguistic nativeness on structural priming in comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 525542. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.766355.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1999). Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 136141. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01293-0.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 427459. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169190; discussion 190226. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x04000056.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenkein, J. (1980). A taxonomy of repeating action sequences in natural conversation. In: Butterworth, B. (Ed.), Language production, Vol. 1, pp. 2147. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schober, M. F. (2006). Dialogue and interaction. In: Brown, K. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (pp. 564571). Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b0-08-044854-2/00792-6.Google Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 153171. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.10.002.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, R. & Travis, C. E. (2011). Testing convergence via code-switching: Priming and the structure of variable subject expression. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 241267. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367006910371025.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, R., & Travis, C. E. (2015). Two languages, one effect: Structural priming in spontaneous code-switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 733753. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728914000406.Google Scholar
Travis, C. E., Torres Cacoullos, R., & Kidd, E. (2017). Cross-language priming: A view from bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728915000127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, E. J., & Labov, W. (1983). Constraints on the agentless passive. Journal of Linguistics, 19, 2958. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226700007441.Google Scholar
Zhao, X., & Li, P. (2013). Simulating cross-language priming with a dynamic computational model of the lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 288303. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728912000624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar