Introduction
Motivational interviewing (MI), first described by Miller (Reference Miller1983), is “a collaborative, person-centred form of guiding to elicit and strengthen motivation for change” (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2009). Though originating in substance abuse treatment, its efficacy in a range of health behaviour domains is demonstrated by a number of systematic reviews (e.g. Resnicow, Davis and Rollnick, Reference Resnicow, Davis and Rollnick2006; Rubak, Sandboek, Lauritzen and Christensen, Reference Rubak, Sandboek, Lauritzen and Christensen2005; Van Dorsten, Reference Van Dorsten2007; VanWormer and Boucher, Reference Van Wormer and Boucher2004). MI is increasingly being applied to physical activity promotion and of nine trials identified by a recent systematic review (Martins and McNeil, Reference Martins and McNeil2009), five reported significant short-term effects upon physical activity. However, only one reported 12-month outcomes, finding no sustained impacts (Harland et al., Reference Harland, White, Drinkwater, Chinn, Farr and Howel1999). Hence, whilst short-term findings vary and long-term efficacy remains to be demonstrated, emerging evidence suggests that MI may in some circumstances offer utility in promoting physical activity.
In furthering this evidence base, an issue deserving careful consideration is whether interventions claiming to deliver MI actually do (Hecht et al., Reference Hecht, Borrelli, Breger, DeFrancesco, Ernst and Resnicow2005). Miller and Rollnick (Reference Miller and Rollnick2009) comment that though diffusing rapidly into many domains, MI is often confused with other approaches, and delivered in a manner that violates its core spirit (Rollnick and Miller, Reference Rollnick and Miller1995). Recent years have therefore seen growing attention to the development and application of scales to assess MI fidelity. The Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity (MITI) scale, for example, provides a tool for coding adherence to the spirit of MI and a range of core MI consistent techniques (Moyers, Martin, Manuel, Hendrickson and Miller, Reference Moyers, Martin, Manuel, Hendrickson and Miller2005; Pierson et al., Reference Pierson, Hayes, Gifford, Roget, Padilla and Bissett2007), facilitating scrutiny of how impacts differ by various dimensions of practitioner competence (e.g. Thyrian et al., Reference Thyrian, Freyer-Adam, Hannover, Roske and Mentzel2007). The Behaviour Change Counselling Index (BECCI) provides a single fidelity score (Lane et al., Reference Lane, Huws-Thomas, Hood, Rollnick, Edwards and Robling2005), offering a simple tool for providing feedback to practitioners.
Early trials of MI to promote physical activity (Brodie and Inoue, Reference Brodie and Inoue2005; Harland et al., Reference Harland, White, Drinkwater, Chinn, Farr and Howel1999; Hillsdon, Thorogood, White and Foster, Reference Hillsdon, Thorogood, White and Foster2002) mostly preceded the widespread dissemination of these measures, and hence reported no fidelity assessments. Whilst more recent trials describe quality control approaches such as reviews with implementers to discuss consultations (Befort et al., Reference Befort, Nollen, Ellerbeck, Sullivan, Thomas and Ahluwalia2008; Bennett, Lyons, Winters-Stone, Nail and Sherer, Reference Bennett, Moore, Vaughan, Rouse, Gibbins and Thomas2007; Hardcastle, Taylor Bailey and Castle, Reference Hardcastle, Taylor, Bailey and Castle2008), the authors are aware of only three that quantify fidelity (Bennett, Young, Nail, Winters-Stone and Hanson, Reference Bennett, Young, Nail, Winters-Stone and Hanson2008; Carels et al., Reference Carels, Darby, Cacciapaglia, Konrad, Coit and Harper2007; Greaves et al., Reference Greaves, Middlebrooke, O'Loughlin, Holland, Piper and Steele2008). Given that these trials mostly involved small numbers of self-selecting practitioners, delivering MI to establish its efficacy, greater control over delivery was likely achieved than might be the case in real-world interventions. However, it remains plausible that variable efficacy may at least in part reflect variable implementation and including fidelity assessment in such trials is good practice. Given that real-world interventions often involve delivery by multiple practitioners with limited counselling experience, close attention to fidelity perhaps becomes increasingly critical within pragmatic evaluations of their effectiveness.
In addition to growing attempts to examine whether MI is implemented, attention is also turning to the need to understand why. Miller and Rollnick (Reference Miller and Rollnick2009) describe a number of key barriers to implementation including the common misperception that MI is easy to learn. The authors describe requests to teach MI in 1–2 hour workshops as common, but as comparable to attempting to teach the viola over lunch. Two-day MI courses are increasingly common, although evidence is building that these too are insufficient to facilitate proficient use without ongoing support (Wahab, Menon and Szalacha, Reference Wahab, Menon and Szalacha2008). One study, for example, found that although a 2-day course convinced clinicians they had learned MI, practice samples showed insufficient change to benefit patients (Miller and Mount, Reference Miller and Mount2001). Hence, research is increasingly attempting to identify the support needed to ensure that skills can be practised (Brug et al., Reference Brug, Spikmans, Aartsen, Breedveld, Bes and Fereira2007; Rubak, Sandbaek, Lauritzen, Borch-Johnsen and Christensen, Reference Rubak, Sandbaek, Lauritzen, Borch-Johnsen and Christensen2009; Forsberg, Forsberg, Lindqvist and Helgason, Reference Forsberg, Forsberg, Lindqvist and Helgason2010).
Whilst training commonly focuses upon providing knowledge, Madson, Loignon and Lane (Reference Madson, Loignon and Lane2009) highlight a need to also consider issues such as self-efficacy, intention to use MI, attitudes and circumstances. Understanding practitioners’ reactions to introductions to MI, and experiences of attempting to apply it to practice, may provide valuable insights into why implementation is more or less successful. To date, qualitative methods have perhaps been underused in understanding these issues. Whilst some studies report positive reactions amongst MI trainees via quantitative satisfaction measures (Broers et al., Reference Broers, Smets, Bindels, Evertsz, Calff and de Haes2005; Lane, Johnson, Rollnick, Edwards and Lyons, Reference Lane, Johnson, Rollnick, Edwards and Lyons2003), in one qualitative study, healthcare providers saw MI as taking too long, whilst the passive role of the MI practitioner conflicted with perceived roles as information providers (Miller and Beech, Reference Miller and Beech2009).
Data for this study are derived from a process evaluation within the pragmatic randomized controlled trial of the National Exercise Referral Scheme (NERS) in Wales, (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Raisanen, Moore, Edwards, Linck and Williams2010). In addition to a 16-week exercise programme, NERS professionals delivered consultations on scheme entry and exit, with protocols stating that initial consultations would include motivational interviewing, alongside health assessments and goal setting. The scheme therefore offered a useful context in which to study a real-world attempt to integrate MI into the daily practice of exercise professionals, most of whom had not encountered it previously.
Early in the evaluation concerns regarding fidelity were raised by evaluators, given that MI had been taught via an initial 1-hour session. In response, the Welsh Assembly Government introduced 2-day courses. One-to-one consultations recorded before these courses were coded using BECCI, confirming inconsistency with MI. Whilst for the reasons described it may be naïve to hypothesize that MI would be delivered with high fidelity following 2-day training, this study combines qualitative interviews with area coordinators, exercise professionals and an MI training provider with longitudinal assessment of MI fidelity to examine: i) implementers’ views on the acceptability and feasibility of using MI in practice following training; ii) whether practices began to move towards an MI consistent approach following training; and iii) the circumstances under which practices began to change.
Method
Research design
The study combines cross-sectional qualitative post-training interviews with longitudinal quantitative monitoring of MI delivery. An overview of the research design is presented in Figure 1. The study received ethical approval from an NHS research ethics committee.
Sampling and participants
Eligible participants for the qualitative aspect were 31 professionals attending one of three training courses between August 2008 and January 2009, of whom 27 took part. Professionals were fitness instructors who had undergone a level 3 exercise referral training course and were registered on the Register of Exercise Professionals (REPs). Additional interviews were conducted with all 10 area coordinators whose professionals attended this training, all of whom took part, and the MI training provider.
As ethical approval for consultation recordings was received after the August course, quantitative monitoring of consultation delivery focused upon professionals attending the October course (n = 23), of whom 14 provided baseline data, and 11 provided data at follow-up. Sampling details and flow of participants through the study are presented in Figure 1.
Training
All professionals attended a 2-day (approximately 13 hours) MI course combining brief presentations with demonstrations, video presentations, discussion, self-assessment, supervised practice, coaching and “real play” exercises. Professionals were asked to think of a behaviour they were ambivalent about, and given exercises in which they alternated between acting as patient or practitioner in order to gain experience of using MI strategies, whilst experiencing an insight from the patients’ perspective about contrasts between MI consistent and non-consistent behaviours.
Qualitative interviews: measures, data collection procedures and analysis
Data collection. All professionals and coordinators were invited to participate by e-mail. After agreeing to take part, a time and date was arranged for a telephone interview (preferred to face-to-face interviews due to the geographical coverage of the national scheme) lasting up to an hour. Following e-mail contact from the evaluation team, the MI trainer offered to share his views having provided the training, and an interview was scheduled. Interviews were conducted by the lead author, who had previous contact with many study participants in meetings and earlier data collection phases. Interviews were recorded with the consent of the informant and transcribed verbatim.
Semi-structured interview schedules. Interviews were guided by schedules of open questions and topics. These were neither prescriptive nor-exhaustive, allowing the interviewer to explore issues of interest, whilst allowing interviewees to discuss issues not anticipated by the interviewer. Coordinator interviews, conducted as part of the wider evaluation, predominantly explored views on barriers and facilitators to delivering NERS and integration into local settings. Whilst not explicitly requesting views on MI, many coordinators volunteered opinions on training, speaking of its potential role within NERS. Exercise professional interviews were also conducted as part of the wider evaluation, exploring professionals’ perceptions of their role within the programme, the scheme and barriers and facilitators of its implementation, scheme impacts on patient behaviour and mechanisms of change. For professionals attending the MI course, interviews also elicited perceptions of the purposes of one to one consultations, the extent to which professionals felt they used MI principles in their practice, and views on the adequacy of training provided. As the interview with the training provider was scheduled at the request of the interviewee, a loose structure was developed, exploring views on delivering training, professionals’ responses to it, expectations for its impact, and perceived feasibility issues in integrating MI into NERS.
Analysis. A predominantly inductive thematic approach to analysis was adopted (Braun and Clarke, Reference Braun and Clarke2006), framed by a critical realist conceptualization of causality (Kazi, Reference Kazi2003), which focuses on how responses to and impacts of interventions are shaped by the perceptions and contextual circumstances of recipients, rather than simply whether interventions “work”. Data were transcribed externally and the lead author checked transcription accuracy. Generation of codes began during accuracy checks, with each transcript subsequently coded using QSR Nvivo. Codes were applied to subsequent transcripts, with earlier transcripts revisited as new codes were generated. Themes were then identified and reviewed through rereading coded material and the full dataset. Qualitative analyses were completed prior to quantitative assessment of training impacts in order to minimize interpretation biases.
Coordinator data are described under a single code “Coordinator views on MI and its role within NERS”, with verbatim quotes illustrating a range of views. Views of the MI training provider are then described under the heading “The MI training provider's views on the potential impacts of training on MI delivery in NERS”. Due to difficulties preserving the anonymity of the training provider, the account of the interview as presented in this paper was e-mailed to the interviewee prior to inclusion, and approved as an accurate representation without changes. Key themes from exercise professional interviews related to: i) awareness of MI and the purpose of the first consultation before and after training; ii) perceived usefulness of training and impact on practice; and iii) implementation issues. Data from exercise professionals are described under each heading, with selected verbatim quotes illustrating a range of views.
Quantitative study component: measures, data collection procedures and analysis
Data collection procedures. GM supplied area coordinators with recording equipment and asked that professionals be requested to each record up to three consultations, and returned to collect recordings and accompanying consent forms at a time agreed with the coordinator. Procedures were repeated at 6-month follow-up.
MI fidelity. The first two useable recordings from each professional at each time point were coded by two independent coders using BECCI (Lane et al., Reference Lane, Huws-Thomas, Hood, Rollnick, Edwards and Robling2005). Whilst MITI is considered the gold standard measure of MI fidelity (Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Martin, Manuel, Hendrickson and Miller2005; Pierson et al., Reference Pierson, Hayes, Gifford, Roget, Padilla and Bissett2007), this requires substantial training, whereas training guidelines for BECCI were more achievable given the resources available for the study. Coders were psychology graduates who had previously attended a 2-day MI course. Instructions for training (Lane, Reference Lane2005) were followed to prepare coders to use the measure. The measure demonstrated excellent inter-rater reliability (r = 0.94 pre training; r = 0.96 post training). The mean score of coder 1 and coder 2 was used as the measure of MI fidelity. The final item asked coders to estimate whether professionals speak for more than half, about half, or less than half of the consultation time.
Statistical analysis. Means, standard deviations and medians for pre and post training fidelity were calculated and converted into percentages. As four professionals at baseline and two at follow-up provided only one recording, the first recording was used as the score for MI fidelity rather than an average of two or more scores. Spearman's rank correlation coefficients comparing first and second recordings for the seven professionals at baseline and the nine at follow-up who provided more than one recording indicated that MI adherence was typically stable between recordings (r = 0.71 and r = 0.87). Significance of post-training changes was assessed using a Wilcoxon Sign test. Mean and standard deviation consultation lengths and frequencies for remaining consultation characteristics were calculated.
Mixed method component
Ten professionals provided quantitative before and after measures and qualitative data. Data sources were linked using a form of single-case design (Kazi, Reference Kazi2003), whereby quantitative change over time is tracked at the individual level and compared against qualitative responses to develop theories regarding why change may or may not have occurred. Qualitative data for each individual participant were coded into substrata of the thematic categories above and tabulated against change in practice. Participants were grouped into clusters by area and responses to training compared against those of their area coordinator.
Results
Coordinators' views on MI and its role within NERS
A selection of verbatim quotes from coordinator interviews are presented in Table 1. Whilst three coordinators whose professionals received training in MI made no reference to its role when discussing how NERS affected behavioural change, and one discussed MI only when discussing views of training within the scheme, others commented that integration of MI offered substantial promise for NERS in their area. One, for example, commented that professionals had gained significant confidence dealing with patients with complex conditions through adopting MI principles. There was, however, substantial variability in perceptions of what MI was, and the roles that it might play in improving practice. Some saw its adoption as highly valuable, though necessitating a radical rethink of consultation structures, introducing principles that conflicted with prior practice. However, others saw MI as largely what instructors did anyway once they had been in the role for sufficient time, with training largely confirming the correctness of current practice, or as relevant only to professionals who were relatively poor communicators. MI was sometimes seen as a tool to enable professionals to extract information, or conflated with goal setting.
The MI training provider's views on potential impacts of training on MI delivery in NERS
The trainer commented that the course was well received by professionals, and that most appeared to see the value of change. However, the trainer also argued that the 2-day course “gives you intellectual insight but doesn't necessarily give you emotional insight such that you can easily change your practice”. Real change was seen as requiring structures for self-reflection and further expert guidance, with the training provider arguing that “like with any new skill, you need to rehearse it, you need to get guidance, you need to get coaching and your confidence goes up then”. In addition to a need for ongoing training to support integration, the training provider identified barriers to implementation relating to structures of NERS consultation, and professionals’ perceptions of their role within these consultations, remarking that when asking professionals what they hoped to gain from the course, professionals consistently commented that “their job was collecting information. They were very clear that that was what their job was. . .they were very clear that what they hoped that we would teach them was how to be more efficient in doing that”. This perception of the primacy of data collection was linked to a “genuine tension between the desire to collect data and what actually would help the person sat in front of you”. The tendency to prioritize data collection over MI was seen as needing to be reversed, with the training provider arguing that if MI is to be integrated, professionals “have to think that this (MI) is the most important thing to do. You can collect the data at the end if the time is available”. The training provider commented that once pressures of data collection were adhered to, time allowed for consultations lay at the outer margins of the minimum required to incorporate MI and that it “sets the wrong tempo for MI if you start off by saying, right I'm going to fill these forms in and ask you lots of questions”.
Professionals’ views on the introduction to motivational interviewing
Exercise professionals talk regarding the introduction to MI fell into three broad themes: awareness of MI and the purpose of the first consultation before and after training; perceived usefulness of training and impact on practice; and implementation issues. Each is described with selected quotes presented in Table 2.
Awareness of MI and the purpose of the first consultation before and after training
Whilst typically recalling having touched on MI briefly in initial training, or sometimes not remembering having covered it at all, professionals commented that they had limited experience of MI prior to the 2-day course. In addition to increasing understandings of MI, some commented that training had made them aware of a disjuncture between how they had seen the purposes of the initial consultation and how its purposes had been conceived in the design stage by national implementers. Whilst professionals often commented on the value of the first consultation in allowing them to build rapport and put patients at ease, its primary purpose had sometimes been seen as one of form filling and information gathering. Some argued that introduction to MI had helped them move away from this view, towards seeing the consultation as an opportunity to elicit talk about change. Rejection of MI was however sometimes linked to maintenance of the view of consultations as being primarily for gathering information, and a perception that MI did not assist this task.
Perceived usefulness of training and impacts on practice
Most saw value in MI and spoke of a willingness to adopt MI. Some described how they had changed practices since training, often offering critical reflection on perceived shortcomings of previous practice. In some instances for example, professionals identified prior tendencies to ask closed questions and tell patients what they should do, which they felt had begun to alter after training. Through these changes, many commented that rapport and the amount of change talk had greatly improved. A minority however were unconvinced of the value of changing their practices, typically commenting either that current practice worked, or that as clients were already motivated to change, MI was unnecessary.
Implementation issues
Whilst some felt that the duration of training was adequate to facilitate practice, many commented that MI had yet to become normalized into practice, and that they would require time and practice to feel confident in using the skills. Hence, as with the MI training provider, the 2-day course was seen by some as the start of the road to implementing MI, but by no means as the end of it. In addition, some identified the tension between data collection and MI described by the training provider, commenting that structures of the initial consultation and the volume of data collection made integration of MI challenging. One professional, for example, expressed a desire for further clarification on how the two tasks could be integrated, whilst another commented that consultation structures would need to be radically rethought to integrate MI, with implicit value judgments and closed questions involved in data collection components clashing with its patient-centred spirit.
Quantitative monitoring of MI delivery before and after training
MI fidelity before and after training. As indicated in Table 3, mean MI fidelity for professionals providing both baseline and follow-up recordings increased slightly after training, although medians were lower at both time points, indicating an undue influence of small numbers of relatively high scores. A Wilcoxon Sign Test indicated that overall improvement in MI fidelity was not significant (Z = .80, p = .42). At both time-points, according to both coders, most professionals spoke more than half the time during consultation, whilst pre and post training consultations were of almost identical length, averaging approximately 35 minutes.
Explaining change in individual practitioners. Although there was little change at the aggregate level, at the individual level, seven professionals scored higher for MI fidelity after training, whilst four scored marginally lower. Whilst most changes were small, three demonstrated clear improvements, with fidelity increasing from below 10% at baseline to between 30 and 40% at follow-up. These professionals, and another who scored 4th highest at follow-up, all opened consultations with a 10–25 minute unstructured discussion of behaviour change, before completing measures. Two others opened by asking the patient to describe a typical day, though moved on to structured data collections after about 5 minutes. In the remaining five, as typical of baseline consultations, talk about behaviour change occurred only during completion of structured questionnaires, with consultations placing substantial emphasis on didactic advice-giving.
Table 4 presents a matrix of sub-themes from interviews with each of the 10 professionals who provided quantitative longitudinal data and took part in a semi-structured interview. Notably, only in areas where the coordinator identified MI as a potentially valuable improvement over current practice did professionals consistently offer critical reflection on their own previous practice. Although two professionals in these areas scored only slightly higher after training, and another regressed to a lower level than at baseline, one in each area demonstrated improvements of 20% or more, with the five highest post-training fidelity scores from these three regions. In two areas where the coordinator did not mention MI when discussing how NERS helped patients become more active, negative views on the usefulness of training were expressed by professionals, with concerns regarding feasibility of integrating MI into current structures expressed in one. In both areas, no professional demonstrated change in practice, with three of the four lowest post-training fidelity scores coming from these two regions. In the remaining area, where the coordinator commented that training provided some useful additional techniques, but mostly reinforced existing practice, the sole professional providing recordings spoke positively of training, yet did not offer critical reflection on previous practice, with fidelity regressing to a lower level than at baseline. However, quantitative data were only obtained from one professional in this area, with the remaining professionals offering greater critical reflection.
* - no improvement, + 0–10% improvement, ++ 10–20% improvement, +++ 20–30% improvement, ++++ >30% improvement
** low stability >10% discrepancy between recording one and two, moderate 5–10% discrepancy between recording one and two, high <5% discrepancy between recording one and two, n/a only one recording provided
Whilst overall stability between first and second recordings was high (r = 0.87), the only professionals whose practices demonstrated low stability were two of the three practitioners who performed highest at follow-up (the third provided only a single recording). Both, whilst maintaining higher than average MI fidelity in both consultations, scored more than 10% lower for second recordings than they did for first recordings. Hence, although demonstrating higher proficiency than at baseline, improvements appeared unstable across consultations.
Discussion
Limited initial MI training for NERS professionals appeared to contribute to both limited understandings of MI and tendencies for consultations to be seen primarily as data collection exercises. Following additional training, some described MI as having little to offer their practice, for reasons including maintenance of a view that data collection was the primary role of consultations or that current practice was effective. However, most described a willingness to adopt MI, sometimes commenting that perceptions of the role of the consultation had begun to change after further training. For many, increased understanding of the value of MI, and perceived impact of initial attempts to use MI on rapport with patients appeared to trigger substantial critical reflections on previous practices such as unsolicited advice-giving. However, consistent with growing evidence that putting MI into practice requires skills development beyond a 2-day course (Forsberg et al., Reference Forsberg, Forsberg, Lindqvist and Helgason2010). professionals and the training provider described a need for practice or further training, whilst further concerns were raised about the compatibility of consultations with MI. These averaged 35 minutes, including a substantial data gathering element that reduced the time available for MI, with structured questioning and implicit value judgments within lifestyle questionnaires seen by some as incompatible with MI.
Hence, despite largely positive perceptions of MI following the course, 6-months after training no significant change in MI fidelity was observed. Fidelity scores at both time-points were comparable to recent assessments of GPs’ routine practice using the same scale (Moran, Bekker and Latchford, Reference Moran, Bekker and Latchford2008). However, several individuals did demonstrate movement towards an MI consistent approach. Whilst time allowed for change talk was still very short, these professionals prioritized discussion of behavioural change over data gathering, allowing time for this prior to completing measures. However, most still emphasized form-filling, dedicating little or no time to unstructured change talk. Improvements occurred only in areas headed by coordinators who commented on the potential value of integrating MI into practice after training and where all professionals expressed positive views on training, consistent with a view that transferring training into practice is easier where colleagues are supportive of integration (Baldwin and Ford, Reference Baldwin and Ford1988). However, practice remained variable between professionals within areas where support appeared unanimous.
Strengths of this study include use of a validated MI fidelity assessment tool (Lane et al., Reference Lane, Huws-Thomas, Hood, Rollnick, Edwards and Robling2005) and integration of qualitative data into before and after fidelity assessment, offering explanations for training impacts. However, weaknesses include that recording consultations may have led to Hawthorne effects (Campbell, Maxey and Watson, Reference Campbell, Maxey and Watson1995). Whilst several recordings per professional would ideally have been obtained, data collection was restricted by the limited time between receipt of ethical approval and implementation of training. The small sample limited power, although statistical significance was not of primary importance to this study. Responses to qualitative interviews were perhaps influenced by the interviewer's links to the wider evaluation, although nesting discussion of MI training into wider discussions of implementation probably made the aims of this particular sub-study less explicit. Although the training provider confirmed the accuracy of the account of their interview, it was not possible to circulate analyses to coordinators and professionals. Convergence between views of the training provider and those of professionals, analyzed prior to the training provider interview, adds credibility to the accounts. Finally, face-to-face interaction may have led to understandings missed by using the telephone, although the anonymity of telephone interviews may have elicited more open responses.
Nevertheless, the study has important implications for programmes aiming to deliver MI. Such programmes should consult with experts in MI on baseline training requirements, ensuring that implementers have adequate skills and understanding of MI prior to commencing practice, and allowing communication of primary functions of consultations throughout the system. Facilitating delivery by professionals with no counselling background and who are perhaps accustomed to more didactic approaches to promoting change possibly requires greater investment than when MI is delivered by self-selected practitioners with counselling experience. Implementation was perhaps made difficult within NERS by the fact that professionals were experienced in schemes not using MI, and had a year working within NERS before the expectation of MI delivery was reinforced through in-depth training. Asking professionals to adopt MI will require them to accept it as an improvement on current practice (Spillane, Reiser and Reimer, Reference Spillane, Reiser and Reimer2002); a position most, though not all, appeared to accept. Miller and Rose (Reference Miller and Rose2009) have compared the process of guiding practitioners towards changing their practice to the process of guiding patients in changing their behaviour. Just as instructing a patient to change may undermine autonomy and self-esteem, causing defence of current behaviour (Brehm, Reference Brehm1966), practitioners may interpret instruction to change as criticism and a threat to professional autonomy, leading to defence of current practice (Spillane et al., Reference Spillane, Reiser and Reimer2002), with MI unlikely to be adopted successfully by the minority of professionals who saw little value in it. Hence, as well as providing training at the earliest opportunity, willingness to adopt MI should be ascertained during staff recruitment, with MI forming part of professionals’ job description.
Furthermore, additional support is needed to translate widespread willingness to adopt MI into ability to practice (Madson et al., Reference Madson, Loignon and Lane2009). Progress towards an MI consistent approach may require integration of mechanisms for critical self-reflection, monitoring and feedback (Bennett, Moore, et al., Reference Bennett, Lyons, Winters-Stone, Nail and Scherer2007; Miller, Yahne, Moyers, Martinez and Pirritano, Reference Miller, Yahne, Moyers, Martinez and Pirritano2004; Mitcheson, Bhavsar and McCambridge, Reference Mitcheson, Bhavsar and McCambridge2009; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Amrhein, Brooks, Carpenter, Levin and Schreiber2007) both to provide support to those recognizing a need for it, and to prevent practitioners from prematurely believing they have become competent (Miller and Mount, Reference Miller and Mount2001). Where coordinators are new to MI, monitoring and feedback structures should be contracted to external parties with expertise in MI. As demonstrated by this paper, use of mixed-method evaluation designs that quantitatively monitor the effectiveness of efforts to implement MI, whilst qualitatively exploring barriers and facilitators to putting MI into practice, may be substantially more useful in informing practice than mono-method approaches.
Finally, consultation structures must be considered carefully. First, consultations must allow sufficient time for MI. Second, MI will often not be the sole purpose of the consultation, and consideration needs to be given to whether implementers are being asked to combine MI with activities with which it is incompatible. Structured assessments pose challenges in that they risk rendering the patient passive, contrary to the patient-centred spirit of MI. If assessments need to be conducted alongside MI, the manner in which these tasks are to be integrated merits careful consideration (Rollnick, Miller and Butler, Reference Rollnick, Miller and Butler2008).
Acknowledgements
GM led the process evaluation design, collected data, conducted analyses and drafted the paper. SM project managed the evaluation of which the process evaluation was a part, and advised on process evaluation design. All authors commented on drafts and approved the final manuscript. The authors would like to thank the NERS evaluation team, the scheme's coordinators Elaine McNish and Jeannie Wyatt-Williams, as well as Janine Hale and Chris Roberts within the Public Health Research Branch of the Welsh Assembly Government for facilitating and supporting the evaluation study. In addition, we would like to thank Professor Stephen Rollnick for advice and comments on drafts of the manuscript, Dr Claire Lane for providing the BECCI scale, advising on its use and providing training materials to prepare coders for its use and for advice and comments on drafts of the manuscript, and Eleri Sian Jones for assistance with coding of MI recordings. We would also like to thank the Welsh Assembly Government for funding the evaluation and all exercise coordinators and professionals and the motivational interviewing training provider who took part in the study. The lead author was supported by a studentship from the Economic and Social Research Council [PTA-031-2006-00086].
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.