Background
Cognitive models of anxiety propose that the tendency to interpret ambiguous information as threatening plays a causal role in the aetiology and maintenance of pathologic anxiety (Beck and Clark, Reference Beck and Clark1988). There is overwhelming evidence that such biased interpretations and anxiety are associated, yet the causal direction remains unclear. Mathews and Mackintosh (Reference Mathews and Mackintosh2000) examined this issue of causality by inducing a negative or positive interpretive bias and measuring the effects on anxiety. Interpretive bias was modified by having participants read ambiguous stories that ended with a word fragment requiring a completion. Fragment completion resolved the ambiguity resulting in either a positive or negative meaning of the story. Results showed that the modification of interpretive bias successfully resulted in a concomitant change of anxiety. Participants in the negative condition became more anxious while anxiety dropped in the positive condition. This Cognitive Bias Modification of Interpretations (CBM-I) method fuelled many new experiments and the original findings have been replicated several times (e.g. Mackintosh, Mathews, Yiend, Ridgeway and Cook, Reference Mackintosh, Mathews, Yiend, Ridgeway and Cook2006; Salemink, van den Hout and Kindt, Reference Salemink, van den Hout and Kindt2007b; Yiend, Mackintosh and Mathews, Reference Yiend, Mackintosh and Mathews2005).
The conclusions that have been drawn about causality are based on the crucial assumption that the CBM-I procedure affects interpretive bias, and that the changed interpretive bias then affects anxiety. In other words, the assumption is that there is an indirect relationship between the CBM-I procedure and anxiety, mediated by an altered interpretive bias. As far as we know, this critical assumption has never been tested. Note, however, that this hypothetical cascade of CBM-I→ interpretative bias→ changed anxiety represents only one of several possible interpretations of the observed data. For instance, the CBM-I procedure could have a direct effect on anxiety by changing mood through abundant exposure to either positive or negative information. This direct effect could be an additional effect, added to the indirect one; or it could fully explain the effects on anxiety. As CBM-I effects on anxiety have been taken as evidence for a causal relationship between interpretive bias and anxiety, it is crucial to know how CBM-I affects anxiety and whether the effects on anxiety are mediated by changed interpretations. In the present paper this is examined in two studies.
Study 1
To get a first impression of the possible relationships between CBM-I and anxiety, we re-analyzed data from an earlier study (Salemink et al., Reference Salemink, van den Hout and Kindt2007b) using a mediation path analysis. There are three possibilities: 1) CBM-I affects the interpretive bias, which in turn affects anxiety. Changes in anxiety are then caused by altered interpretations and not by the modification procedure itself; 2) There is both a direct and an indirect effect of CBM-I on anxiety; 3) CBM-I directly affects anxiety. That is, the modification procedure directly causes changes in anxiety due to mood induction, with no mediating role for interpretive bias. To investigate whether CBM-I affects current anxious state in different ways than more stable tendencies to feel anxious, both state and trait anxiety were measured.
Method
CBM-I data by Salemink et al. (Reference Salemink, van den Hout and Kindt2007b) were re-analyzed. Eighty-one unselected students participated for course credit (77 female/4 male). Their mean age was 21.1 years (SD = 2.8).
Materials and results
To modify interpretive bias, participants read ambiguous social stories, which ended with a word fragment (Mathews and Mackintosh, Reference Mathews and Mackintosh2000). Solution of the fragment resolved the ambiguity in a positive or negative way, depending on the assigned condition (n = 40 in the positive and n = 41 in the negative condition). During CBM-I, “probe word fragments” were presented that were similar to the modification stories, but ended in a fragmented word with fixed positive and negative valence, irrespective of modification condition. Time taken to solve these probe fragments was used as a reaction time measure for interpretive bias, and results showed that compared to the negatively trained participants (M pos = 1347ms, SD = 434; M neg = 1318ms, SD = 491, t(40) = 0.43, ns), positively trained participants were faster in solving positive continuations of the story than negative continuations (M pos = 1326ms, SD = 453; M neg = 1513ms, SD = 467, t(39) = −4.62, p < .001).
After CBM-I, participants read a new set of 10 social stories that remained ambiguous (recognition task). Afterwards, four interpretations of each story were presented; (a) possible positive and (b) possible negative interpretation, and (c) positive and (d) negative foil sentence. Participants rated each interpretation for its similarity in meaning to the original story (1 = very different in meaning and 4 = very similar in meaning). Results showed that positively trained participants interpreted the new ambiguous information more positively compared to negatively trained participants (M pos = 3.25, SD = 0.25; M neg = 2.83, SD = 0.45, t(79) = −5.14, p < .001). In contrast to the standard manipulation checks, no effects were observed on two other interpretive bias tasks, suggesting limited generalizability of the trained interpretive bias.
Change in anxiety was measured with the state and trait versions of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs, Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs1983). Participants in the negative condition became more state anxious (M before = 33.1, SD = 4.9; M after = 35.7, SD = 7.6, t(40) = −2.57, p < .05), while positively trained participants got less anxious (M before = 35.9, SD = 9.0; M after = 33.0, SD = 6.7, t(39) = 3.28, p < .01). Regarding trait anxiety, positively trained participants became less trait anxious (M before = 34.9, SD = 6.6; M after = 33.3, SD = 6.6, t(39) = 3.39, p < .01), while the change in the negatively trained group was not significant (M before = 34.2, SD = 5.9; M after = 33.9, SD = 6.4, t(40) = 0.60, ns).
Statistical analyses
Mediation analyses were carried out separately for the two indices of induced interpretative bias: reaction time data and recognition data (r = −.26). The indices were calculated by subtracting the means for negative information from that of positive information.
A stepwise procedure was used to test two models. Based on the theoretical framework, the first model represents the indirect effect, with the interpretive bias mediating the relationship between CBM-I and anxiety. It consists of a path from CBM-I to the interpretive bias and a path from the interpretive bias to change in anxiety. Model 2 includes a direct path from CBM-I to anxiety. The fit of the models was evaluated using the chi-square goodness-of-fit test. As this test is criticized for its dependence on sample size, absolute (root mean square error of approximation, RMSEA) and incremental fit (comparative fit index, CFI) indices were included. As the sample size was relatively small for structural equation modelling, a bootstrap method was performed that showed that the statistics under consideration were unbiased (Efron and Tibshirani, Reference Efron and Tibshirani1993).
Results
State anxiety
First, the model representing the indirect effect between CBM-I and state anxiety through interpretive bias (Model 1) was analyzed using the reaction time index. This model did not fit the data (χ2 (1) = 11.9, p < .001, CFI = .48, RMSEA = .37). The second model (including a direct path from CBM-I to anxiety) resulted in a fully saturated model, hence with no degrees of freedom left (χ2 (0) = 0). This model had a significantly better fit (χ2difference = 11.9, Δdf = 1, p < .001). The CBM-I procedure was a significant predictor of change in anxiety (r = −.42, β = −0.38, p < .001) with positive modification resulting in a reduction of anxiety. Furthermore, CBM-I was a significant predictor of interpretive bias (r = .29, β = −0.29, p < .01), while the interpretive bias was not a significant predictor of anxiety. The final model accounted for a total of 19% of the variance in anxiety and 8% of the variance in interpretive bias.
Using the recognition data, Model 1, again, did not fit the data (χ2 (1) = 5.38, p < .05, CFI = .91, RMSEA = .23). Model 2 fit significantly better (χ2 (0) = 0, χ2difference = 5.38, Δdf = 1, p < .05). Again, the CBM-I procedure was a significant predictor of anxiety (r = .42 β = −0.30, p < .05) and interpretive bias (r = .53, β = 0.60, p < .001). Interpretive bias did not predict change in anxiety. The final model accounted for 20% of the variance in anxiety change and 36% of the variance in interpretive bias.
In sum, changes in state anxiety were not related to the interpretive bias; they were caused by direct effects of the CBM-I procedure. Thus, the interpretive bias did not mediate the relationship between CBM-I and changes in state anxiety (Figure 1, part A).
Trait anxiety
Similar analyses were performed using trait anxiety as the dependent variable. The first model tested the indirect effect (reaction time measure) and provided a good fit to the data (χ2 (1) = 1.7, p = .19, CFI = .94, RMSEA = .09). Model 2 did not result in a significant improvement of the model (χ2difference = 1.7, Δdf = 1, p = .19). The model consisting only of indirect effects represented the trait anxiety data most accurately, with CBM-I being a significant predictor of interpretive bias (r = .29, β = −0.29, p < .01) and interpretive bias predicting change in trait anxiety (r = .27, β = 0.27, p < .05). This model accounted for 8% of the variance in interpretive bias and 7% of the variance in trait anxiety.
With the recognition data, similar results were obtained. The first model resulted in a good fit to the data (χ2 (1) = 0.24, p = .62, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = .00). The second model did not significantly improve the model's fit (χ2difference = 0.24, Δdf = 1, p = .62). In the first model, CBM-I was a significant predictor of interpretive bias (r = .53, β = 0.60, p < .001) and interpretive bias was a significant predictor of changes in trait anxiety (r = .25, β = −0.28, p < .01)Footnote 1. The CBM-I accounted for 36% of the variance in interpretive bias, which in turn explained 8% of the variance in trait anxiety.
In sum, changes in trait anxiety observed after a CBM-I procedure are due to the modified interpretive bias (Figure 1, part B).
Study 2
Mediation analyses suggested that changes in state anxiety were directly caused by the CBM-I procedure itself. Given the widespread use of the CBM-I procedure, it appeared worthwhile to further study what elements of the procedure are responsible for the observed effects on state anxiety. In the original CBM-I studies the disambiguation of the social scenarios and the active generation of solutions for the word fragments were seen as the crucial elements in affecting mood. To test the role of these elements and to test the alternative hypothesis that mere exposure to valenced materials would be sufficient to affect state anxiety, participants were exposed to positive or negative materials much as they were in the original CBM-I procedure, but this time it was not preceded by an ambiguous social story. Half of the participants were asked to complete word fragments (completion yielded positive or negative words), while the others were exposed to complete words (that had either a positive or negative valence). Using the argument that CBM-I could affect state anxiety through exposure to valenced information, it was predicted that the exposure to positive information (whether presented as completed or incomplete words) would result in a decline in anxiety and the exposure to negative information would result in an increase.
Method
Participants
Eighty-nine unselected students participated (74 female/15 male); 45 participants were in the positive exposure condition (23 in the complete words vs. 22 in the fragmented words condition) and 44 in the negative exposure condition (22 in both the complete and fragmented words condition). Their mean age was 21.5 years (SD = 2.8).
Materials
The words used in the present study were used in earlier CBM-I studies. As in some cases removing the ambiguous story resulted in words that lacked the intended modification valence and some words had multiple solutions, such words were replaced (60 words = 35%) with words from two blocks that were used in earlier studies. The number of trials was similar to that of earlier CBM-I studies; eight blocks, each containing 13 words. Eight words were the so-called (mood) induction words; these are the crucial words with a positive or negative valence. Two words were the so-called probe words and three other words were included as fillers. Stimuli were presented in a random order in each block.
Participants in the fragment completion condition were asked to complete the fragments as quickly as possible by pressing the spacebar as soon as they could think of the correct completion. They were then asked to type in the first missing letter of the fragment and the completed word was displayed for 1 s. Trials in this condition had a duration of approximately 2880 ms and therefore trials in the complete word condition also lasted for 2880 ms. Participants in this latter condition were instructed to read and pay attention to the words. To check whether interpretations were inadvertently changed, interpretations were assessed with the recognition task.
Procedure
Participants were allocated at random to one of the experimental conditions. The computer-program started with the state and trait versions of the STAI. Then participants either carried out the positive or the negative condition and either the complete or the fragmented word condition. This was followed by the second STAI-state and the recognition task.
Results
Anxiety
A 2 (valence) x 2 (word-type) x 2 (time) ANOVA indicated a Valence x Time interaction, F(1, 85) = 4.21, p < .05, η p2 = .05. There was a trend in the predicted direction for anxiety to decrease in the group who had processed positive information, t(44) = 1.70, p < .10 (M pre = 33.4, SD = 8.2; M post = 32.1, SD = 6.6) as opposed to a non-significant increase in the group who had processed negative information, t(43) = −1.2, ns (M pre = 33.7, SD = 8.6; M post = 34.8, SD = 7.9). Thus, independent of word type, mere exposure to valenced information seemed to have congruent effects on state anxiety.
Interpretations
A 2 (valence) x 2 (word-type) x 2 (recognition sentence type) x 2 (valence recognition sentence) ANOVA was performed on the recognition data. Besides simple main effects of sentence type and valence recognition sentence, all results, including any effects with exposure valence or word-type, were not significant. As intended, there are no indications of differences in interpretations.
Discussion
CBM-I procedures have been used to test the causal nature of the relationship between interpretive bias and anxiety by modifying interpretive bias and examining direct effects on state and trait anxiety. It is easy to conclude that the bias causes observed changes in affect. The current mediation analyses tested this causal pathway and revealed that changes in trait anxiety were indeed caused by the altered interpretations. Interestingly, there was in fact no relationship between an altered interpretive bias and changes in state anxiety. Given that these initial findings were only based on statistical analyses, an experiment was designed to directly test whether an element of the CBM-I procedure (exposure to valenced materials) could affect anxiety. This study showed that exposure to positive or negative words was sufficient to produce (small) changes in state anxiety.
A weakness of the present study is that the original CBM-I procedure was not incorporated in the second experiment and thus a direct comparison between anxiety changes instigated under CBM-I and present conditions is not possible. When comparing the present effect size regarding group differences in state anxiety (d = 0.44) with earlier effect sizes (Mathews and Mackintosh, Reference Mathews and Mackintosh2000: d = 1.55, Salemink, van den Hout and Kindt, Reference Salemink, van den Hout and Kindt2007a; Salemink et al., Reference Salemink, van den Hout and Kindt2007b: d = 0.27 and 0.92, Yiend et al., Reference Yiend, Mackintosh and Mathews2005: d = .86 and d = 0.23) then it reveals that the effect sizes fluctuate strongly. The present effect size does fall in the range of observed effects; however it seems likely that CBM-I has an additional effect on state anxiety besides exposure to valenced material. A direct comparison between the full CBM-I and a dismantled version remains an issue for further research.
The finding that interpretive bias seems to affect trait, but not state anxiety, is surprising as trait anxiety is defined as the stable tendency to react with state anxiety to stressful situations. Furthermore, state and trait anxiety scores are generally highly correlated. One possibility is that questions about one's present state may be answered by a simple introspection of one's current mood. Whereas inferring your feelings of general anxiety (trait anxiety) seems more of an elaborative process involving activation of the autobiographical data set, scanning it and evaluating it against the question being asked. This process might, thereby, leave more room for the interpretive bias to exert its influence. Note that the observed effects on trait anxiety are consistent with earlier findings of CBM-I affecting the degree to which individuals respond anxiously to a stress task (Mackintosh et al., Reference Mackintosh, Mathews, Yiend, Ridgeway and Cook2006; Wilson, Macleod, Mathews and Rutherford, Reference Wilson, Macleod, Mathews and Rutherford2006).
In sum, the present study showed that changes in state anxiety were not related to changes in interpretive bias and the former is thus not a valid indicator of a causal relationship. This bears implications for previous CBM-I experiments where conclusions about causality have been drawn after observing such changes in state anxiety. The relationship between CBM-I and changes in trait anxiety is, on the other hand, mediated by the interpretive bias. This is promising in the light of possible clinical application. Given that interpretive bias affects trait anxiety (a long lasting vulnerability factor for developing anxious mood), inducing a more positive interpretive bias in patients with an anxiety disorder should have beneficial effects on their trait anxiety level. Steps are being taken to examine whether the CBM-I method can be used as a tool in treating anxious individuals.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Gerty Lensvelt-Mulders for assisting in the Amos analyses.
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.