Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-l4dxg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T23:57:50.977Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Subjective effort derives from a neurological monitor of performance costs and physiological resources

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2013

Mattie Tops
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands. m.tops@vu.nlhttp://community.frontiersin.org/people/MattieTops/8492s.l.koole@vu.nlhttp://www.psy.vu.nl/nl/over-de-faculteit/medewerkers-alfabetisch/medewerkers-i-l/s-koole/index.asp
Maarten A. S. Boksem
Affiliation:
Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus University, 3062 PA Rotterdam, The Netherlands. Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands. maarten@boksem.nlwww.boksem.nl
Sander L. Koole
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands. m.tops@vu.nlhttp://community.frontiersin.org/people/MattieTops/8492s.l.koole@vu.nlhttp://www.psy.vu.nl/nl/over-de-faculteit/medewerkers-alfabetisch/medewerkers-i-l/s-koole/index.asp

Abstract

Kurzban et al.'s expectancy-value mechanism of effort allocation seems relevant in situations when familiar tasks are initiated. However, we think additional mechanisms are important when people continue with a task for a prolonged time. These mechanisms, which are particularly relevant for performance of novel or urgent tasks, involve neural systems that track performance costs and resources.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Why are some tasks experienced as more effortful than others? To address this question, it is useful to distinguish between reactive action control in unpredictable environments and predictive control in predictable environments. These different types of action control are supported by different brain systems. Predictive control areas are associated with the dorsal prefrontal cortex, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and dorsal striatum, which sustain feedforward action control in tasks that are familiar and predictable (Luu et al. Reference Luu, Jiang, Poulsen, Mattson, Smith and Tucker2011; Tops & Boksem Reference Tops and Boksem2011; Reference Tops and Boksem2012). By contrast, reactive control areas include the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and anterior insula (AI), which sustain momentary feedback-guided control when tasks are performed that are novel, urgent, or unpredictable. Reactive control thus represents a specialized mode of operation for detecting new information, encoding it in memory, and assimilating it into preexisting knowledge structures, and for changing earlier schemata, thereby facilitating future predictive control (Hasher & Zacks Reference Hasher and Zacks1979; Tops & Boksem Reference Tops and Boksem2011). Because reactive control reduces predictive homeostatic regulation of the internal milieu (discussed below), such cognitive control requires the momentary tracking of physiological costs and resources and is experienced as effortful. The experience of effort is hence an adaptive motivational mechanism that limits the (re-)initiation and prolonged performance of tasks that demand reactive control, especially when there are insufficient perceived benefits, threats, or resources to compensate for the physiological costs of reactive control (Boksem & Tops Reference Boksem and Tops2008).

The notions of predictability and controllability are central to understanding which challenges trigger a physiological stress response (Sapolsky Reference Sapolsky2005). Physiological responses to challenge parallel the two forms of action control: Reactive homeostatic responses arise in relation to changes in physiological variables that have already occurred, and predictive homeostatic responses emerge in anticipation of predictably timed challenges (Moore-Ede Reference Moore-Ede1986; Romero et al. Reference Romero, Dickens and Cyr2009; cf. Landys et al. Reference Landys, Ramenofsky and Wingfield2006). When a challenge or task is perceived as predictable and controllable, because resources are perceived to be sufficient for the task (e.g., enough muscle strength), predictive homeostasis is maintained and the task may not be experienced as effortful. By contrast, situational novelty (e.g., Hasher & Zacks Reference Hasher and Zacks1979; Shiffrin & Schneider Reference Shiffrin and Schneider1977) and unpredictability of cognitive operations (Ackerman Reference Ackerman1987; Fisk & Schneider Reference Fisk and Schneider1983) require effortful processing and can trigger reactive physiological responses that potentially incur health costs (Romero et al. Reference Romero, Dickens and Cyr2009). Importantly, reactive homeostatic control may decrease less urgent predictive homeostatic regulation, causing “somatic neglect” of, for example, circadian variation in appetite (Koole et al., in press).

Neuroimaging evidence supports our thesis that reactive control systems translate information about action costs and resources into a motivational feeling of effort. Through its reciprocal connections with autonomic and visceral centers of the nervous system such as the hypothalamus (Carmichael & Price Reference Carmichael and Price1995), the AI may be involved in the monitoring and regulation of peripheral resources such as glucose levels (Allport et al. Reference Allport, Butcher, Baird, MacGregor, Desmond, Tress, Colman and Davis2004), muscle condition (Craig Reference Craig2003), autonomic activation (Critchley et al. Reference Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman and Dolan2004), and the processing of aversive bodily states (Paulus & Stein Reference Paulus and Stein2006). In addition, insula activation has been related to the subjective perception of physical effort and exertion (de Graaf et al. Reference de Graaf, Gallea, Pailhous, Anton, Roth and Bonnard2004; Williamson et al. Reference Williamson, McColl, Mathews, Ginsburg and Mitchell1999; Reference Williamson, McColl and Mathews2003). The IFG/AI areas that are active when people experience subjective effort are also implicated in compensatory effort allocation with time on task. One study found the bilateral AI to be involved in assessing the level of energy expenditure required to reach a proposed effort (Prévost et al. Reference Prévost, Pessiglione, Metereau, Clery-Melin and Dreher2010), while several other studies suggested that increased attentional effort during performance over extended periods of time or after sleep deprivation is associated with increased activation of right-hemisphere ventral cortical areas including IFG/AI, and sometimes in the context of activity declines in dACC and/or the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Bell-McGinty et al. Reference Bell-McGinty, Habeck, Hilton, Rakitin, Scarmeas, Zarahn, Flynn, DeLaPaz, Basner and Stern2004; Chuah et al. Reference Chuah, Venkatraman, Dinges and Chee2006; Coull et al. Reference Coull, Frackowiak and Frith1998; Paus et al. Reference Paus, Zatorre, Hofle, Caramanos, Gotman, Petrides and Evans1997; Walker et al. Reference Walker, Stickgold, Alsop, Gaab and Schlaug2005). Moreover, momentary lapses in attention, which increase with time on task and fatigue, are associated with reduced activity in this right ventral attentional network, whereas its compensatory recruitment during subsequent trials is associated with recovery from lapses in attention (Weissman et al. Reference Weissman, Robets, Visscher and Woldorff2006).

Thus, the AI may influence action-selection by monitoring the availability of resources and the physiological costs associated with actions. The readout of this monitor may be experienced as feelings of effort, resistance, and discomfort that influence choices to initiate or (dis)continue task performance (Tops & de Jong Reference Tops and de Jong2006). Unlike what Kurzban et al. propose, increased subjective effort does not necessarily shift engagement towards alternative, more rewarding options, but may also stimulate disengagement, inactivity, and recuperation when perceived resources (as signaled by the AI) are low (Boksem & Tops Reference Boksem and Tops2008). In our view, this is the most important role of subjective effort in decision-making. Indeed, effort may be considered as an adaptive signal that the present behavioral strategy is no longer appropriate, because it continues to demand reactive control that usurps costly physiological resources when substantial resources have already been invested and the goal evidently has not yet been achieved. Feelings of effort may provide the cognitive system with a signal that stimulates lowering of current goals and/or seeking of less demanding alternative strategies.

A major advantage of our account over Kurzban et al.'s is that ours more precisely explains which tasks trigger subjective effort and fatigue (i.e., those that require reactive control, such as tasks that are novel or urgent). Moreover, our account is able to address the transition of prolonged effortful demand into persistent forms of fatigue. When the situation is uncontrollable, individuals are forced to rely on reactive control, associated with feelings of effort, up-regulation of reactive homeostatic responses, and decreased predictive homeostatic regulation. Although adaptive in the short-term when dealing with important and urgent situations, prolonged reactive homeostatic control can lead to enduring physiological changes (Romero et al. Reference Romero, Dickens and Cyr2009), which may give rise to chronic fatigue.

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1987) Individual differences in skill learning: An integration of psychometric and information processing perspectives. Psychological Bulletin 102:327.Google Scholar
Allport, L. E., Butcher, K. S., Baird, T. A., MacGregor, I., Desmond, P. M., Tress, B. M., Colman, P. & Davis, S. M. (2004) Insular cortical ischemia is independently associated with acute stress hyperglycemia. Stroke 33:1886–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell-McGinty, S., Habeck, C., Hilton, H. J., Rakitin, B., Scarmeas, N., Zarahn, E., Flynn, J., DeLaPaz, R., Basner, R. & Stern, Y. (2004) Identification and differential vulnerability of a neural network in sleep deprivation. Cerebral Cortex 14:496502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boksem, M. A. S. & Tops, M. (2008) Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits. Brain Research Reviews 59(1):125–39. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001.Google Scholar
Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. (1995) Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology 363:642–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chuah, Y. M. L., Venkatraman, V., Dinges, D. F. & Chee, M. W. L. (2006) The neural basis of interindividual variability in inhibitory efficiency after sleep deprivation. Journal of Neuroscience 26(27):7156–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coull, J. T., Frackowiak, R. S. & Frith, C. D. (1998) Monitoring for target objects: Activation of right frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task. Neuropsychologia 36(12):1325–34.Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2003) Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 13:500505.Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A. & Dolan, R. J. (2004) Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience 7:189–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Graaf, J. B., Gallea, C., Pailhous, J., Anton, J. L., Roth, M. & Bonnard, M. (2004) Awareness of muscular force during movement production: An fMRI study. NeuroImage 21:1357–67.Google Scholar
Fisk, A. D. & Schneider, W. (1983) Category and word search: Generalizing search principles to complex processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9:177–95.Google Scholar
Hasher, L. & Zacks, R. T. (1979) Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108:356–88.Google Scholar
Koole, S. L., Tops, M., Strübin, S., Bouw, J., Schneider, I. K. & Jostmann, N. B. (in press) The ego fixation hypothesis: Involuntary persistence of self-control. In: The control within: Motivation and its regulation, ed. Forgas, J. P. & Harmon-Jones, E.. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Landys, M. M., Ramenofsky, M. & Wingfield, J. C. (2006) Actions of glucocorticoids at a seasonal baseline as compared to stress-related levels in the regulation of periodic life processes. General and Comparative Endocrinology 148:132–49.Google Scholar
Luu, P., Jiang, Z., Poulsen, C., Mattson, C., Smith, A. & Tucker, D. M. (2011) Learning and the development of contexts for action. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5:159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore-Ede, M. C. (1986) Physiology of the circadian timing system: Predictive versus reactive homeostasis. American Journal of Physiology 250(5, Pt 2):R737–52.Google Scholar
Paulus, M. P. & Stein, M. B. (2006) An insular view of anxiety. Biological Psychiatry 60:382–87.Google Scholar
Paus, T., Zatorre, R. J., Hofle, N., Caramanos, Z., Gotman, J., Petrides, M. & Evans, A. C. (1997) Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9(3):392408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prévost, C., Pessiglione, M., Metereau, E., Clery-Melin, M. L. & Dreher, J. C. (2010) Separate valuation subsystems for delay and effort decision costs. Journal of Neuroscience 30(42):14080–90.Google Scholar
Romero, L. M., Dickens, M. J. & Cyr, N. E. (2009) The Reactive Scope Model–A new model integrating homeostasis, allostasis, and stress. Hormones and Behavior 55(3):375–89.Google Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M. (2005) The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science 308(5722):648–52.Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M. & Schneider, W. (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review 84:127–90.Google Scholar
Tops, M. & Boksem, M. A. S. (2011) A potential role of the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula in cognitive control, brain rhythms and event-related potentials. Frontiers in Psychology 2(330):114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tops, M. & Boksem, M. A. S. (2012) “What's that?” “What went wrong?” Positive and negative surprise and the rostral-ventral to caudal-dorsal functional gradient in the brain. Frontiers in Psychology 3(21):15.Google Scholar
Tops, M. & de Jong, R. (2006) Posing for success: Clenching a fist facilitates approach. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 13(2):229–34.Google Scholar
Walker, M. P., Stickgold, R., Alsop, D., Gaab, N. & Schlaug, G. (2005) Sleep-dependent motor memory plasticity in the human brain. Neuroscience 133(4):911–17.Google Scholar
Weissman, D. H., Robets, K. C., Visscher, K. M. & Woldorff, M. G. (2006) The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nature Neuroscience 9(7):971–78.Google Scholar
Williamson, J. W., McColl, R. & Mathews, D. (2003) Evidence for central command activation of the human insular cortex during exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology 94:1726–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, J. W., McColl, R., Mathews, D., Ginsburg, M. & Mitchell, J. H. (1999) Activation of the insular cortex is affected by the intensity of exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology 87:1213–19.Google Scholar