Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T20:00:00.709Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Second-person social neuroscience: Connections to past and future theories, methods, and findings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2013

Nicolas Vermeulen
Affiliation:
Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), Université Catholique de Louvain (UCL), 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. Nicolas.Vermeulen@uclouvain.behttp://uclouvain.academia.edu/NicolasVermeulenGordy.Pleyers@uclouvain.behttp://www.ecsa.ucl.ac.be/pleyers National Fund for Scientific Research (FRS-FNRS), B-1000 Brussels, Belgium
Gordy Pleyers
Affiliation:
Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), Université Catholique de Louvain (UCL), 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. Nicolas.Vermeulen@uclouvain.behttp://uclouvain.academia.edu/NicolasVermeulenGordy.Pleyers@uclouvain.behttp://www.ecsa.ucl.ac.be/pleyers
Martial Mermillod
Affiliation:
UniversitéGrenoble Alpes, LPNC, F-38040, Grenoble & CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105, F-38040 Grenoble, France. Martial.Mermillod@upmf-grenoble.frhttp://webu2.upmf-grenoble.fr/lpnc/membre_martial_mermillod Institut Universitaire de France, 75005 Paris, France

Abstract

We argue that Schilbach et al. have neglected an important part of the social neuroscience literature involving participants in social interactions. We also clarify some part of the models the authors discussed superficially. We finally propose that social neuroscience should take into consideration the effect of being observed and the complexity of the task as potentially influencing factors.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Our first point is that Schilbach et al. have neglected a relatively important part of the social neuroscience literature in which participants are actually involved in social interaction or exclusion/rejection with others. This major part of the literature shows that an overlap in neural activation exists between physical pain and social pain (following rejection). Studies that illustrate this point most commonly use the Cyberball paradigm, a computerized ball-tossing game eliciting feelings of social rejection and distress, which has previously been used in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies (e.g., Eisenberger & Lieberman Reference Eisenberger and Lieberman2004; Eisenberger et al. Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Williams2003). In this paradigm, participants who were first involved in a simulated ball-tossing game with two other players were then implicitly excluded from the game by the other two players who only passed the ball to each other, thereby socially rejecting the participant. Interestingly, the fMRI results showed that rejection produced brain activity in areas that are also activated when people experience physical pain.

We agree with Schilbach et al.'s claim that a second-person neuroscience would be particularly relevant to understanding mental or behavioral disorders. We have, in fact, recently examined social rejection (using Cyberball) in alcohol-dependent inpatients (Maurage et al. Reference Maurage, Joassin, Philippot, Heeren, Vermeulen, Mahau, Delperdande, Corneille, Luminet and De Timary2012). In this study, 22 abstinent alcohol-dependent participants and 22 paired controls played Cyberball during fMRI recording. Participants were first included by other players, then excluded and finally re-included (when the other two players resumed passing the ball to the participant). We found increased activation in brain areas typically associated with social-rejection feelings and with impaired ability to inhibit these feelings (as indexed by a reduction in frontal activation) in alcohol-dependent participants compared to matched controls. Reduced frontal regulation was suggested to be responsible for the interpersonal alterations observed in alcohol-dependence, which seems to be reinforced by impaired fronto-cingulate connectivity. As suggested by Schilbach et al., this very recent publication confirms the importance of second-person neuroscience studies as a dynamic tool for helping differential diagnosis in psychiatric disorders and also shows neglected studies related to second-person neuroscience.

Some other examples from this important field of literature may be found in studies investigating obedience to authority (such as the Milgram experiment). In a recent fMRI study, Cheetham et al. (Reference Cheetham, Pedroni, Antley, Slater and Jäncke2009) investigated the neural basis of obedience and empathy in participants who were instructed to punish a victim with electric shocks for every incorrect answer the victim gave. Other important examples of second-person social neuroscience come from studies on racism (Olsson et al. Reference Olsson, Ebert, Banaji and Phelps2005), out-group dehumanization (Hein et al. Reference Hein, Silani, Preuschoff, Batson and Singer2010), and even cognitive dissonance (Kitayama et al. Reference Kitayama, Snibbe, Markus and Suzuki2004). These studies represent only a few examples among many others of second-person social neuroscience effects that deserve to be reported in the current article.

The second point we argue is that Schilbach et al. have neglected an important aspect of the Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model. The SIMS model recently proposed by Niedenthal et al. (Reference Niedenthal, Mermillod, Maringer and Hess2010) does not only constitute a model of how involuntary mimicry occurs during social interactions (as discussed by Schilbach et al.), but also specifies the involvement of different neural areas (e.g., amygdala, somatosensory cortex) in the psychological understanding of others' feelings during second-person interactions. This model constitutes a theoretical model of second-person understanding of emotional states, which was applied to smiling only because this emotional expression constitutes one of the more complex and ambiguous expressions involved during social interaction. For instance, the SIMS model specifies the influence of different social contexts (e.g., cultural, affiliative) on the use of functional triggers (mainly eye contact) inducing subsequent embodied or grounded processes. As far as we understand, this SIMS model clearly fits with what Schilbach and colleagues have coined as second-person neuroscience “going social” and represents a direct and detailed second-person theoretical model of social interactions.

Finally, based on the findings from social psychology, we would like to stress the importance of taking into consideration, in future social neuroscience studies, the effect of being observed as well as the complexity of the task. Researchers have shown that performance may be impacted by the mere (or even imagined) presence of other people. More specifically, it has been claimed that in simple (well-learned) tasks, the presence of others leads to performance increments, whereas in complex (not well-learned) tasks performance is negatively influenced by the presence of others. This effect has been named the “activation theory model” by Zajonc (Reference Zajonc1965; see Strauss [Reference Strauss2001] for a review of this phenomenon). As well, many studies in nonhuman primates have also shown that these effects are not limited to humans but have been observed in other social species, such as Capuchin primates (Dindo et al. Reference Dindo, Whiten and de Waal2009). We believe it is important to consider this phenomenon in future social (e.g., second-person neuroscience) experimental situations because it implies that performance – in fMRI, for instance – does not rely solely on participants' abilities but also depends on the internal awareness of the presence (or envisaged presence) of others. In our opinion, this highlights the importance of better understanding whether (and how) activity in the neural network may be modulated by the feeling of being observed and/or evaluated. Moreover, such social neuroscience fMRI investigations may confirm (or disconfirm) the involvement of specific cognitive processes during social interactions (attention, short-term memory, etc.) (Muller et al. Reference Muller, Atzeni and Butera2004). This could be particularly important in psychopathology such as (social) anxiety disorders or alexithymia, as most of those disorders are known to be related to impaired attentional processes (Rossignol et al. Reference Rossignol, Anselme, Vermeulen, Philippot and Campanella2007; Vermeulen et al. Reference Vermeulen, Luminet, de Sousa and Campanella2008) or memory processes (Vermeulen & Luminet Reference Vermeulen and Luminet2009; Vermeulen et al. Reference Vermeulen, Toussaint and Luminet2010).

References

Cheetham, M., Pedroni, A. F., Antley, A., Slater, M. & Jäncke, L. (2009) Virtual Milgram: Empathic concern or personal distress? Evidence from functional MRI and dispositional measures. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 29:113. Available at: http://www.frontiersin.org/Human_Neuroscience/10.3389/neuro.09.029.2009/full Google Scholar
Dindo, M., Whiten, A. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2009) Social facilitation of exploratory foraging behavior in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). American Journal of Primatology 71:419–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I. & Lieberman, M. D. (2004) Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(7):294300.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D. & Williams, K. D. (2003) Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302(5643):290–92.Google Scholar
Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D. & Singer, T. (2010) Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members' suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68:149–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R. & Suzuki, T. (2004) Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychological Science 15:527–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maurage, P., Joassin, F., Philippot, P., Heeren, A., Vermeulen, N., Mahau, P., Delperdande, C., Corneille, O., Luminet, O. & De Timary, P. (2012) Disrupted regulation of social exclusion in alcohol-dependence: An fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 37:2067–75. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, D., Atzeni, T. & Butera, F. (2004) Coaction and upward social comparison reduce the illusory conjunction effect: Support for distraction-conflict theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40:659–65.Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M. & Hess, U. (2010) The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(6):417–33; discussion 433–80. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000865.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., Ebert, J. P., Banaji, M. R. & Phelps, E. A. (2005) The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science 309:785–87.Google Scholar
Rossignol, M., Anselme, C., Vermeulen, N., Philippot, P. & Campanella, S. (2007) Categorical perception of anger and disgust facial expression is affected by non-clinical social anxiety: An ERP study. Brain Research 1132(1):166–76.Google Scholar
Strauss, B. (2001) Social facilitation in motor tasks: A review of research and theory. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 3:237–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vermeulen, N. & Luminet, O. (2009) Alexithymia factors and memory performances for neutral and emotional words. Personality and Individual Differences 47:305309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vermeulen, N., Luminet, O., de Sousa, M. C. & Campanella, S. (2008) Categorical perception of anger is disrupted in alexithymia: Evidence from a visual ERP study. Cognition and Emotion 22(6):1052–67.Google Scholar
Vermeulen, N., Toussaint, J. & Luminet, O. (2010) The influence of alexithymia and music on the incidental memory for emotion words. European Journal of Personality 24(6):551–68.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1965) Social facilitation. Science 149(3681):269–74.Google Scholar