Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T00:18:09.075Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of the amygdala in the appraising brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 May 2012

David Sander
Affiliation:
Laboratory for the Study of Emotion Elicitation and Expression, Department of Psychology, FPSE and Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, CH-1205 Geneva, Switzerland. david.sander@unige.chhttp://www.unige.ch/fapse/EmotionLabhttp://www.affective-sciences.org/

Abstract

Lindquist et al. convincingly argue that the brain implements psychological operations that are constitutive of emotion rather than modules subserving discrete emotions. However, the nature of such psychological operations is open to debate. I argue that considering appraisal theories may provide alternative interpretations of the neuroimaging data with respect to the psychological operations involved.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Affective neuroscience has considerably extended our knowledge of the emotional brain during the last two decades. Most of the work in this domain has consisted of searching for discrete dedicated brain systems underlying each basic emotion, using as evidence either neuropsychological dissociations (see Calder et al. Reference Calder, Lawrence and Young2001) or brain imaging results (see Vytal & Hamann Reference Vytal and Hamann2010). Although this approach has extended our knowledge of the emotional brain, it has been strongly challenged by both empirical results and conceptual analyses (for details, see Sander, in press). Given that research on the brain basis of emotion has only rarely been explicitly considered in relation to psychological theories of emotion, the general aim of the current target article by Lindquist et al. is very beneficial to emotion research.

Lindquist et al. convincingly argue that we need to consider how the brain implements psychological operations that are constitutive of emotion rather than modules subserving discrete emotions. However, the nature of such psychological operations is open to debate. The authors only oppose their psychological constructionist view to the locationist view. Here, I argue that appraisal theories provide alternative interpretations to those offered by both the basic emotions perspective and by the authors. Lindquist et al. do not consider what can be seen as the third major research tradition in addition to “basic emotion” models and “core affect” models: namely, appraisal models. As a justification, the authors argue that: “Relatively little work from an appraisal perspective has investigated the brain basis of emotion (although see Sander et al. Reference Sander, Grafman and Zalla2003; Reference Sander, Grandjean, Kaiser, Wehrle and Scherer2007). Therefore, we do not discuss appraisal models further in this article” (sect. 2, para. 2). I feel that, although an affective neuroscience approach to appraisal mechanisms is relatively recent (see Sander et al. Reference Sander, Grandjean and Scherer2005), many appraisal mechanisms (e.g., novelty detection, intrinsic pleasantness, or goal-relevance) have in fact been the focus of intense empirical research in cognitive and affective neuroscience, but typically without links being made directly to emotion elicitation (see Sander, in press). In addition, some studies have explicitly tested appraisal-driven hypotheses (e.g., Grandjean & Scherer Reference Grandjean and Scherer2008). As described below, conceptual analyses and empirical research have also pointed to a role of particular brain structures (e.g., the amygdala) in specific appraisal mechanisms (e.g., relevance detection).

Where Lindquist et al. do consider alternative interpretations of their results (sect. 6.1), they consider alternative methodological interpretations of the absence of findings supporting basic emotions theories, but they do not consider alternative conceptual interpretations of the presence of their meta-analytical findings. Consider the authors' conclusion concerning the brain region that has most clearly been associated with emotion in the literature: the amygdala. The authors write that their meta-analytical finding “is consistent with our hypothesis that the amygdala responds preferentially to salient exteroceptive (vs. interoceptive) sensations” (sect. 5.1, para. 6). Attributing a key role to the amygdala in processing saliency of exteroceptive stimuli indeed characterizes this brain structure with respect to a specific psychological operation, rather than to a basic emotion. Historically, the amygdala was typically associated with one emotion: fear. Associating a basic emotion to a specific brain system was consistent with basic emotion models (see Ekman Reference Ekman, Dalgleish and Powers1999, p. 50). A common view has been that the amygdala is central to a “fear system,” or even a “fear module” (Öhman & Mineka Reference Öhman and Mineka2001). However, the lack of empirical evidence to conclude that the amygdala is specific to fear led scholars such as Kringelbach and Berridge (Reference Kringelbach and Berridge2009) to consider that equating the amygdala with fear is one of those “overly simple equations between neurobiology and psychology that merge into myth” (p. 481).

In the framework of appraisal theories of emotion, Sander and colleagues have argued that the computational profile of the amygdala is best characterized as default detection of stimuli appraised as relevant, given the individual's current concerns, such as goals, needs, and values (Sander et al. Reference Sander, Grafman and Zalla2003; see also, Sander, in press). The idea that the amygdala is critical for relevance detection is grounded in appraisal models of emotion (see Frijda Reference Frijda1986, p. 390; Sander et al. Reference Sander, Grandjean and Scherer2005). Therefore, an alternative interpretation of the function of the amygdala that does not advocate a specific role of the amygdala for fear can be offered from an appraisal perspective. I believe that there is an interesting distinction between our analysis of the function of the amygdala and the authors' suggestion that the amygdala is associated with the psychological operation of “core affect” (see Figure 2 of the target article). Indeed, Lindquist et al. define “core affect” as the mental representation of bodily sensations that can be experienced as feelings of hedonic pleasure and displeasure with some degree of arousal. However, the very notion that a bodily sensation is mentally represented implies that a bodily response has been elicited before interoception could possibly lead to experienced feelings. In our account, relevance detection is supposed to be primary to the mental representation of bodily sensations because it is supposed to be involved in the elicitation of the emotional response, including the generation of bodily sensations. When considering conceptualization/categorization, Lindquist et al. “hypothesize that this psychological operation makes a prediction about what caused core affective changes within one's own body or what caused the affective cues (e.g., facial actions, body postures, or vocal acoustics) in another person” (sect. 3, para. 6). By contrast, appraisal theories are not typically concerned with how an individual predicts the cause of an affective change, but rather, with how a relevant event actually causes an affective change (see Moors Reference Moors2009; Scherer & Ellsworth Reference Scherer, Ellsworth, Sander and Scherer2009). When the authors attribute a saliency processing function to the amygdala, it makes a critical theoretical difference whether they consider the role of saliency in (1) categorizing an external event as being a potential cause of an already present core affect, or rather in (2) the context-sensitive evaluation of an event as a function of the current concerns of the individual, which causes a change in the affective response, the latter being very much in line with appraisal theories of emotion.

References

Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D. & Young, A. W. (2001) Neuropsychology of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2:352–63. Available at: http://jppsg.ac.uk/psych/resources/lawrence2001.pdf.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1999) Basic emotions. In: Handbook of cognition and emotion, ed. Dalgleish, T. & Powers, M. J., pp. 4560, John Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986) The emotions. (Studies in emotions and social interaction). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grandjean, D. & Scherer, K. R. (2008) Unpacking the cognitive architecture of emotion processes. Emotion 8(3):341–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kringelbach, M. L. & Berridge, K. C. (2009) Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(11):479–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moors, A. (2009) Theories of emotion causation: A review. Cognition and Emotion 23(4):625–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A. & Mineka, S. (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review 108:483522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sander, D. (in press) Models of emotion: The affective neuroscience approach. In: Handbook of human affective neuroscience, ed. Armony, J. L. & Vuilleumier, P.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grafman, J. & Zalla, T. (2003) The human amygdala: An evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences 14(4):303–16. Available at: http://www.affective-sciences.org/system/files/Sander_etal_RevNeuro.PDF.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sander, D., Grandjean, D, Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T. & Scherer, K. R. (2007) Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19:470–80. Available at: http://www.lemanic-neuroscience.ch/PENSTrainingCenter/articles/dsdg_ejcp06.pdf.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D. & Scherer, K. R. (2005) A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks 18:317–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. & Ellsworth, P. C. (2009) Appraisal theories. In: The Oxford Companion to emotion and the affective sciences, ed. Sander, D. & Scherer, K. R.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vytal, K. & Hamann, S. (2010) Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22(12):2864–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed