Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T17:38:36.609Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reconsolidation versus retrieval competition: Rival hypotheses to explain memory change in psychotherapy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Chris R. Brewin*
Affiliation:
Clinical Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. c.brewin@ucl.ac.uk

Abstract

I suggest it is premature to assume memory reconsolidation provides a unifying model of psychotherapeutic change given our current state of knowledge, and that other basic memory mechanisms, also supported by neuroscience, have a stronger claim at present. In particular, I propose that retrieval competition provides a more plausible alternative to memory reconsolidation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Two opposing views concerning memory change have held sway in cognitive science as in psychotherapy research (Brewin Reference Brewin1989; Reference Brewin2006). One states that interventions such as supplying misinformation or conducting psychological therapy can in principle result in a permanent change in the underlying memory representation: Here, Lane et al. argue for this position and that it is attributable to a process of reconsolidation. The alternative view is that representations cannot be permanently altered, and that such interventions create new memories that leave the original intact. Whether old or new memories then determine behaviour is decided through a process of retrieval competition, with the most accessible memory having the greatest influence (Brewin Reference Brewin2006).

Reconsolidation has not yet been extensively studied in humans, and there is controversy over whether it has been unambiguously established. A number of studies have used reconsolidation manipulations to reduce people's emotional response to a fear-inducing experience, an approach which leaves the declarative recollection of that experience intact. Other studies, such as those reported by the authors, have demonstrated changes in memory for one set of objects brought about by participants being given an indirect reminder of this set just before being exposed to a new set of objects. Using this design, in which there is no explicit retrieval of the original memory, it is hard to prove unambiguously that reconsolidation has taken place, although other recent research has addressed some of these issues (Chan & LaPaglia Reference Chan and LaPaglia2013; Kroes et al. Reference Kroes, Tendolkar, van Wingen, van Waarde, Strange and Fernandez2014).

Even if the manipulations in these studies are producing genuine reconsolidation effects on declarative memory, these are relatively small in absolute terms. They are also obtained with designs involving the memorisation of personally meaningless stimuli. Given apparent boundary conditions for reconsolidation effects (Alberini et al. Reference Alberini, Johnson, Ye and Alberini2013), such effects might not occur with memories of painful experiences that people have recalled and perhaps ruminated over dozens if not hundreds of times. Moreover, anxious individuals may be less likely to show fear-related reconsolidation effects (Soeter & Kindt Reference Soeter and Kindt2013). Finally, the claim that memories have been permanently changed can logically only be studied by thorough retesting and trying to elicit the original memory in another way, something that remains to be done.

Some predictions made by Lane et al. do not obviously favour a reconsolidation approach. For example, one suggestion was that propranolol is likely to be contra-indicated in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) therapy because it may slow the reconsolidation of memories that incorporate a new sense of safety. Preliminary evidence appears to support the use of propranolol, however (Brunet et al. Reference Brunet, Poundja, Tremblay, Bui, Thomas, Orr, Azzoug, A, Birmes and Pitman2011). The authors also suggested that selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) are likely to impair the processes of change in psychotherapy because their adverse effects on REM sleep interferes with reconsolidation. However, most of the evidence suggests that psychotherapy in combination with SSRIs achieves better outcomes than alone (e.g., March et al. Reference March, Silva, Petrycki, Curry, Wells, Fairbank, Burns, Domino and McNulty2004; van Apeldoorn et al. Reference van Apeldoorn, van Hout, Mersch, Huisman, Slaap, Hale, Visser, van Dyck and den Boer2008).

The main alternative possibility is that psychotherapy creates new memories that compete with existing ones. Many contemporary theories of human and animal cognition (e.g, Kesner & Rogers Reference Kesner and Rogers2004; Poldrack & Packard Reference Poldrack and Packard2003) favour the idea that learning produces a variety of new representations that can collaborate or compete with existing memories for control of behavior. Lane et al. mention extinction, a well-established therapeutic process which is thought to operate in this way, but do not explain why they consider reconsolidation to be a more likely candidate for explaining therapeutic change.

Among the advantages of a retrieval competition approach are that it does not assume a very malleable memory system that is constantly subject to change. In evolutionary terms, it would seem better to have a memory system that was able to keep a permanent record of extremely stressful or threatening events, because this information could be critical for survival. In fact, although it is fashionable to emphasise the malleability of memory, there is also plenty of evidence for its stability and reliability (Brewin & Andrews, submitted). This is hard to explain if memories are constantly being updated, leaving no trace of the original behind.

Another advantage accrues from the fact that relapse is reasonably common even after successful psychotherapy. This is hard to explain if it is assumed the underlying memory has been permanently updated. It is much more consistent with the idea that recovery is brought about by the creation of stable alternative memory structures but that the original remains able to be reactivated under a specific set of circumstances. This is a helpful model when teaching relapse prevention procedures to patients nearing the end of a course of therapy. It emphasises that the return of negative feelings and cognitions is a continuing hazard but one that is not catastrophic and that probably reflects the presence of specific triggers. Patients can then be encouraged to re-institute the coping mechanisms they have been taught rather than interpret the relapse as a sign of treatment failure.

At present our understanding of therapeutic change is rudimentary, and proposals for new mechanisms grounded in cognitive neuroscience are extremely welcome. It is likely that the many varieties of transformation that patients undergo will never be explained by any one mechanism, whether reconsolidation or retrieval competition. Basic research on memory change does, however, constitute one of the most promising lines of enquiry presently available, and findings from both animal and human research have the capacity to deliver new insights into what makes therapy effective. Although I agree with the authors that different forms of memory, such as semantic and episodic memory, are strongly interlinked, this does not mean that they are all equally important in different types of psychopathology. There are also perceptual forms of memory that appear to be distinct from episodic memory and may become relatively disconnected within conditions such as posttraumatic stress disorder (Brewin Reference Brewin2014; Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Gregory, Lipton and Burgess2010). It seems highly likely that our understanding of different change mechanisms needs to develop in tandem with a more differentiated view of human memory systems.

References

Alberini, C. M., Johnson, S. A. & Ye, X. (2013) Memory reconsolidation: Lingering consolidation and the dynamic memory trace. In: Memory reconsolidation, ed. Alberini, C. M., pp. 81117. Academic.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. R. (1989) Cognitive change processes in psychotherapy. Psychological Review 96:379–94. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.96.3.379.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R. (2006) Understanding cognitive-behaviour therapy: A retrieval competition account. Behaviour Research and Therapy 44:765–84. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.02.005.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R. (2014) Perceptual memory, episodic memory and their interaction: Foundations for a theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin 140:6997. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/a0033722.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R. & Andrews, B. (submitted) A new mythology? Reconstructive aspects of memory and their relevance for the courtroom.Google Scholar
Brewin, C. R., Gregory, J., Lipton, M. & Burgess, N. (2010) Intrusive images in psychological disorders: Characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment implications. Psychological Review 117:210–32. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/a0018113 Google Scholar
Brunet, A., Poundja, J., Tremblay, J., Bui, E., Thomas, E., Orr, S. P. Azzoug, A, , Birmes, P & Pitman, R. K. (2011) Trauma reactivation under the influence of propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress symptoms and disorder: 3 open-label trials. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 31:547–50. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0b013e318222f360.Google Scholar
Chan, J. C. & LaPaglia, J. A. (2013) Impairing existing declarative memory in humans by disrupting reconsolidation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 110(23):9309–13. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1218472110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kesner, R. P. & Rogers, J. (2004) An analysis of independence and interactions of brain substrates that subserve multiple attributes, memory systems, and underlying processes. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 82:199215. Available at: http:doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.05.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroes, M. C. W., Tendolkar, I., van Wingen, G. A., van Waarde, J. A., Strange, B. A. & Fernandez, G. (2014) An electroconvulsive therapy procedure impairs reconsolidation of episodic memories in humans. Nature Neuroscience 17:204206. Available at: http:doi.org/10.1038/nn.3609 Google Scholar
March, J., Silva, S., Petrycki, S., Curry, J., Wells, K., Fairbank, J., Burns, B., Domino, M. & McNulty, S. (2004) Fluoxetine, cognitive-behavioral therapy, and their combination for adolescents with depression: Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study (TADS) randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association 292(7):807–20. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1001/jama.292.7.807 Google ScholarPubMed
Poldrack, R. A. & Packard, M. G. (2003) Competition among multiple memory systems: Converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia 41:245–51. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00157-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soeter, M. & Kindt, M. (2013) High trait anxiety: A challenge for disrupting fear memory reconsolidation. PLOS ONE 8(11):e75239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Apeldoorn, F. J., van Hout, W. J., Mersch, P. P., Huisman, M., Slaap, B. R., Hale, W. W., Visser, S., van Dyck, R. & den Boer, J. A. (2008) Is a combined therapy more effective than either CBT or SSRI alone? Results of a multicenter trial on panic disorder with or without agoraphobia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 117:260–70. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01157.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed