Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T16:12:23.740Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Numbers in action

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2021

David C. Burr
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology, and Child Health, University of Florence, 50139Florence, Italy. davidcharles.burr@unifi.itgiovanni.anobile@unifi.itelisa.castaldi@gmail.comroberto.arrighi@unifi.ithttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/giovanni-anobilehttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/roberto-arrighi CNR Neuroscience Institute, Department of Biological Sciences, 56100Pisa, Italy. https://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/david-burr
Giovanni Anobile
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology, and Child Health, University of Florence, 50139Florence, Italy. davidcharles.burr@unifi.itgiovanni.anobile@unifi.itelisa.castaldi@gmail.comroberto.arrighi@unifi.ithttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/giovanni-anobilehttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/roberto-arrighi
Elisa Castaldi
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology, and Child Health, University of Florence, 50139Florence, Italy. davidcharles.burr@unifi.itgiovanni.anobile@unifi.itelisa.castaldi@gmail.comroberto.arrighi@unifi.ithttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/giovanni-anobilehttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/roberto-arrighi Department of Translational Research and New technologies in Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, 56126Pisa, Italy. https://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/elisa-castaldi
Roberto Arrighi
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology, and Child Health, University of Florence, 50139Florence, Italy. davidcharles.burr@unifi.itgiovanni.anobile@unifi.itelisa.castaldi@gmail.comroberto.arrighi@unifi.ithttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/giovanni-anobilehttps://www.pisavisionlab.org/members/roberto-arrighi

Abstract

To understand the number sense, we need to understand its function. We argue that numerosity estimation is fundamental not only for perception, but also preparation and control of action. We outline experiments that link numerosity estimation with action, pointing to a generalized numerosity system that serves both perception and action preparation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Clarke and Beck do an impressive job reviewing, and largely refuting, objections for the existence of a number sense, such as congruency, confounds, and imprecision. Arguments of this type are not new. For example, imprecision is at the basis of the well-known philosophical “problem of the speckled hen,” presented by Gilbert Ryle to Ayer (Reference Ayer1940): “Consider the sense datum yielded by a single glance of a speckled hen: how many speckles does the datum comprise?.” That humans cannot enumerate the number of speckles was considered a major challenge to prevailing philosophical theories about “given, direct experiences.” Why did Ryle choose number for his challenge, rather than the color, height, or weight of the hen, all equally impossible to judge with great precision? Clearly, our inability to enumerate a discrete number of specks makes the point more intuitively. Perhaps, it is the digital nature of numbers, which implies a discrete and precise description; or perhaps because we have multiple ways of measuring number, including rapid but approximate estimation (approximate number system [ANS]), systematic, and errorless serial counting, as well as exploiting grouping strategies (Anobile, Castaldi, Moscoso, Burr, & Arrighi, Reference Anobile, Castaldi, Moscoso, Burr and Arrighi2020a; Starkey & McCandliss, Reference Starkey and McCandliss2014). We can, therefore, internally check our rough numerosity estimation, readily betraying its imprecision: checking analog attributes requires instruments such as photometers, tape-measures, or scales. However, the fact that numerosity can be gaged in various ways, with variable precision, does not refute the existence of a number sense. On the contrary, that number estimation is imprecise and essentially noise-limited is further evidence that it is a sensory system. Ayer did not have the concept of noise-limitation in 1940 (introduced a few years later to psychology and physiology), but correctly anticipated that although the hen does have a definite number of speckles, the sense datum has only an imprecise guess: essentially, the approximate number system.

Perhaps, the more pressing question is not so much whether a number sense exists, or what class of numbers it encodes, but what purpose does it serve? Has it evolved primarily for estimating the number of speckles on a hen? – or the number of times the hen pecks? – or to help control and monitor the hen's own pecking behavior? It is likely that all are relevant, but the role of perception in action has traditionally been underrated (Goodale, Reference Goodale2014). We argue that numerosity perception is intrinsically linked with action. This is particularly clear in complex tasks such as ballet routines, music production, and the extraordinary waggle dance of bees. But action and number are strongly linked in most movement tasks, such as walking, talking, or eating. It is, therefore, perhaps not surprising that neurons have been identified in monkey cortex that are selective to the number of actions the monkey makes, either turns or pushes (Sawamura, Shima, & Tanji, Reference Sawamura, Shima and Tanji2002).

We have used adaptation techniques to reveal a strong link between action and number estimation in humans (Anobile, Arrighi, Togoli, & Burr, Reference Anobile, Arrighi, Togoli and Burr2016). Participants tapped in mid-air with their dominant hand, either very quickly, or around one tap per second. Fast tapping caused robust underestimation of the numerosity of subsequently presented stimuli, and slow tapping caused robust overestimation (see Fig. 1).

Figure 1. Effects of motor adaptation on perceived numerosity. Average perceived numerosity after a few seconds of slow (blue) or fast mid-air tapping (red), as a function of physical numerosity.

The effects were large, around 25%, and equally strong for estimating the number of items in a spatial array as for the number of events in a temporal sequence. This reinforces evidence of a generalized sense of number, spanning space, time, and sensory modality (Arrighi, Togoli, & Burr, Reference Arrighi, Togoli and Burr2014), and shows that this general sense is strongly linked to action. Importantly, adaptation (either to tapping or to sequential stimuli) does not generalize over the entire visual field but is confined to the immediate spatial vicinity where the hand had tapped or the stimuli presented (irrespective of the tapping hand). This demonstrates a spatially specific perceptual origin, rather than adaptation or a more general cognitive effect (such as internal counting). Interestingly, the spatial selectivity (for tactile sequences) is as strong in the congenitally blind as in sighted participants (Togoli, Crollen, Arrighi, & Collignon, Reference Togoli, Crollen, Arrighi and Collignon2020), showing that visual experience is unnecessary.

The effect of adapting to hand-tapping on perception was not limited to numerosity, but observed also with duration and spatial location estimates (Anobile, Domenici, Togoli, Burr, & Arrighi, Reference Anobile, Domenici, Togoli, Burr and Arrighi2020b; Petrizzo, Anobile, & Arrighi, Reference Petrizzo, Anobile and Arrighi2020). This again is to be expected, given the close links between space, time, and number (Walsh, Reference Walsh2003), and their clear role in action (especially, time).

Other links between action and numerosity perception have been reported with saccadic eye movements. Observers can saccade very quickly toward the more numerous of two arrays, implying a link between action and numerosity systems through dedicated pre-attentive mechanisms (Castaldi, Burr, Turi, & Binda, Reference Castaldi, Burr, Turi and Binda2020). At the time of saccades, numerosities of spatial arrays are grossly underestimated, paralleling the effects on temporal duration and spatial extent (Burr, Ross, Binda, & Morrone, Reference Burr, Ross, Binda and Morrone2010). Saccades also affect symbolic numbers: Participants underestimate the results of additions and subtractions when digits are presented at the time of saccades (Binda, Morrone, & Bremmer, Reference Binda, Morrone and Bremmer2012). Pupil size is modulated by perceived numerosity, even in the absence of a psychophysical task (Castaldi, Pomè, Cicchini, Burr, & Binda, Reference Castaldi, Pomè, Cicchini, Burr and Binda2021).

All these results reinforce the existence of an approximate number system in humans, and show that this system encodes numerosity in a generalized manner, across space and time and sensory modality, for use in both perception and action (Anobile, Arrighi, Castaldi, & Burr, Reference Anobile, Arrighi, Castaldi and Burr2021). As perception and action are strongly linked in everyday life, the emergence of a sensorimotor mechanism would seem to be a parsimonious and evolutionary useful strategy. For these functions, natural numbers (which include the fascinating case of zero; Nieder, Reference Nieder2016) are sufficient, but we cannot exclude the possibility that the same system encodes rational numbers such as fractions when required.

Financial support

This work was supported by Horizon 2020 (DB, grant number 832813); the Italian Ministry of Education, University, and Research (DB, grant number 2017SBCPZY; RA, grant number 2017XBJN4F); and the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant (EC, grant number 885672).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Anobile, G., Arrighi, R., Castaldi, E., & Burr, D. C. (2021). A sensorimotor numerosity system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(1), 2436. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anobile, G., Arrighi, R., Togoli, I., & Burr, D. C. (2016). A shared numerical representation for action and perception. eLife, 5, 114. doi:10.7554/eLife.16161CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anobile, G., Castaldi, E., Moscoso, P. A. M., Burr, D. C., & Arrighi, R. (2020a). “groupitizing”: A strategy for numerosity estimation. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 13436. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68111-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anobile, G., Domenici, N., Togoli, I., Burr, D., & Arrighi, R. (2020b). Distortions of visual time induced by motor adaptation. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 149(7), 13331343. doi: 10.1037/xge0000709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arrighi, R., Togoli, I., & Burr, D. C. (2014). A generalized sense of number. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 281(1797), 20141791. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.1791Google ScholarPubMed
Ayer, A. J. (1940). The foundations of empirical knowledge. London: Macmillan and Co., Limited.Google Scholar
Binda, P., Morrone, M. C., & Bremmer, F. (2012). Saccadic compression of symbolic numerical magnitude. PLoS One, 7(11), e49587. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049587CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burr, D. C., Ross, J., Binda, P., & Morrone, M. C. (2010). Saccades compress space, time and number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 528533. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castaldi, E., Burr, D., Turi, M., & Binda, P. (2020). Fast saccadic eye-movements in humans suggest that numerosity perception is automatic and direct. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 287(1935), 20201884. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.1884Google Scholar
Castaldi, E., Pomè, A., Cicchini, G. M., Burr, D., & Binda, P. (2021). Pupil size automatically encodes numerosity. V-VSS, May 21–26, 2021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodale, M. A. (2014). How (and why) the visual control of action differs from visual perception. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 281(1785), 20140337. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0337Google ScholarPubMed
Nieder, A. (2016). Representing something out of nothing: The dawning of zero. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(11), 830842. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrizzo, I., Anobile, G., & Arrighi, R. (2020). Motor adaptation distorts visual space. Vision Research, 171, 3135. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sawamura, H., Shima, K., & Tanji, J. (2002). Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. Nature, 415(6874), 918922. doi: 10.1038/415918aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starkey, G. S., & McCandliss, B. D. (2014). The emergence of “groupitizing” in children's numerical cognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 120137. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Togoli, I., Crollen, V., Arrighi, R., & Collignon, O. (2020). The shared numerical representation for action and perception develops independently from vision. Cortex, 129, 436445. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483488. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Effects of motor adaptation on perceived numerosity. Average perceived numerosity after a few seconds of slow (blue) or fast mid-air tapping (red), as a function of physical numerosity.