Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T13:23:17.548Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

No reliable gender differences in attachment across the lifespan

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2009

Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg
Affiliation:
Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, NL-2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands. bakermans@fsw.leidenuniv.nlvanijzen@fsw.leidenuniv.nlhttp://www.socialsciences.leidenuniv.nl/educationandchildstudies/childandfamilystudies/organisation/bakermans-kranenburg.jsphttp://www.socialsciences.leidenuniv.nl/educationandchildstudies/childandfamilystudies/organisation/van-ijzendoorn.jsp
Marinus H. van IJzendoorn
Affiliation:
Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, NL-2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands. bakermans@fsw.leidenuniv.nlvanijzen@fsw.leidenuniv.nlhttp://www.socialsciences.leidenuniv.nl/educationandchildstudies/childandfamilystudies/organisation/bakermans-kranenburg.jsphttp://www.socialsciences.leidenuniv.nl/educationandchildstudies/childandfamilystudies/organisation/van-ijzendoorn.jsp
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In middle childhood, boys show more avoidant attachments and girls more ambivalent attachments as a prelude to gender differentiation in reproductive strategies. However, we have failed to find systematic and method-independent gender differences in middle or late childhood attachments, nor in adult attachment representations. We conclude that Del Giudice's model rests on a brittle empirical basis.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Del Giudice's model of different reproductive strategies for females versus males hinges critically on the assumption that boys show more avoidant attachments and girls more ambivalence in the developmental period after early childhood. Attachment research in infancy and early childhood did not detect gender differences, whereas, according to Del Giudice in the target article, the picture changes “dramatically” in middle childhood. The question we address here is: Does the picture indeed change, and if so, in what respect?

Del Giudice lists seven studies to document this radical change. The three studies using the Coping Strategies Questionnaire (CSQ) should, however, be discounted as sources of evidence because patterns of attachment behavior and mental representations of attachment cannot be validly assessed by means of self-reports. Children as well as parents lack insight into their own attachment interactions and relationships, in particular when they are insecurely attached. Their insecurity distorts their self-perception (for meta-analytical evidence, see Van IJzendoorn et al. Reference Van IJzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Riksen-Walraven2004).

Four pertinent studies remain (Del Giudice, in press; Granot & Mayseless Reference Granot and Mayseless2001; Kerns et al. Reference Kerns, Abraham, Schlegelmilch and Morgan2007; Toth et al., personal communication, October 19, 2007). Only the Kerns et al. (Reference Kerns, Abraham, Schlegelmilch and Morgan2007) study produced results contrasting with Del Giudice's model, showing that female 9–11-year-olds were more often classified as avoidantly attached relative to male participants. The other studies pointed to the expected direction of insecure boys being more often avoidant and insecure girls more often ambivalent.

In our Leiden Attachment Research Program, we assessed quality of attachment in two studies on 7-year-old children (Gilissen et al. Reference Gilissen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn and Linting2008; Pannebakker Reference Pannebakker2007), and in one study on 14-year-olds (Beijersbergen et al., in press). Furthermore, after a brief literature search, we found pertinent studies by Ammaniti et al. (Reference Ammaniti, Van IJzendoorn, Speranza and Tambelli2000), Bureau et al. (Reference Bureau, Beliveau, Moss and Lepine2006), and Gloger-Tippelt and Koenig (Reference Gloger-Tippelt and Konig2007) on children aged 6–10 years. Following on Del Giudice's focus on the secure, avoidant, and ambivalent categories, we excluded the category of disorganized attachment, or used forced classifications when available. The combined distribution of secure, avoidant, and ambivalent attachments across all samples (including those discussed by Del Giudice) is 49% secure, 37% avoidant, and 14% ambivalent attachments for boys. For girls, the distribution is 64% secure, 22% avoidant, and 14% ambivalent attachments (see Table 1). Haberman's adjusted standardized residuals show significant differences between boys and girls for the secure (fewer boys) and the avoidant (more boys) classifications, but not for the ambivalent classification.

Table 1. Distributions of attachment in middle childhood and adulthood

* Significant adjusted standard residuals in bold

3 Beijersbergen et al. (in press); Ammaniti et al. (Reference Ammaniti, Van IJzendoorn, Speranza and Tambelli2000)

4 Van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg (in preparation)

Dividing the studies according to their assessment procedures (doll play narratives based on Bretherton et al. Reference Bretherton, Ridgeway, Cassidy, Greenberg, Cicchetti and Cummings1990; Cassidy Reference Cassidy1988), observations of separation/reunion (Main & Cassidy Reference Main and Cassidy1988), and modified Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; Hesse Reference Hesse, Cassidy and Shaver1999; Main et al. Reference Main, Kaplan, Cassidy, Bretherton and Waters1985), we only found gender differences in the set of studies using narratives (see Table 1). Apparently, the gender effect is measurement-specific, and systematic errors of measurement might be the source of these differences. Narrative procedures might focus more on content than on coherence, contaminating formal avoidance with substantive “macho” accounts. In addition, differences in verbal abilities between boys and girls in this age group may play a role (Verschueren & Marcoen Reference Verschueren and Marcoen1999). It should be noted that in Granot and Mayseless' (2001) study, secure children (mostly girls) had significantly higher scores on cognitive achievement, and avoidant children (predominantly boys) had the lowest scores on cognition.

Furthermore, the gender difference in studies with doll play narratives is restricted to boys showing more avoidant attachments, and not less or more ambivalence than girls. In the narrative approach, boys do follow the predicted pathway of avoidance, but girls do not prefer the ambivalent attachment strategy (see Table 1). In the observational studies, no gender differences in middle childhood have been documented. In the two studies using the AAI, boys were only slightly under-represented in the secure category and were not significantly over-represented in one of the insecure categories (Table 1).

It should be noted that numerous studies on attachment representations in adulthood using the Adult Attachment Interview – the gold standard for assessing attachment representations, which is independent of cognitive abilities (Hesse Reference Hesse, Cassidy and Shaver1999) – have not come up with any replicable gender differences in dismissing versus preoccupied attachments. In a meta-analysis of studies using the AAI (Van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg Reference Van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg1996), and in a recent update (Van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg Reference Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Steele and Steele2008; in preparation), we traced 17 studies on 687 males and 59 studies on 2997 females (mostly parents of reproductive age). The distribution of males was 33% dismissing, 48% secure, and 18% preoccupied attachments, while the distribution of females was 30% dismissing, 50% secure, and 20% preoccupied attachments. No significant gender difference was found (see Table 1).

We conclude that Del Giudice's model has a brittle empirical basis. We did not find systematic and method-independent gender differences in middle- or late-childhood attachments, nor were any gender differences in adulthood present in studies using the gold standard to assess attachment representations. The speculative model badly needs some repairs to accommodate with an obstinate empirical reality.

References

Ammaniti, M., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Speranza, A. M. & Tambelli, R. (2000) Internal working models of attachment during late childhood and early adolescence: An exploration of stability and change. Attachment and Human Development 2:328–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beijersbergen, M. D., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Juffer, F. (in press) Stress regulation in adolescents: Physiological reactivity during the adult attachment interview and conflict interaction. Child Development.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D. & Cassidy, J. (1990) Assessing internal working models of the attachment relationship: An attachment story completion task. In: Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention, ed. Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M., pp. 273308. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bureau, J. F., Beliveau, M. J., Moss, E. & Lepine, S. (2006) Association between the mother-child bond and the stories of attachment in the school age. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science (Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement) 38:5062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, J. (1988) Child-mother attachment and the self in six-year-olds. Child Development 59:121–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Giudice, M. (2008) Sex-biased ratio of avoidant/ambivalent attachment in middle childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 26:369–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilissen, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Linting, M. (2008) Electrodermal reactivity during the Trier Social Stress Test for Children: Interaction between the serotonin transporter polymorphism and children's attachment representation. Developmental Psychobiology 50(6):615–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gloger-Tippelt, G. & Konig, L. (2007) Attachment representations in 6-year-old children from one and two parent families in Germany. School Psychology International 28:313–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granot, D. & Mayseless, O. (2001) Attachment security and adjustment to school in middle childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development 25:530–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hesse, E. (1999) The Adult Attachment Interview: Historical and current perspectives. In: Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications, ed. Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R.. Guilford.Google Scholar
Kerns, K. A., Abraham, M. M., Schlegelmilch, A. & Morgan, T. A. (2007) Mother-child attachment in later middle childhood: Assessment approaches and associations with mood and emotion regulation. Attachment and Human Development 9:3353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Main, M. & Cassidy, J. (1988) Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age six: Predictable from infant attachment classification and stable over a 1-month period. Developmental Psychology 24:415526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Main, M., Kaplan, N. & Cassidy, J. (1985) Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In: Growing points in attachment theory and research, ed. Bretherton, I. & Waters, E.. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 50(1–2, Serial No. 209), pp. 66106.Google Scholar
Pannebakker, F. (2007) Morality from infancy to middle childhood. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Leiden University, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1996) Attachment representations in mothers, fathers, adolescents, and clinical groups: A meta-analytic search for normative data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 64:821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2008) The distribution of adult attachment representations in clinical groups: Meta-analytic search for patterns of attachment. In: The Adult Attachment Interview in clinical context, ed. Steele, H. & Steele, M.,pp. 6998. Guilford.Google Scholar
Van IJzendoorn, M. H. & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (in preparation) Distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical and non-clinical groups: A meta-analysis.Google Scholar
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. A. (2004) Is the Attachment Q-Sort a valid measure of attachment security in young children? Child Development 75:11881213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verschueren, K. & Marcoen, A. (1999) Representation of self and socioemotional competence in kindergartners: Differential and combined effects of attachment to mother and to father. Child Development 70:183201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Distributions of attachment in middle childhood and adulthood