Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T21:13:31.679Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Navigating in a volumetric world: Metric encoding in the vertical axis of space

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2013

Theresa Burt de Perera
Affiliation:
Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom. Theresa.Burt@zoo.ox.ac.ukRobert.Holbrook@zoo.ox.ac.ukVictoria.Davis@zoo.ox.ac.ukAlex.Kacelnik@zoo.ox.ac.ukTim.Guilford@zoo.ox.ac.ukhttp://oxnav.zoo.ox.ac.uk/
Robert Holbrook
Affiliation:
Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom. Theresa.Burt@zoo.ox.ac.ukRobert.Holbrook@zoo.ox.ac.ukVictoria.Davis@zoo.ox.ac.ukAlex.Kacelnik@zoo.ox.ac.ukTim.Guilford@zoo.ox.ac.ukhttp://oxnav.zoo.ox.ac.uk/
Victoria Davis
Affiliation:
Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom. Theresa.Burt@zoo.ox.ac.ukRobert.Holbrook@zoo.ox.ac.ukVictoria.Davis@zoo.ox.ac.ukAlex.Kacelnik@zoo.ox.ac.ukTim.Guilford@zoo.ox.ac.ukhttp://oxnav.zoo.ox.ac.uk/
Alex Kacelnik
Affiliation:
Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom. Theresa.Burt@zoo.ox.ac.ukRobert.Holbrook@zoo.ox.ac.ukVictoria.Davis@zoo.ox.ac.ukAlex.Kacelnik@zoo.ox.ac.ukTim.Guilford@zoo.ox.ac.ukhttp://oxnav.zoo.ox.ac.uk/
Tim Guilford
Affiliation:
Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom. Theresa.Burt@zoo.ox.ac.ukRobert.Holbrook@zoo.ox.ac.ukVictoria.Davis@zoo.ox.ac.ukAlex.Kacelnik@zoo.ox.ac.ukTim.Guilford@zoo.ox.ac.ukhttp://oxnav.zoo.ox.ac.uk/

Abstract

Animals navigate through three-dimensional environments, but we argue that the way they encode three-dimensional spatial information is shaped by how they use the vertical component of space. We agree with Jeffery et al. that the representation of three-dimensional space in vertebrates is probably bicoded (with separation of the plane of locomotion and its orthogonal axis), but we believe that their suggestion that the vertical axis is stored “contextually” (that is, not containing distance or direction metrics usable for novel computations) is unlikely, and as yet unsupported. We describe potential experimental protocols that could clarify these differences in opinion empirically.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Following behavioural and neurophysiological research on how three-dimensional space is encoded by animals, Jeffery et al. hypothesize that vertebrates represent space in a bicoded fashion, with spatial information about the plane of locomotion computed and represented separately from space in the orthogonal axis. We agree with this. Indeed, experimental evidence shows that fish that move freely through a volume represent space in two, separable, vertical and horizontal components, and not as an integrated three-dimensional unitary representation (Holbrook & Burt de Perera Reference Holbrook and Burt de Perera2009; Reference Holbrook and Burt de Perera2011b). This is supported by studies on rats (Grobéty & Schenk Reference Grobéty and Schenk1992a; Hayman et al. Reference Hayman, Verriotis, Jovalekic, Fenton and Jeffery2011; Jovalekic et al. Reference Jovalekic, Hayman, Becares, Reid, Thomas, Wilson and Jeffery2011). However, this still leaves open the fundamental question of whether the vertical axis is represented “contextually” (defined as not available for novel quantitative processing) or metrically.

Jeffery et al. are ambiguous on this point, as their bicoding hypothesis can accommodate the vertical axis being represented metrically, but they favour the idea that it is likely to be encoded contextually, even for animals that move freely through three dimensions. They reasonably argue that contextual coding may free the animal's brain from complex trigonometric calculations in three dimensions, but this comes at the cost of constraining spatial computations. For instance, metric encoding allows the animal to know when it is getting close, is roughly where it should be, or has gone too far, and it allows the animal to use the relation between multiple landmarks, correcting for distance and perspective when computing novel trajectories. It has been argued that in the horizontal plane, ants integrate cross-modally metric information obtained from path integration with visual snapshots of landmarks (Lent et al. Reference Lent, Graham and Collett2010; Mueller & Wehner Reference Mueller and Wehner2010). For animals that move freely in a volume, the same computational possibilities are important in the vertical axis.

Because metric information contains spatial relationships, the presence of metric representations can be expressed in how the pattern of error or generalization is distributed along the vertical axis. We offer two suggestions. First, we expect generalization along a metric vertical axis either side of significant locations, and for this “error” to obey Weber's law, displaying a roughly proportional relation between magnitude of the percept and accuracy (Foley & Matlin Reference Foley and Matlin2010). Such generalization would not apply to contextual representations. This could be tested by measuring error in recall of a rewarded vertical position for an animal that can move freely in the vertical axis (and hence express that error). Second, generalization curves around a positive and a negative instance should interfere if the instances are close in a metric vertical axis, and this interference should take the behavioural form of a computable peak shift (Domjan Reference Domjan2009). So, if an animal is rewarded for visiting a place at a certain elevation, the presence of a punishing instance marginally above this should result in a predictable downwards displacement of response.

Whether an animal encodes the vertical axis metrically or not may be related to the animal's movement and whether it is surface bound with three degrees of freedom (forward-backward, left-right, and rotational (yaw) or moves with six degrees of freedom by the addition of up-down, roll and pitch. In flying or swimming animals, the equality in the freedom of movement in the horizontal and vertical components of space is likely to favour metrical scales for space in both the horizontal and vertical dimensions.

References

Domjan, M. (2009) The principles of learning and behavior, 6th edition. Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Foley, H. J. & Matlin, M. W. (2010) Sensation and perception, 5th edition. Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Grobéty, M. C. & Schenk, F. (1992a) Spatial learning in a three-dimensional maze. Animal Behaviour 43(6):1011–20.Google Scholar
Hayman, R., Verriotis, M. A., Jovalekic, A., Fenton, A. A. & Jeffery, K. J. (2011) Anisotropic encoding of three-dimensional space by place cells and grid cells. Nature Neuroscience 14(9):1182–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holbrook, R. I. & Burt de Perera, T. B. (2009) Separate encoding of vertical and horizontal components of space during orientation in fish. Animal Behaviour 78(2):241–45.Google Scholar
Holbrook, R. I. & Burt de Perera, T. (2011b) Three-dimensional spatial cognition: Information in the vertical dimension overrides information from the horizontal. Animal Cognition 14:613–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jovalekic, A., Hayman, R., Becares, N., Reid, H., Thomas, G., Wilson, J. & Jeffery, K. (2011) Horizontal biases in rats' use of three-dimensional space. Behavioural Brain Research 222(2):279–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lent, D. D., Graham, P. & Collett, T. S. (2010) Image-matching during ant navigation occurs through saccade-like body turns controlled by learned visual features. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107:16348–53.Google Scholar
Mueller, M. & Wehner, R. (2010) Path integration provides a scaffold for landmark learning in desert ants. Current Biology 20:1368–71.Google Scholar