We appreciate the attempt in the target article by Redish et al. to provide an integrative framework for addiction from a multiple systems view, and we find many of the suggestions regarding addictive behaviors thought provoking. Our main concern is related to the lack of fit of the theoretical framework with recent data and models from psychological science.
Redish et al. distinguish between two broad processes that jointly explain addictions: an explicit and slow planning system, relying on stimulus/response-outcome (S/R-O) associations, and an implicit and fast habit system relying on stimulus-action/response (S-A/R) associations (Table 3 of the target article). In many recent psychological theories of addiction, a distinction is also made between two broad systems that superficially resemble the proposed systems (e.g., Deutsch & Strack Reference Deutsch, Strack, Wiers and Stacy2006; Evans & Coventry Reference Evans, Coventry, Wiers and Stacy2006; Stacy & Wiers Reference Stacy, Wiers, Munafo and Albero2006; Wiers & Stacy Reference Wiers and Stacy2006a; Reference Wiers and Stacy2006b; Wiers et al. Reference Wiers, Bartholow, van den Wildenberg, Thush, Engels, Sher, Grenard, Ames and Stacy2007). These models follow general dual process models in psychology in which the most characteristic difference between the systems concerns the representational format, not the contents of the associations (e.g., Strack & Deutsch Reference Strack and Deutsch2004). In this model, the reflective system uses the representations in the impulsive system and can perform logical operations on these representations, such as negation or placing the contents in a different time (e.g., the future). Deutsch et al. (Reference Deutsch, Gawronski and Strack2006) have demonstrated the consequence of this difference in representational format. When an association is learned and later participants are told that in fact the association was not true, participants correct this as becomes evident from their explicit expectancies, but when their automatic associations are assessed, they still show the original association. A study by Krank and Swift (Reference Krank and Swift1994) shows that this can have serious real-life consequences: adolescents who were told that alcohol does not make you sexy (a prevalent alcohol expectancy in adolescents; Goldman et al. Reference Goldman, Del Boca, Darkes, Leonard and Blane1999), showed stronger automatic associations between alcohol and sex a week later. This effect is opposite to what would be predicted if this association was only governed by a propositional planning system (cf. Gawronski et al. Reference Gawronski, Deutsch, Mbirkou, Seibt and Strack2008).
Relatedly, Redish et al. do not acknowledge a range of automatic associations that are conceptual (multi-modal) in nature, which do not necessarily involve S-A/R associations. These associative processes can be dissociated from explicit memory and operations of the planning systems. Examples include associations processed during semantic priming (e.g., Hutchison Reference Hutchison2003; Weingardt et al. Reference Weingardt, Stacy and Leigh1996), implicit conceptual memory (e.g., Levy et al. Reference Levy, Stark and Squire2004), or construct activation tasks (e.g., Arndt et al. Reference Arndt, Greenberg and Cook2002). The automatic activation of a variety of different classes of associations (beyond S-A/R) can spontaneously bias processing and judgments in ways that do not appear to involve planning, as revealed in social psychological research (e.g., Bargh & Morsella Reference Bargh and Morsella2008). Hence, our general point here is that there are many more associative processes that predict behavior than the S-A/R associations of the habit system and that these can be dissociated from explicit memory processes in the planning system.
When we turn to psychological addiction research, recent studies have found that relatively automatic processes such as an attentional bias for a substance, automatic memory associations, and action tendencies to approach alcohol are relatively independent from explicit processes such as motives and expectancies (see Wiers et al. Reference Wiers, Bartholow, van den Wildenberg, Thush, Engels, Sher, Grenard, Ames and Stacy2007 for a review). Expectancies and substance associations typically show low correlations and predict unique variance in substance use (e.g., Houben & Wiers Reference Houben and Wiers2006; Stacy Reference Stacy1997; Wiers et al. Reference Wiers, van Woerden, Smulders and De Jong2002). In recent studies, the relative predictive power of automatic substance associations and explicit expectancies to explain substance use was assessed in adolescents who differed in working memory (WM) capacity. For smoking (Grenard et al., in press) and alcohol use (Thush et al. Reference Thush, Wiers, Ames, Grenard, Sussman and Stacy2008), substance use was better predicted by automatic associations in participants with relatively poor WM capacity. Interestingly, Thush et al. (Reference Thush, Wiers, Ames, Grenard, Sussman and Stacy2008) found the opposite pattern for explicit expectancies, which predicted alcohol use better in participants with high WM capacity. Hence, some individuals' substance use appears to be more driven by their automatic associations, whereas others (with equally strong automatic substance associations but with a stronger WM capacity on top) appear to be more “rational” substance users. In summary, the distinction between relatively automatic or implicit associations and explicit expectancies has been fruitful in psychological research on addiction (see Wiers & Stacy Reference Wiers and Stacy2006a; Reference Wiers and Stacy2006b; Wiers et al. Reference Wiers, van, Luitgaarden, van, Wildenberg and Smulders2005; Reference Wiers, Bartholow, van den Wildenberg, Thush, Engels, Sher, Grenard, Ames and Stacy2007). Although Redish et al. also differentiate between two main systems (planning and habit), many of the processes distinguished in psychological research appear to be inaccurately grouped together and subsumed in their planning system.
A third issue concerns the role of conscious motivation in drug seeking. Redish et al. state that overvaluation of expected drug outcomes leads to craving and that this overvaluation in the planning system might be the result of incentive salience attribution, citing Robinson and Berridge (Reference Robinson and Berridge1993; Reference Robinson and Berridge2003). These authors, however, differentiate between “wanting,” the neural process of incentive salience attribution, and subjective craving, which can, but does not necessarily, result from the unconscious “wanting” process. This distinction may explain, for example, that smokers, after a Pavlovian conditioning procedure, show strong approach tendencies to smoking stimuli, particularly in the presence of cues predicting smoking opportunity, without these cues eliciting increased subjective craving (Thewissen et al. Reference Thewissen, Havermans, Geschwind, van, Hout and Jansen2007). In line with this distinction, a recent meta-analysis found low correlations between subjective craving and an attentional bias for alcohol (Field et al., in preparation). Redish et al. attribute devaluation and need-dependent evaluations exclusively to the planning system. However, recent studies suggest that automatic evaluations can be need-dependent (e.g., Seibt et al. Reference Seibt, Häfner and Deutsch2007).
In summary, we believe that there is evidence that relatively automatic appetitive processes in addiction should not be categorized together with subjective craving and explicit expectancies in the planning system. This does not imply that they are part of the habit system either. Perhaps these relatively automatic processes are an intermediate step between explicit processes in the planning system characteristic of drug use initiation and the reward independent associations characteristic of the habit system in late phases of addiction (cf. Everitt & Robbins Reference Everitt and Robbins2005).