Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T17:00:30.224Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The labeled line / basic taste versus across-fiber pattern debate: A red herring?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2008

Edward Alan Fox
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, Behavioral Neurogenetics Laboratory and Ingestive Behavior Research Center, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907. au_gc@psych.purdue.eduhttp://www.gradschool.purdue.edu/PULSe/faculty.cfm?fid=41&range=1
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Why has the labeled line versus across-fiber pattern debate of taste coding not been resolved? Erickson suggests that the basic tastes concept has no rational definition to test. Similarly, however, taste neuron types, which are fundamental to the across-fiber pattern concept, have not been formally defined, leaving this concept with no rational definition to test. Consequently, the two concepts are largely indistinguishable.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

The original conception of across-fiber patterns (AFPs) described by Erickson was based on Young's theory of color vision, which included the idea that the eye is provided with three distinct sets of broadly tuned receptors (Young Reference Young1802; Reference Young, Teevan and Birney1807). Although Erickson agreed that receptor or neuron types are similarly required for taste, he stated their existence has not been established, as they have not been formally defined. This failure to formally define taste cell types results in an AFP theory that is not testable, and therefore, AFPs suffer the same deficit Erickson ascribed to the concept of labeled lines / basic tastes (LLs). A serious consequence is that the LL and AFP models of taste coding are largely indistinguishable, creating confusion at both theoretical and empirical levels.

The relationship between the two models can be compared to alternating perceptions of a Necker cube, an analogy used famously by Dawkins (Reference Dawkins1976) to suggest that what appeared to be two different theories were actually only two different ways of looking at the same theory. For instance, LL and AFP information could be encoded within the same neural representation: the rich information contained in an AFP could be utilized by brain circuits that discriminate tastes, and in parallel, best-response neuron activity embedded within that AFP could be used to convey categorical information for circuits that make rapid yes/no decisions, such as an infant's acceptance of sweet tastes and rejection of bitter ones. In instances where virtually all of the AFP information is carried in the best-response neurons, the AFP and LL signaling are nearly identical – tuning this narrow may actually occur for sweet taste in primate primary taste fibers (Danilova et al. Reference Danilova, Danilov, Roberts, Tinti, Nofre and Hellekant2002). Similarly, Erickson suggested that neural approaches for yes/no circumstances could require only LLs in the form of an AFP reduced to a single neuron and that this possibility is consistent with the AFP model.

The lack of uniqueness of LLs and AFPs leaves open the possibility of interpreting any data as being for or against either model. This is illustrated here by reviewing the types of evidence that have typically been taken as support for the LL model, followed by a demonstration of how this evidence can be turned on its head to fit nicely into an AFP framework.

Groupings of taste stimuli into basic tastes using cluster analysis of neuron responses to multiple basic taste stimuli have been largely replicated across species and nervous system levels (e.g., Scott & Plata-Salaman Reference Scott and Plata-Salaman1999). Moreover, their validity has been strengthened by showing that conditioned taste aversions (CTAs) involving a basic taste generalize to taste stimuli in the same basic taste category, but not to those of other categories (e.g., Nowlis et al. Reference Nowlis, Frank and Pfaffmann1980). Erickson rightly argues that although these cluster analyses may separate neurons, they do not define types. Nevertheless, he suggests that if the neurons within one group responded similarly to manipulations of the taste system, and neurons of other groups responded differently, such evidence would favor neuron types (Scott & Giza Reference Scott and Giza2000).

Indeed, CTA, pharmacological, nutritional, and hormonal manipulations, especially of salt- and sweet-responsive neuron groups, including sodium deprivation, lingual amiloride or gymnemic acid application, deoxycorticosterone acetate (DOCA) priming followed by intracerebroventricular renin, and intravenous insulin or glucose, have provided such evidence (Chang & Scott Reference Chang and Scott1984; Giza & Scott Reference Giza and Scott1987a; Reference Giza and Scott1987b; Hellekant et al. Reference Hellekant, Ninomiya and Danilova1998; Jacobs et al. Reference Jacobs, Mark and Scott1988; McCaughey & Scott Reference McCaughey and Scott2000; Scott & Giza Reference Scott and Giza1990; Verhagen et al. Reference Verhagen, Giza and Scott2005). These findings are strengthened by functional-anatomical relationships of taste pathways that are consistent with LLs, including the innervation of single or small numbers of taste buds by primary taste neurons in some species, transynaptic neural tracing demonstrating differential projections of sweet and bitter receptors, differential localization of the activation of brainstem sensory neurons by bitter versus sweet or acidic stimuli, and unique regions of cortical activation associated with each basic taste (Accolla et al. Reference Accolla, Bathellier, Petersen and Carleton2007; Harrer & Travers Reference Harrer and Travers1996; Sugita & Shiba Reference Sugita and Shiba2005; Travers Reference Travers2002; Zaidi & Whitehead Reference Zaidi and Whitehead2006).

Mouse transgenics have also been used to selectively manipulate individual taste receptor cell types, and the results have been taken by some to be a hands-down win for LLs (Chandrashekar et al. Reference Chandrashekar, Hoon, Ryba and Zucker2006). Knockouts of sweet or bitter taste receptors and ablation of cells expressing sour receptors each resulted in loss of sensitivity to the respective basic taste without affecting responses to other basic tastes (Damak et al. Reference Damak, Rong, Yasumatsu, Kokrashvili, Varadarajan, Zou, Jiang, Ninomiya and Margolskee2003; Huang et al. Reference Huang, Chen, Hoon, Chandrashekar, Guo, Trankner, Ryba and Zuker2006; Zhao et al. Reference Zhao, Zhang, Hoon, Chandrashekar, Erlenbach, Ryba and Zuker2003). Consistent results have been obtained using genetic “rewiring” of taste pathways: Expression of a bitter receptor in taste cells normally expressing only sweet receptors resulted in “rewired” mutant mice, which readily ingested a bitter stimulus that was aversive to normal mice (Mueller et al. Reference Mueller, Hoon, Erlenbach, Chandrashekar, Zuker and Ryba2005).

Additionally, one of the biggest stumbling blocks for LLs has been data suggesting taste receptor cells are broadly tuned (e.g., Gilbertson et al. Reference Gilbertson, Boughter, Zhang and Smith2001). However, it has recently been shown that each taste receptor type occurs in a different population of taste receptor cells (Chandrashekar et al. Reference Chandrashekar, Hoon, Ryba and Zucker2006), and about 80% of Type II taste receptor cells, each of which contains only one type of taste receptor, respond to only one basic taste (Tomchik et al. Reference Tomchik, Berg, Kim, Chaudhari and Roper2007), suggesting that taste receptor typologies – potential substrates for LLs – exist.

In total then, the empirical data seem quite strong in favor of LLs. Why then hasn't the knockout punch to the AFP model been delivered? It could be argued that manipulation of a specific taste neuron, receptor, or receptor cell type would not only affect a LL, but could also have a specific effect on AFP coding. For example, in the genetically “rewired” mice described earlier, sweet-best neurons would become bitter-best neurons, and cells in the population that normally responded in any degree to sweet receptor activation would now respond in a similar degree to bitter receptor activation. Consequently, a bitter taste would now evoke the AFP normally activated by sweet taste. Therefore, as a result of the failure to develop formally sound definitions for AFP and LL models, this genetic manipulation, or, in fact, any selective manipulation, cannot distinguish these models.

References

Accolla, R., Bathellier, B., Petersen, C. C. & Carleton, A. (2007) Differential spatial representation of taste modalities in the rat gustatory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 27(6):13961404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandrashekar, J., Hoon, M. A., Ryba, J. P. & Zucker, C. S. (2006) The receptors and cells for mammalian taste. Nature 444:288–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, F. C. & Scott, T. R. (1984) Conditioned taste aversions modify neural responses in the rat nucleus tractus solitarius. Journal of Neuroscience 4(7):1850–62. Available at: http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/reprint/4/7/1850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damak, S., Rong, M., Yasumatsu, K., Kokrashvili, Z., Varadarajan, V., Zou, S., Jiang, P., Ninomiya, Y. & Margolskee, R. F. (2003) Detection of sweet and umami taste in the absence of taste receptor T1r3. Science 301(5634):850–53. Epub 2003 Jul 2017 available at: http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/301/5634/850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danilova, V., Danilov, Y., Roberts, T., Tinti, J. M., Nofre, C. & Hellekant, G. (2002) Sense of taste in a new world monkey, the common marmoset: Recordings from the chorda tympani and glossopharyngeal nerves. Journal of Neurophysiology 88(2):579–94. Available at: http://jn.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/88/2/579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976) Preface to 1989 edition. In: The selfish gene, 2nd edition, pp. viiixi. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gilbertson, T. A., Boughter, J. D., Zhang, H. & Smith, D. V. (2001) Distribution of gustatory sensitivities in rat taste cells: Whole-cell responses to apical chemical stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience 21:4931–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giza, B. K. & Scott, T. R. (1987a) Blood glucose level affects perceived sweetness intensity in rats. Physiology & Behavior 41(5):459–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giza, B. K. & Scott, T. R. (1987b) Intravenous insulin infusions in rats decrease gustatory-evoked responses to sugars. American Journal of Physiology 252(5, Pt 2): R9941002. Available at: http://ajpregu.physiology.org/cgi/reprint/252/5/R994Google ScholarPubMed
Harrer, M. I. & Travers, S. P. (1996) Topographic organization of Fos-like immunoreactivity in the rostral nucleus of the solitary tract evoked by gustatory stimulation with sucrose and quinine. Brain Research 711(1–2):125–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hellekant, G., Ninomiya, Y. & Danilova, V. (1998) Taste in chimpanzees. III: Labeled-line coding in sweet taste. Physiology & Behavior 65(2):191200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, A. L., Chen, X., Hoon, M. A., Chandrashekar, J., Guo, W., Trankner, D., Ryba, N. J. & Zuker, C. S. (2006) The cells and logic for mammalian sour taste detection. Nature 442(7105):934–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, K. M., Mark, G. P. & Scott, T. R. (1988) Taste responses in the nucleus tractus solitarius of sodium-deprived rats. Journal of Physiology 406:393410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCaughey, S. A. & Scott, T. R. (2000) Rapid induction of sodium appetite modifies taste-evoked activity in the rat nucleus of the solitary tract. American Journal of Physiology 279(3):R1121–31. Available at: http://ajpregu.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/279/3/R1121Google ScholarPubMed
Mueller, K. L., Hoon, M. A., Erlenbach, I., Chandrashekar, J., Zuker, C. S. & Ryba, N. J. (2005) The receptors and coding logic for bitter taste. Nature 434(7030):225–29. Available at: http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v434/n7030/abs/nature03352.htmlCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowlis, G. H., Frank, M. E. & Pfaffmann, C. (1980) Specificity of acquired aversions to taste qualities in hamsters and rats. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 94(5):932–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, T. R. & Giza, B. K. (1990) Coding channels in the taste system of the rat. Science 249(4976):1585–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, T. R. & Giza, B. K. (2000) Issues of gustatory neural coding: Where they stand today. Physiology & Behavior 69(1–2):6576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, T. R. & Plata-Salaman, C. R. (1999) Taste in the monkey cortex. Physiology & Behavior 67(4):489511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugita, M. & Shiba, Y. (2005) Genetic tracing shows segregation of taste neuronal circuitries for bitter and sweet. Science 309:781–85. Available at: http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/309/5735/781CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomchik, S. M., Berg, S., Kim, J. W., Chaudhari, N. & Roper, S. D. (2007) Breadth of tuning and taste coding in mammalian taste buds. Journal of Neuroscience 27:10840–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Travers, S. P. (2002) Quinine and citric acid elicit distinctive Fos-like immunoreactivity in the rat nucleus of the solitary tract. American Journal of Physiology 282(6):R17981810.Google ScholarPubMed
Verhagen, J. V., Giza, B. K. & Scott, T. R. (2005) Effect of amiloride on gustatory responses in the ventroposteromedial nucleus of the thalamus in rats. Journal of Neurophysiology 93(1):157–66. Available at: http://jn.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/93/1/157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, T. (1802) On the theory of light and colours. Philosophical Transactions, Royal Society of London 92:1248.Google Scholar
Young, T. (1807/1961) On physical optics. A course of lectures on natural philosophy and the mechanical arts, I. In: Color vision, ed. Teevan, R. C. & Birney, R. C.. Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Zaidi, F. N. & Whitehead, M. C. (2006) Discrete innervation of murine taste buds by peripheral taste neurons. Journal of Neuroscience 26(32):8243–53. Available at: http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/26/32/8243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, G. Q., Zhang, Y. F., Hoon, M. A., Chandrashekar, J., Erlenbach, I., Ryba, N. J. P. & Zuker, C. S. (2003) The receptors for mammalian sweet and umami taste. Cell 115:255–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed