Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T06:26:53.837Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Implicit analogy: New direct evidence and a challenge to the theory of memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2008

Anthony J. Greene
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI 53223. ag@uwm.eduhttp://www.uwm.edu/~ag
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The authors propose that analogical reasoning may be achieved without conscious or explicit deliberation. The argument would be strengthened by more convincingly demonstrating instances of analogy that do not require explicit deliberation. Recent findings demonstrate that deliberative or explicit strategies are not necessary for flexible expression under novel circumstances (Greene et al. 2001) to include analogical transfer (Gross & Greene 2007). This issue is particularly critical because the existence of relational priming poses a serious challenge to the widely held notion that flexible expression of learned relations requires deliberative processes.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Leech et al. make a compelling argument that analogical transfer may not require a deliberative or explicit mapping process. However, they provide no direct evidence showing successful implicit analogy. The analogy tasks reviewed in the target article do not provide evidence which rules out the use of deliberative strategies. In one such task the intent was to show that analogical transfer did not require a mapping of base to target (Ripoll et al. Reference Ripoll, Brude and Coulon2003). Although the finding tends to confirm that subjects perform analogical transfer without deliberative mapping, it does not rule out the possibility that analogical transfer was itself accomplished deliberately. Indeed, participants were all explicitly informed that their task was to complete an analogy. In another task, pre-exposure to a problem solution in the base task biased participants to employ a similar strategy in the target task (Schunn & Dunbar Reference Schunn and Dunbar1996). However, the only evidence that participants employed an implicit strategy was that they did not mention the base problem as a foundation for the solution to the target problem. Although these findings are both consistent with the use of implicit strategies, neither directly rules out the possibility that deliberative strategies were used.

Recently, direct evidence showed that analogical transfer occurs in the absence of deliberative strategies (Gross & Greene Reference Gross and Greene2007). Participants learned a transverse patterning sequence and transferred the learned relations to a novel stimulus set. For transverse patterning, given a choice between two faces, A and B, participants learn by trial and error to select A (A>B), and likewise they learn B>C and C>A. To test for analogical transfer, participants simultaneously learn a partial set of relations among different faces, X>Y and Y>Z, and are then tested on the novel pairing X?Z (the question mark indicated that no choice was ever reinforced). In the control condition, the training on the transverse patterning set is omitted, and participants uniformly infer that X>Z. However, with exposure to the transverse patterning set, a significant portion of participants apply the transverse patterning relations and select Z>X. This analogical transfer occurred without intent or awareness of the transfer. Two tests of awareness were used. First, a post-experimental questionnaire was employed with increasingly leading questions about the use of analogical transfer. Only those participants who asserted – in a forced-choice question – that no analogical reason existed for the Z>X choice were categorized as unaware. In addition, it was noted during the debriefing that many participants were surprised that they had not been trained on the Z?X choice. To test this, a recognition task revealed that most participants did not recognize Z?X as a novel configuration. Had a deliberative strategy been the foundation for analogical transfer, participants would have had to recognize the novel configuration to apply the mapping.

Analogical transfer is but one instance of relational priming. Evidence that complex relations can be learned implicitly is found in the contextual cueing task (Chun & Phelps Reference Chun and Phelps1999; Greene et al. Reference Greene, Gross, Elsinger and Rao2007) and the relational manipulation task (Ryan et al. Reference Ryan, Althoff, Whitlow and Cohen2000). Furthermore, evidence that implicit relational learning can be expressed under novelty is demonstrated by the transitive inference task (Ellenbogen et al. Reference Ellenbogen, Hu, Payne, Titone and Walker2007; Greene et al. Reference Greene, Spellman, Dusek, Eichenbaum and Levy2001; Reference Greene, Gross, Elsinger and Rao2006).

These additional relational priming tasks may also assess the semantic priming hypothesis put forth in the target article. The authors propose that relational priming is a facet of semantic priming (e.g., “chicken is to hen” may semantically prime the term “female” so that “horse is to mare” may be correctly selected), suggesting that implicit relational learning is verbally mediated. However, because several implicit relational learning tasks, including analogy, are nonverbal tasks, it is difficult to hypothesize that such verbal primes necessarily mediate performance, particularly in the spatial tasks (contextual cueing and relational manipulation). In fact, some evidence suggests that verbal strategies may bias the use of explicit strategies when implicit strategies could otherwise be employed (for a review, see Greene Reference Greene2007), suggesting that it may be more fruitful to explore the relational priming hypothesis using nonverbal stimuli.

The evidence that analogical transfer and other forms of relational priming can be accomplished implicitly forces a major change in the theory of memory. It has previously been asserted that only deliberative processes could support learning that is sufficiently flexible for abstract application under novelty (e.g., Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Poldrack and Eichenbaum1997; Reber et al. Reference Reber, Knowlton and Squire1996). It is now evident that such relational flexibility is not solely the domain of declarative memory (Chun & Phelps Reference Chun and Phelps1999; Ellenbogen et al. Reference Ellenbogen, Hu, Payne, Titone and Walker2007; Greene Reference Greene2007; Greene et al. Reference Greene, Spellman, Dusek, Eichenbaum and Levy2001; Reference Greene, Gross, Elsinger and Rao2006; Reference Greene, Gross, Elsinger and Rao2007; Gross & Greene Reference Gross and Greene2007; Ryan et al. Reference Ryan, Althoff, Whitlow and Cohen2000). The consequence of the discovery of such implicit tasks is that the declarative memory model (e.g., Squire Reference Squire1992) must be considered inadequate in its current form.

References

Chun, M. M. & Phelps, E. A. (1999) Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage. Nature Neuroscience 2:844–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, N. J., Poldrack, R. A. & Eichenbaum, H. (1997) Memory for items and memory for relations in the procedural/declarative memory framework. Memory 5:131–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellenbogen, J. M., Hu, P. T., Payne, J. D., Titone, D. & Walker, M. P. (2007) Human relational memory requires time and sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 104:7723–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, A. J. (2007) Human hippocampal-dependent tasks: Is awareness necessary or sufficient? Hippocampus 17:429–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, A. J., Gross, W. L., Elsinger, C. L. & Rao, S. M. (2006) An fMRI analysis of the human hippocampus: Inference, context and task awareness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18:1156–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, A. J., Gross, W. L., Elsinger, C. L. & Rao, S. M. (2007) Hippocampal differentiation without recognition: An fMRI analysis of the contextual cueing task. Learning and Memory 14:548–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, A. J., Spellman, B. A., Dusek, J. A., Eichenbaum, H. B. & Levy, W. B. (2001) Relational learning with and without awareness: Transitive inference using nonverbal stimuli in humans. Memory and Cognition 29:893902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, W. L. & Greene, A. J. (2007) Analogical inference: The role of awareness in abstract learning. Memory 15:838–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reber, P. J., Knowlton, B. J. & Squire, L. R. (1996) Dissociable properties of memory systems: Differences in the flexibility of declarative and nondeclarative knowledge. Behavioral Neuroscience 110:861–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ripoll, T., Brude, T. & Coulon, D. (2003) Does analogical transfer involve a term-to-term alignment? Memory and Cognition 31:221–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, J. D., Althoff, R. R., Whitlow, S. & Cohen, N. J. (2000) Amnesia is a deficit in relational memory. Psychological Science 11:454–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schunn, C. D. & Dunbar, K. (1996) Priming, analogy, and awareness in complex reasoning. Memory and Cognition 24:271–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1992) Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review 99:195231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed