Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T20:01:18.735Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Epidemiological foundations for the insurance hypothesis: Methodological considerations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Joseph M. Boden
Affiliation:
Christchurch Health and Development Study, University of Otago, Christchurch School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Christchurch, New Zealand. joseph.boden@otago.ac.nzgeraldine.mcleod@otago.ac.nz
Geraldine F. H. McLeod
Affiliation:
Christchurch Health and Development Study, University of Otago, Christchurch School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Christchurch, New Zealand. joseph.boden@otago.ac.nzgeraldine.mcleod@otago.ac.nz

Abstract

Nettle et al. evaluate evidence for the insurance hypothesis, which links obesity with the perception of food scarcity. Epidemiological findings in this area have generally been weak and inconsistent. The present commentary examines three key methodological issues arising from the literature on the association between obesity and the perception of food scarcity in humans, with suggestions for future epidemiological research.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

The target article by Nettle et al. evaluates the evidence for the insurance hypothesis, which posits that obesity in humans is linked to food insecurity, such that the presence of environmental cues that signal food shortage lead to an increase in fat storage. In environments where the presence of food shortage signals is chronic, this process may lead to increased levels of obesity in the population.

To examine this hypothesis, the authors present a review and meta-analysis of the epidemiological evidence of the linkage between food insecurity and obesity in humans. The analysis reveals a robust positive association, but one that is limited to females in high-income nations. The authors make some general comments concerning the limitations of the epidemiological research, but to fully evaluate the insurance hypothesis, it is critical to examine in detail these limitations of the meta-analysis and, in turn, to present an epidemiological approach that would be well equipped to examine this hypothesis.

The first critical limitation is that most of the studies conducted in this area have employed a cross-sectional design rather than a longitudinal design. Although cross-sectional designs allow for the ascertainment of associations and the development of hypotheses concerning the possible causal relationships between food insecurity and obesity, it is not possible to establish causality using studies of this nature. It should be noted that cross-sectional designs may be useful in determining the extent to which any associations between food insecurity and obesity generalize across populations, but these considerations are secondary in comparison to studies designed in a manner to test adequately a causal hypothesis.

A second critical limitation in the literature is that few of these studies have been able to adequately control for possible sources of confounding in the association between food insecurity and obesity. Nettle et al. report that their meta-analysis did not detect a difference between estimates of association that were adjusted for confounding (socioeconomic status) and those that were not, suggesting that the existing studies may have failed to sufficiently control for confounding. Yet, controlling for confounding is a key factor in the ascertainment of causality; without adequate control for confounding, it is impossible to evaluate whether food insecurity plays a causal role in increasing obesity. The use of longitudinal designs, with repeated measures of both the perception of food insecurity and obesity, would allow the fitting of conditional fixed-effects models (Allison Reference Allison2009; Hamerle & Ronning Reference Hamerle, Ronning, Arminger, Clogg and Sobel1995), which account for all sources of non-observed fixed confounding that influence both food insecurity perception and obesity and which can be augmented by observed time-dynamic co-variate factors during the period of observation. Given the availability of repeated measures data, fixed-effects modeling provides a robust indication of possible causality. Nettle et al. note (in sect. 5.3) that they were able to find only one study that had repeated measures of both food insecurity and obesity (Whitaker & Sarin Reference Whitaker and Sarin2007).

In addition, although the opposite causal pathway (obesity causing food insecurity) seems implausible, it may be plausible to suggest that individuals who are obese have different perceptions of food availability than individuals who are not, and that individuals who are obese are perhaps more likely to perceive food sources as insecure rather than secure. To ascertain the direction of causality and rule out the possibility that obesity could be driving an increase in the perception of food insecurity, repeated measures data could also be used to test the likely direction of causality using structural equation modeling procedures (Fergusson et al. Reference Fergusson, Boden and Horwood2007; Fergusson et al. Reference Fergusson, McLeod, Horwood, Swain, Chapple and Poulton2015) that compare the fit of models that represent (a) a causal pathway from perceptions of food insecurity to obesity, (b) a causal pathway from obesity to perceptions of food insecurity, and (c) a reciprocal causal pathway in which each plays a causal role in the other.

It is clear from the target article by Nettle et al. that an understanding of group differences plays an important role in our understanding of the role of food insecurity in the risk of obesity. However, a third critical limitation is that the studies reviewed have been unable to adequately test important group differences in the association between food insecurity and obesity. Nettle et al. report that their meta-analysis revealed evidence of gender differences, such that the association applied for females but not for males, but was unable to detect any differences related to age or other major group difference (such as ethnicity). Although it may be difficult to design a single study that can adequately test all plausible group differences in the association between food insecurity and obesity, the use of nested designs or multigroup analyses (Boden et al. Reference Boden, Sanders, Munford, Liebenberg and McLeod2015; Boden et al. Reference Boden, Sanders, Munford and Liebenberg2016; Fergusson et al. Reference Fergusson, Boden and Horwood2008; Muthén & Muthén Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2012) allows the fitting of models across groups and permits tests of the equivalence of model fit across these groups, using only a single model. Multiple tests of groups’ differences may be applied to a single model as well. The signal advantage of this modeling procedure is that it proves possible to directly compare parameter estimates across groups with the model without increasing standard error, thereby reducing model imprecision and increasing the sensitivity of the model to detect effects. A nested or multiple-group model approach can also be combined with the conditional fixed-effects and structural equation modeling approaches detailed above to provide a comprehensive approach to testing the robustness of the association to confounding, the direction of causality in the association, and the ascertainment of critical group differences in the association between the perception of food insecurity and obesity.

Nettle et al. provide a thorough review and analysis of the epidemiological literature concerning food insecurity and obesity. It is clear that, on the basis of this review, there are considerable methodological weaknesses that compromise the robustness of the observed associations. The application of a series of design improvements and modeling procedures would allow a much better understanding of the nature of these associations and provide further evidence to evaluate the insurance hypothesis.

References

Allison, P. D. (2009) Fixed effects regression models. Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, J. M., Sanders, J., Munford, R. & Liebenberg, L. (2016) The same but different? Applicability of a general resilience model to understand a population of vulnerable youth. Child Indicators Research 118. doi: 10.1007/s12187-016-9422-y.Google Scholar
Boden, J. M., Sanders, J., Munford, R., Liebenberg, L. & McLeod, G. F. H. (2015) Paths to positive development: A model of outcomes in the New Zealand youth transitions study. Child Indicators Research 9:123. doi: 10.1007/s12187-015-9341-3.Google Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M. & Horwood, L. J. (2007) Unemployment and suicidal behaviour in a New Zealand birth cohort: A fixed effects regression analysis. Crisis 28(2):95101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M. & Horwood, L. J. (2008) Developmental antecedents of interpartner violence in a New Zealand birth cohort. Journal of Family Violence 23:737–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., McLeod, G. F. H., Horwood, L. J., Swain, N. R., Chapple, S. & Poulton, R. (2015) Life satisfaction and mental health problems (18 to 35). Psychological Medicine 45(11):2427–36. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000422.Google Scholar
Hamerle, A. & Ronning, G. (1995) Panel analysis for qualitative variables. In: Handbook of statistical modeling for the social and behavioral sciences, ed. Arminger, G., Clogg, C. C. & Sobel, M. E., pp. 401–51. Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012) Mplus user's guide, 7th edition. Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Whitaker, R. C. & Sarin, A. (2007) Change in food security status and change in weight are not associated in urban women with preschool children. Journal of Nutrition 137(9):2134–39.Google Scholar