Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:38:07.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bonding system in nonhuman primates and biological roots of musicality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2021

Yuko Hattori*
Affiliation:
Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Center for International Collaboration and Advanced Studies in Primatology (CICASP), Kanrin, Inuyama, Aichi484-8506, Japan. hattori.yuko.8w@kyoto-u.ac.jp; http://www.cicasp.pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp/people/yuko-hattori

Abstract

Comparative studies of primates indicate that humans have evolved unique motivations and cognitive skills for sharing emotions, experiences, and collaborative actions. Given the characteristics of music, the music and social bonding (MSB) hypothesis by Savage et al. fits this view. Within a cross-species approach, predispositions not observed in current communication system may contribute to a better understanding of the biological roots of human musicality.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Living in groups involves a balance of costs and benefits and, especially for primates, this inevitably entails competition and cooperation with the other group members. If food resources are scarce and all individuals are competitive, splitting into smaller groups will be favored. However, if group members cooperate with each other, for example by hunting together, larger groups become advantageous. Despite the high costs of investing in bonding relationships with non-kin, one prominent characteristic of primate society is the large and complex group memberships including non-kin group members (Byrne & Whiten, Reference Byrne and Whiten1988; Harcourt & de Waal, Reference Harcourt and de Waal1992), and conducive social skills and bonding mechanisms have, therefore, evolved among primates (Van Schaik, Reference Van Schaik2016).

Social play, for example, generally follows a bell-shaped curve through ontogeny with a peak in the juvenile phase (Fagen, Reference Fagen, Pereira and Fairbanks1993); however, primates in particular continue to play as adults (Palagi, Reference Palagi2018). Some species such as great apes play frequently even when they become adults, and researchers have proposed that social play functions to share positive emotions (e.g., laughing) and reduce stress or tension (Palagi et al., Reference Palagi, Burghardt, Smuts, Cordoni, Dall'Olio, Fouts, Řeháková-Petrů, Siviy and Pellis2006a, Reference Palagi, Cordoni, Demuru and Bekoff2006b), effects that have parallels with music and dance. It is reported that the relative striatal volume is correlated with the rate of social (but not non-social) play behavior (Graham, Reference Graham2011). The striatum structure is intimately connected to dopaminergic pathways, and thus to the experience and anticipation of pleasure and reward, as well as to sequential behavior, motor control, and cognitive flexibility (Ashby, Turner, & Horvitz, Reference Ashby, Turner and Horvitz2010; Elliott, Newman, Longe, & Deakin, Reference Elliott, Newman, Longe and Deakin2003; Erikson et al., Reference Erikson, Boot, Basak, Neider, Prakesh, Voss, Graybiel, Simons, Fabiani, Gratton and Kramer2010; Vink et al., Reference Vink, Kahn, Raemaekers, van den Heuvel, Boersma and Ramsey2005). Given that social play, along with grooming, plays a crucial role in social bonding, which in turn may have fitness benefits (e.g., in chimpanzees, Muller & Mitani, Reference Muller and Mitani2005), selection may have favored a link between play and an enhanced ability to make sequential behaviors for rhythmic and repetitive joint actions with others, and experiencing pleasure and reward through those activities.

However, among primates, humans are extremely highly motivated to share psychological states with others (shared intentionality, Tomasello & Carpenter, Reference Tomasello and Carpenter2007). As “togetherness” or “jointness” is what researchers consider to distinguish cooperative or collaborative interactions between humans and nonhuman primates (Tomasello & Moll, Reference Tomasello and Moll2010), a bonding system such as music might have been selected for such purposes. Some research has led to the suggestion that coalition signaling did not evolve in most nonhuman primate societies because their bonding relationships are not strong enough, and so they succumb to the collective action problem (CAP) or social dilemma, when all individuals would benefit by cooperating but fail to do so because of conflicting interests that discourage joint action (Willems & Van Schaik, Reference Willems and Van Schaik2015). This suggests that credible signaling of coalition strength, size, and cooperation ability mentioned in Mehr et al. can evolve after group members have strongly bonded relationships.

Concerning the cross-species approach, the perception/production dissociation mentioned by Savage et al. should be possible to explore in nonhuman primates. Indeed, although imitation is relatively rare in nonhuman primates, they recognize when their actions are being imitated (Haun & Call, Reference Haun and Call2008; Paukner, Anderson, Borelli, Visalberghi, & Ferrari, Reference Paukner, Anderson, Borelli, VOsalberghi and Ferrari2005) and show more affiliative responses to those who recently imitated them (Paukner, Suomi, Visalberghi, & Ferrari, Reference Paukner, Suomi, Visalberghi and Ferrari2009). In vocal communication, some species such as Japanese macaques match acoustic features (i.e., frequency range) when they call back to other group members (Sugiura, Reference Sugiura1998). However, whether acoustic similarity increases bonding in relationships has not been rigorously tested. Additionally, I suggest that abilities or predispositions related to music-making but which are not components of the current communication system, should also be considered for understanding the biological roots of musicality. For example, chimpanzees, but not monkeys, seem to be intrinsically motivated to match their body movement (i.e., tapping) to auditory rhythms (Hattori, Tomonaga & Matsuzawa, Reference Hattori, Tomonaga and Matsuzawa2013; Reference Hattori, Tomonaga and Matsuzawa2015; Zarco, Merchant, Prado, & Mendez, Reference Zarco, Merchant, Prado and Mendez2009), although it is a rudimentary capacity and entrainment occurs only when the auditory tempo is close to the individual's spontaneous motor tempo. We also recently reported that rhythmic body movements such as swaying can be induced in chimpanzees (Hattori, Reference Hattori, Anderson and Kuroshimain press; Hattori & Tomonaga, Reference Hattori and Tomonaga2020a, Reference Hattori and Tomonagain press). A similar effect has been reported in the wild, in the form of chimpanzees' rhythmic displays and vigorous charges in reaction to rain or waterfalls (Goodall, Reference Goodall1986). Although neither the rain dance nor waterfall dance appears to be a joint activity involving multiple individuals, components of musicality may have arisen based on those predispositions in chimpanzees' and also humans' ancestors. As nonhuman animals do not themselves make music, any proto-musical behaviors have their own species-specific variations. Understanding such variations in relation to bonding systems is a laudable aim of further research in the comparative approach to the biology of music.

Financial support

This study was funded by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) 20H04490.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Ashby, F. G., Turner, B. O., & Horvitz, J. C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends in the Cognitive Sciences, 14, 208215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Uniquely expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Elliott, R., Newman, J. L., Longe, O. A., & Deakin, J. F. W. (2003). Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex in financial reward in humans: A parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 303307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erikson, K. I., Boot, W. R., Basak, C., Neider, M. B., Prakesh, R. S., Voss, M. W., Graybiel, A. M., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., & Kramer, A. F. (2010). Striatal volume predicts level of video game skill acquisition. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 4557.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. (1993). Primate juvenile and primate play. In Pereira, M. E. & Fairbanks, L. A. (Eds.), Juvenile primates (pp. 182196). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. In Pattern of behavior (p. 335). The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Graham, K. L. (2011). Coevolutionary relationship between striatum size and social play in nonhuman primates. American Journal of Primatology, 73(4), 314322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harcourt, A. H., & de Waal, F. B. M. (1992). Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hattori, Y. (in press). Behavioral coordination and synchronization in non-human primates. In Anderson, J.R. & Kuroshima, H. (Eds.), Comparative cognition: Commonalities and diversity. Springer.Google Scholar
Hattori, Y., Tomonaga, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2015). Distractor effect of auditory rhythms on self-paced tapping in chimpanzees and humans. PLoS ONE, 10, e0130682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hattori, Y., & Tomonaga, M. (2020a). Rhythmic swaying induced by sound in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(2), 936942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hattori, Y., & Tomonaga, M. (in press). Reply to Bertolo et al.: Rhythmic swaying in chimpanzees has implications for understanding the biological roots of human music and dance. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Hattori, Y., Tomonaga, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2013). Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Scientific Report, 3, 1566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., & Call, J. (2008). Imitation recognition in great apes. Current Biology, 18(7), R288R290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, M. N., & Mitani, J. C. (2005). Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 275331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E. (2018). Not just for fun! Social play as a springboard for adult social competence in human and non-human primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72(6), 90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., Burghardt, G. M., Smuts, B., Cordoni, G., Dall'Olio, S., Fouts, H. N., Řeháková-Petrů, M., Siviy, S. M., & Pellis, S. M. (2016a). Rough- and tumble play as a window on animal communication. Biological Reviews, 91, 311327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., Cordoni, G., Demuru, E., & Bekoff, M. (2016b). Fair play and its connection with social tolerance, reciprocity and the ethology of peace. Behaviour, 153, 11951216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paukner, A., Anderson, J. R., Borelli, E., VOsalberghi, E., & Ferrari, P. F. (2005). Macaques (Macaca nemestrina) recognize when they are being imitated. Biology Letters, 1(2), 219222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paukner, A., Suomi, S. J., Visalberghi, E., & Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. Science, 325(5942), 880883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiura, H. (1998). Matching of acoustic features during the vocal exchange of coo calls by Japanese macaques. Animal Behaviour, 55, 673687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. Springer.Google Scholar
Van Schaik, CP. (2016). The primate origins of human nature. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Vink, M., Kahn, R. S., Raemaekers, M., van den Heuvel, M., Boersma, M., & Ramsey, N. F. (2005). Function of striatum beyond inhibition and execution of motor responses. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 336344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willems, E. P., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Collective action and the intensity of between-group competition in non-human primates. Behavioral Ecology, 26(2), 625631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarco, W., Merchant, H., Prado, L., & Mendez, J. C. (2009). Subsecond timing in primates: Comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102, 31913202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed