Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T19:40:20.031Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Becoming an expert: Ontogeny of expertise as an example of neural reuse

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 June 2016

Alessandro Guida
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Université Rennes 2, Rennes, 35043 Rennes Cedex, France. alessandro.guida@univ-rennes2.frhttp://alessandro-guida.blogspot.fr/
Guillermo Campitelli
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Social Science, Edith Cowan University, Perth WA6027, Australia. g.campitelli@ecu.edu.auhttp://gcampitelli.com
Fernand Gobet
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L69 7ZA, United Kingdom. fernand.gobet@liverpool.ac.ukhttp://www.chrest.info/fg/home.htm

Abstract

In this commentary, we discuss an important pattern of results in the literature on the neural basis of expertise: (a) decrease of cerebral activation at the beginning of acquisition of expertise and (b) functional cerebral reorganization as a consequence of years of practice. We show how these two results can be integrated with the neural reuse framework.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

In After Phrenology, Anderson (Reference Anderson2014) presented the neural reuse framework, which opposes both modular and holistic views of brain architecture. In evolutionary terms, neural reuse claims that the brain evolves, not by adding new specialized modules, but by acquiring new functions by recombining local areas in new ways. Ontogenetically, neural reuse involves the processes of interactive differentiation (local areas have different profiles and interact in different ways as a function of development) and neural search (the active testing of multiple neuronal combinations until finding the most appropriate one for a specific skill, i.e., the neural niche of that skill).

One testable hypothesis of the neural reuse framework is that novices in a domain of expertise show widespread brain activation when performing a domain-specific task, whereas experts show a more focused pattern of brain activity. This is because, at the beginning of acquiring a skill, the brain is searching for an appropriate combination of areas, whereas the experts' brains have already settled in a specific network of brain areas to perform domain-specific tasks. Indeed, Anderson (Reference Anderson2014) presents data supporting this hypothesis (e.g., Merabet et al. Reference Merabet, Hamilton, Schlaug, Swisher, Kiriakapoulos, Pitskel, Kauffman and Pascual-Leone2008; Petersen et al. Reference Petersen, van Mier, Fiez and Raichle1998; Petersson et al. Reference Petersson, Elfgren and Ingvar1997; Poldrack et al. Reference Poldrack, Desmond, Glover and Gabrieli1998).

The field of research on the neural implementation of expertise has been very prolific in the last 15 years; therefore, it seems pertinent to evaluate Anderson's hypothesis exhaustively in light of new data. In fact, Guida et al. (Reference Guida, Gobet, Tardieu and Nicolas2012; see also Guida et al. Reference Guida, Gobet and Nicolas2013) reviewed the literature on neural implementation of expertise in tasks related to working memory. They found two effects: (a) studies investigating individuals who receive training in working memory-related tasks (from two hours up to five weeks) show mainly a decrease of cerebral activity in prefrontal and parietal working memory areas after training, whereas (b) studies using experts and novices in different fields performing domain-specific working memory-related tasks tend to show that the brain areas activated to perform those tasks differ between novices and experts (Guida et al. [2012] referred to this effect as functional neural reorganization). As suggested by Anderson (Reference Anderson2014), the first effect (i.e., the reduction of brain activity as a result of a number of hours of training) is consistent with the process of neural search. Even though the second effect was not envisaged by Anderson (Reference Anderson2014), we propose that it is also compatible with neural search. The first effect reflects the fact that a developed skill finds a neural niche within the network of brain areas used at the beginning of skill acquisition, whereas functional neural reorganization reflects a more radical type of neural search: the skill finds its neural niche in a different set of brain areas. In the rest of this commentary, we explain in more detail these two effects, which are connected by Guida and colleagues (Guida et al. Reference Guida, Gobet, Tardieu and Nicolas2012; Reference Guida, Gobet and Nicolas2013) in a two-stage framework, and link them to the three implications of the neural reuse framework that Anderson put forward in his précis (sect. 1, para. 3).

Experts: Reusing the mediotemporal lobe

(1) “First and most obvious, newly acquired capacities are generally supported by mixing and matching the same neural elements in new ways” (sect. 1, para. 3). This first implication is in accordance with the expertise literature. As a consequence of their extended practice, experts develop domain-specific knowledge structures (i.e., chunks and more sophisticated knowledge structures; see an explanation in the next section). These new knowledge structures allow experts to reuse the mediotemporal lobe in a completely different way compared to novices (Campitelli et al. Reference Campitelli, Gobet, Head, Buckley and Parker2007; Guida et al. Reference Guida, Gobet, Tardieu and Nicolas2012; Reference Guida, Gobet and Nicolas2013). Whereas novices typically use episodic long-term memory areas (e.g., the mediotemporal lobe) for performing long-term memory tasks, experts are able to (re)use these areas also for performing working-memory tasks.

As theorized by Ericsson and Kintsch (Reference Ericsson and Kintsch1995) and Gobet and Simon (Reference Gobet and Simon1996), this reuse occurs when there is a tight connection between working memory and long-term memory through retrieval cues or slotted schemas, which allow a fast transfer of information between these two types of memory (see also Gobet Reference Gobet2000a; Reference Gobet2000b). Therefore, the interaction between working memory and long-term memory is crucial for functional neural reorganization to take place. This certainly echoes Anderson's (Reference Anderson2014, p. 40) view that “function depends much more on the interactions between parts than on the actions of parts,” and relates to Anderson's second principle of a functionalist neuroscience: “Our complex and diverse behavioral repertoire is supported primarily by the brain's ability to dynamically establish multiple different functional coalitions” (2014, p. 296).

The relation between working memory and episodic long-term memory also relates to another interesting effect described by Anderson: unmasking. The basic idea is that brain regions are supposed to be specialized to process one type of input (e.g., the occipital cortex is supposed to be specialized in processing visual input). However, under special conditions in which the source of dominant input is disrupted (e.g., injury, sensory deprivation), this brain area can process a different type of input, unmasking a new processing capacity for this area. Moreover, Anderson argues that the disruption of normal input is not a necessary condition and that unmasking can be observed also under more typical conditions. The development of expertise, as postulated by the two-stage framework, offers a good example of this. It is indeed possible that the decrease of activity in the first stage may help the unmasking and thus reuse of the mediotemporal lobe, allowing the occurrence of the second stage. We also agree with Anderson when he proposes that unmasking must not be seen as passive. From our point of view, functional neural reorganization occurs through the use of knowledge structures. The efficiency of these structures is a necessary condition. However, the biological reasons that undergird such processes are unknown.

Experts: Re-using spatial processes

(2) “Second, and perhaps less obvious, neural reuse would appear to support and encourage procedural and behavioral reuse” (sect. 1, para. 3). Guida et al. (Reference Guida, Gobet, Tardieu and Nicolas2012) provided an explanation of the cognitive processes that both cause and are the consequence of the two identified patterns of brain activity in expertise studies. The first stage – decrease of cerebral activity – has been linked to chunking (Chase & Simon Reference Chase and Simon1973; Cowan Reference Cowan2001; Gobet et al. Reference Gobet, Lane, Croker, Cheng, Jones, Oliver and Pine2001). When practice begins, individuals start binding various domain-specific patterns (e.g., in chess, configurations of pieces) together, which ultimately result in a compression (Mathy & Feldman Reference Mathy and Feldman2012) of the elements into one structure, a chunk. Once chunks are built, separate domain-specific patterns can be processed as one element, which means that less cognitive resources are needed, and this is reflected as a reduction of brain activation to perform a domain-specific task. As a consequence of practice and expertise, chunks grow in size (e.g., Cowan et al. Reference Cowan, Chen and Rouder2004; Chen & Cowan Reference Chen and Cowan2005) and complexity (e.g., Chase & Simon Reference Chase and Simon1973; Gobet & Simon Reference Gobet and Simon1996), and with years of training, they become high hierarchical chunks: knowledge structures. These structures (Ericsson & Kintsch Reference Ericsson and Kintsch1995; Gobet & Simon Reference Gobet and Simon1996) allow experts to encode information in episodic long-term memory in a fast and reliable fashion even in conditions typical of working-memory tasks (rapid presentation of several elements), which is not possible for novices using similar cell assemblies in the mediotemporal lobe.

As pointed out above, to be able to use episodic long-term memory in a fast and reliable fashion, individuals must develop specific knowledge structures. This illustrates how human beings, “repurpose our behavioral routines in multiple circumstances for myriad cognitive ends” (sect. 7, para. 3). A well-known example in the domain of expertise is the method of loci, which is thought to be the first (internal) mnemonic (Worthen & Hunt Reference Worthen and Hunt2011; Yates Reference Yates1966), initially proposed by Simonides of Ceos more than two millennia ago. In ancient Greece, orators would visualize a sequence of familiar locations (in their house or a familiar route with salient locations) before a speech and use them to mentally store important words. Subsequently, during their speech, they would take a mental tour and retrieve each word through the familiar locations. This technique is still in use among expert mnemonists (Pridmore Reference Pridmore2013). Maguire et al. (Reference Maguire, Valentine, Wilding and Kapur2003) revealed the functional cerebral pattern of these mental walks by comparing mnemonists with all-comers. They found comparable activations in both groups in prefrontal working memory areas, but specific activations for the mnemonists, in the left medial superior parietal cortex, in the bilateral retrosplenial cortex, and in the right posterior hippocampus (for a similar pattern, see also Pesenti et al. Reference Pesenti, Zago, Crivello, Mellet, Samson, Duroux, Seron, Mazoyer and Tzourio-Mazoyer2001); these regions are important for episodic memory and crucial for spatial memory and navigation (e.g., Burgess et al. Reference Burgess, Maguire and O'Keefe2002). Therefore, it seems that with hours of training, mnemonists are capable of using the mental image of their house (or of a route) as a slotted schema and transfer the incoming information from working memory to long-term memory by associating the new information with each slot (the familiar locations), instead of simply storing the information in working memory. The consequence of the use of the method of loci is an increased memory capacity and cerebral functional reorganization.

This example clearly shows how experts reuse spatial cognitive processes to encode verbal information. These elements are linked to section 7 of Anderson's précis, “Reuse, interaction, and ‘higher-order’ cognition,” and perfectly illustrate the fact that “we have found ways to reuse our physical capacities to augment our mental ones; in a process supported by neural reuse” (sect. 7, para. 3). Interestingly, a similar process has also been found in “all-comers.” Van Dijck and Fias (Reference van Dijck and Fias2011, see also Guida et al. Reference Guida, Leroux, Lavielle-Guida and Noël2015) showed that verbal information processed in immediate memory was mentally organized from left to right based on the order of presentation. This suggests that order in working memory could be coded through spatial positional tagging (Abrahamse et al. Reference Abrahamse, van Dijck, Majerus and Fias2014; Guida & Lavielle-Guida Reference Guida and Lavielle-Guida2014). Based on this idea, Guida and Lavielle-Guida (Reference Guida and Lavielle-Guida2014) proposed that spatial positional tags in all-comers were comparable to the spatial locations of expert mnemonists, proposing the generic term of spatialization. The final twist that links all-comers to expertise is that the left to right spatial positional tagging observed in all-comers could be the result of expertise in reading and writing.

Experts versus novices: Same working-memory tasks but different processes and cerebral substratum

(3) “The third implication … not every cognitive achievement … need be supported by a specific targeted adaptation” (sect. 1, para. 3). As emphasized by Anderson, the last implication follows neatly from the previous points. Indeed, we believe that the assemblies of neurons that code for location did not evolve for encoding and retrieving words like in the method of loci. This example enters in a much wider picture when taking into account working memory. It is well established that prefronto-parietal areas are crucial for working memory in all-comers (Cowan Reference Cowan2011; Postle et al. Reference Postle, Berger and D'Esposito1999; Postle & D'Esposito Reference Postle and D'Esposito1999; Todd & Marois Reference Todd and Marois2004; Vogel & Machizawa Reference Vogel and Machizawa2004). However, as highlighted above (see also Guida et al. Reference Guida, Gobet, Tardieu and Nicolas2012; Reference Guida, Gobet and Nicolas2013), when experts execute working memory related-tasks within their domain of expertise, completely different brains areas are activated (e.g., the mediotemporal lobe). In this case, expertise via new assemblies of cells allows experts to circumvent the limits of working memory by using a part of episodic long-term memory. This shows that a same cognitive achievement (here, working memory task) needs not to be supported by a specific targeted adaptation.

Conclusion

The neural reuse framework proposes that the same assemblies of cells can be used for different cognitive functions or tasks in different contexts. Research into the neural implementations of expertise supports Anderson's (Reference Anderson2014) hypothesis that brain activity decreases and becomes more focalized when a skill is learned. However, another pattern of results in the expertise literature was not envisaged by Anderson – a change of the set of networks used to perform a working-memory task as a function of expertise. We presented an explanation of how this effect can be explained by the concept of neural reuse.

References

Abrahamse, E., van Dijck, J.-P., Majerus, S. & Fias, W. (2014) Finding the answer in space: The mental whiteboard hypothesis on serial order in working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M. L. (2014) After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A. & O'Keefe, J. (2002) The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron 35:625–41.Google Scholar
Campitelli, G., Gobet, F., Head, K., Buckley, M. & Parker, A. (2007) Brain localisation of memory chunks in chessplayers. International Journal of Neuroscience 117:1641–59.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G. & Simon, H. A. (1973) Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology 4:5581.Google Scholar
Chen, Z. & Cowan, N. (2005) Chunk limits and length limits in immediate recall: A reconciliation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31:1235–49.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2001) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24:87185.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2011) The focus of attention as observed in visual working memory tasks: Making sense of competing claims. Neuropsychologia 49:1401–406.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., Chen, Z. & Rouder, J. N. (2004) Constant capacity in an immediate serial-recall task: A logical sequel to Miller (1956). Psychological Science 15:634–40.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. & Kintsch, W. (1995) Long-term working memory. Psychological Review 102:211–45.Google Scholar
Gobet, F. (2000a) Retrieval structures and schemata: A brief reply to Ericsson and Kintsch. British Journal of Psychology 91:591–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gobet, F. (2000b) Some shortcomings of long-term working memory. British Journal of Psychology 91:551–70.Google Scholar
Gobet, F., Lane, P. C. R., Croker, S. C. H., Cheng, P., Jones, G., Oliver, I. & Pine, J. M. (2001) Chunking mechanisms in human learning. Trends in Cognitive Science 5:236–43.Google Scholar
Gobet, F. & Simon, H. A. (1996) Templates in chess memory: A mechanism for recalling several boards. Cognitive Psychology 31:140.Google Scholar
Guida, A., Gobet, F. & Nicolas, S. (2013) Functional cerebral reorganization: A signature of expertise? Reexamining Guida, Gobet, Tardieu, and Nicolas' (2012) two-stage framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7:590.Google Scholar
Guida, A., Gobet, F., Tardieu, H. & Nicolas, S. (2012) How chunks, long-term working memory and templates offer a cognitive explanation for neuroimaging data on expertise acquisition: A two-stage framework. Brain and Cognition 79:221–44.Google Scholar
Guida, A. & Lavielle-Guida, M. (2014) 2011 space odyssey: Spatialization as a mechanism to code order allows a close encounter between memory expertise and classic immediate memory studies. Frontiers in Psychology 5:573.Google Scholar
Guida, A., Leroux, A., Lavielle-Guida, M. & Noël, Y. (2015) A SPoARC in the dark: Spatialization in verbal immediate memory. Cognitive Science. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12316.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M. & Kapur, N. (2003) Routes to remembering: The brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience 6:9095.Google Scholar
Mathy, F. & Feldman, J. (2012) What's magic about magic numbers. Chunking and data compression in short-term memory. Cognition 122:346–62.Google Scholar
Merabet, L. B., Hamilton, R., Schlaug, G., Swisher, J. D., Kiriakapoulos, E. T., Pitskel, N. B., Kauffman, T. & Pascual-Leone, A. (2008) Rapid and reversible recruitment of early visual cortex for touch. PLoS One 3(8):e3046: 112.Google Scholar
Pesenti, M., Zago, L., Crivello, F., Mellet, E., Samson, D., Duroux, B., Seron, X., Mazoyer, B. & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2001) Mental calculation in a prodigy is sustained by right prefrontal and medial temporal areas. Nature Neuroscience 4:103107.Google Scholar
Petersen, S. E., van Mier, H., Fiez, J. A. & Raichle, M. E. (1998) The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task-performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95:853–60.Google Scholar
Petersson, K. M., Elfgren, C. & Ingvar, M. (1997) A dynamic role of the medial temporal lobe during retrieval of declarative memory in man. NeuroImage 6:111.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H. & Gabrieli, J. D. (1998) The neural basis of visual skill learning: An fMRI study of mirror reading. Cerebral Cortex 8:110.Google Scholar
Postle, B. R., Berger, J. S. & D'Esposito, M. (1999) Functional neuroanatomical double dissociation of mnemonic and executive control processes contributing to working memory performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 96:12959–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postle, B. R. & D'Esposito, M. (1999) “What”-then-“Where” in visual working memory: An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11:585–97.Google Scholar
Pridmore, B. (2013) How to be clever. Lulu Press.Google Scholar
Todd, J. J. & Marois, R. (2004) Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature 428:751–54.Google Scholar
van Dijck, J. P. & Fias, W. (2011) A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition 119:114–19.Google Scholar
Vogel, E. K. & Machizawa, M. G. (2004) Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature 428:748–51.Google Scholar
Worthen, J. B. & Hunt, R. R. (2011) Mnemonology: Mnemonics for the 21st century. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Yates, F. A. (1966) The art of memory. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar