Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-mggfc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-20T16:00:55.089Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

ARACHNE'S WEB: WOMEN, WEAVING AND NETWORKS OF KNOWLEDGE IN THE BRONZE AGE SOUTHERN AEGEAN

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 September 2019

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The appearance of loom weights at a number of southern Aegean sites in the Middle and early Late Bronze Age is indicative of the adoption of a new weaving technology: the use of the warp-weighted loom. The specific type of loom weight (discoid) recovered is a Cretan form, and this evidence of Cretan influence is also seen in a wider range of material culture features at these settlements during this period. Weaving is a complex skill and learning requires contact between novice and expert practitioner over an extended period of time; the introduction of a new weaving technology therefore raises the question of how the necessary technical knowledge and know-how was transferred from one individual or community to another. The archaeological indicators of this new technological practice, the loom weights themselves, are objects that very rarely travel, except with their owners; the presence of loom weights manufactured from non-local ceramic fabrics at some of the southern Aegean sites can therefore provide a window into the patterns of mobility through which the new technology is likely to have spread. Both in the Bronze Age and subsequent Archaic and Classical periods, weaving was closely associated with women. Loom weights thus constitute archaeological markers for the craftswomen who used them. This paper explores the insight they can offer into female networks of teaching, learning and craft practice in the second millennium bc.

Ο ιστός της αράχνης: οι γυναίκες, η υφαντική και τα γνωστικά δίκτυα την εποχή του Χαλκού στο Νότιο Αιγαίο

Η εμφάνιση των υφαντικών βαρών σε αρκετά μέρη στο νότιο Αιγαίο την περίοδο της Μέσης και αρχές της Ύστερης εποχής του Χαλκού είναι ενδεικτικό της υιοθέτησης μίας νέας υφαντικής τεχνολογίας: δηλαδή, της χρήσης του όρθιου αργαλειού. Ο συγκεκριμένος τύπος υφαντικού βάρους (δισκοειδής) που ανακαλύφθηκε, είναι Κρητικού τύπου και η ανακάλυψη της Κρητικής επιρροής είναι επίσης εμφανής σε ένα ευρύ φάσμα χαρακτηριστικών των υλικών καταλοίπων στους οικισμούς κατά τη διάρκεια αυτής της περιόδου. Η υφαντική αποτελεί μία πολύπλοκη δεξιοτεχνία και η εκμάθηση της απαιτεί την στενή συνεργασία μεταξύ των αρχάριων και των επαγγελματιών για μεγάλο χρονικό διάστημα. Έτσι, η εισαγωγή μίας νέας υφαντικής τεχνολογίας θέτει το ερώτημα πώς η απαραίτητη τεχνική γνώση και η τεχνογνωσία μεταφερόταν από το ένα άτομο στο άλλο ή από την μία κοινότητα στην άλλη. Οι αρχαιολογικές ενδείξεις αυτής της νέας τεχνολογικής πρακτικής, τα ίδια δηλαδή υφαντικά βάρη, είναι αντικείμενα που σπάνια ταξιδεύουν, εκτός αν τα μεταφέρουν οι ιδιοκτήτες τους. Η ύπαρξη αγνύθων που παράγονται από μη τοπικά κεραμικά υλικά σε ορισμένες από τις περιοχές του νότιου Αιγαίου μπορεί ως εκ τούτου να αποτελούν ένα «παράθυρο» στα πρότυπα της κινητικότητας μέσω της οποίας η νέα τεχνολογία είναι πιθανό να έχει εξαπλωθεί. Τόσο στην Εποχή του Χαλκού όσο και στις επόμενες Αρχαϊκές και Κλασικές περιόδους, η ύφανση συνδέεται στενά με τις γυναίκες. Επομένως, τα υφαντικά βάρη αποτελούν αρχαιολογικά στοιχεία για τις τεχνίτριες που τα χρησιμοποίησαν. Αυτό το άρθρο διερευνά τις γνώσεις που μπορούν να προσφέρουν στα δίκτυα γυναικείας διδασκαλίας, εκμάθησης και χειροτεχνίας στη δεύτερη χιλιετία π.Χ.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Council, British School at Athens 2019 

INTRODUCTION

The appearance of loom weights at a number of southern Aegean sites in the Middle and early Late Bronze Age is indicative of the adoption of a new weaving technology: the use of the warp-weighted loom. The specific type of loom weight (discoid) recovered is an earlier Cretan form, and this evidence of Cretan influence corresponds with the adoption of a wider range of Cretan and Cretan-style material culture features at these settlements, a phenomenon known as ‘Minoanisation’ (Broodbank Reference Broodbank2004; Davis Reference Davis and Shelmerdine2008; Hägg and Marinatos Reference Hägg and Marinatos1984; Macdonald, Hallager and Niemeier Reference Macdonald, Hallager and Niemeier2009; Gorogianni, Pavúk and Girella Reference Gorogianni, Pavúk and Girella2016).

The transmission of weaving skills requires contact between novice and expert practitioners over an extended period of time, and the introduction of a new weaving technology therefore raises the question of how the necessary technical knowledge and know-how were transferred from one individual/community to another. By examining how technological skills are learnt and considering possible mechanisms for their transmission, this article explores the processes through which Cretan weaving techniques may have been adopted abroad.

TEXTILES AND LOOM TECHNOLOGY IN THE BRONZE AGE SOUTHERN AEGEAN

In the Bronze Age Aegean, as in many other regions and time periods, textiles would have constituted a significant component of the material and visual environment. The demand for cloth would have been considerable; textiles were needed for a wide variety of purposes, such as clothing, bedding, wall-hangings, canopies, coverings for furniture and floors, sails, bags, sacks and wrappings. In contrast to modern, industrialised societies in which individuals engage with cloth as consumers rather than as producers (with textiles largely having come to be taken for granted as a result), most of the textiles used in pre-industrial societies were produced within a domestic context. The textile production process, from the acquisition and preparation of the raw material to the finished fabric, is highly labour intensive and involves a number of separate stages (see overview in Andersson Strand Reference Andersson Strand, Andersson Strand and Nosch2015); meeting the need for cloth within Aegean Bronze Age communities would have required a substantial investment of time, as well as specialised craft knowledge.

Cloth can be woven on a number of different types of loom. In the Bronze Age eastern Mediterranean, there is evidence for the use of three loom types: the horizontal ground loom, the vertical two-beam loom and the warp-weighted loom (Barber Reference Barber1991, 83–116). The looms themselves rarely leave any archaeological trace, since they were made of wood. However, on the warp-weighted loom, loom weights (usually made of clay) were used to apply tension to the warp threads, which hung from the upper beam of the loom (Fig. 1). The presence of loom weights therefore indicates the use of this particular type of loom. On Crete and the Greek mainland the warp-weighted loom was already in use in the Neolithic period (Barber Reference Barber1991, 99–100; Carington Smith Reference Carington Smith1975, 122–6, 135–8, 154–9; Andersson Strand and Nosch Reference Andersson Strand, Nosch, Andersson Strand and Nosch2015, 362). However, on current evidence, it seems that this loom type went out of use in the southern part of the Greek mainland and the islands at the end of the Early Bronze Age, and did not reappear on the mainland until the Late Bronze Age (Carington Smith Reference Carington Smith, McDonald and Wilkie1992, 687–9). Sufficient Middle Bronze Age domestic deposits have been excavated in this region to indicate this absence is not simply due to non-recovery of relevant evidence. On Crete, the warp-weighted loom continued to be used throughout the Bronze Age.

Fig. 1. The warp-weighted loom. Drawing: Annika Jeppsson © Annika Jeppsson and the Centre for Textile Research, University of Copenhagen.

THE APPEARANCE OF DISCOID LOOM WEIGHTS AT SOUTHERN AEGEAN SETTLEMENTS

A number of different loom weight types have been recovered from Bronze Age settlements on Crete, among them a discoid form which often has a flattened or a grooved top (Fig. 2). This type of loom weight was in use on the island from the third millennium bc. For example, discoid loom weights were present in Early Minoan (EM) II contexts (c.2650–2200 bc) at the site of Myrtos, Fournou Korifi on the south coast of Crete (Warren Reference Warren1972). During the first half of the second millennium bc, loom weights of this type also began to appear at a number of sites on the southern Aegean islands and along the south-west Anatolian coast.

Fig. 2. Examples of discoid loom weights: a) discoid rounded; b) discoid elliptical. Photographs: J. Cutler.

In the Cyclades, discoid loom weights have been recovered from Ayia Irini on Kea, Phylakopi on Melos, Akrotiri on Thera and Mikre Vigla on Naxos, as well as Kolonna on Aegina and the islands of Kythera and Antikythera further to the west. In the south-east Aegean, this type of loom weight has been found at Serraglio on Kos, Trianda and other locations on Rhodes, the Vathy Cave on Kalymnos, the Heraion on Samos and Emporio on Chios, and on the island of Karpathos. Along the south-west Anatolian littoral, discoid weights have been recorded at Miletus, Iasos, Teichiussa, Çeşme-Bağlararasi and Liman Tepe, as well as Bakla Tepe on the Cumaovasi plain. They have also been recovered further to the north, at Mikro Vouni on Samothrace and Koukonisi on Lemnos (Fig. 3; Cutler Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012; Reference Cutlerforthcoming). The first appearance of discoid loom weights at these sites varies markedly, from the start of the Middle Bronze Age to early in the Late Bronze Age, a degree of variation comparable to that for the adoption of different technological and stylistic characteristics of Cretan pottery in the wider southern Aegean (Nikolakopoulou and Knappett Reference Nikolakopoulou, Knappett, Kiriatzi and Knappett2016; Abell and Hilditch Reference Abell, Hilditch, Gorogianni, Pavúk and Girella2016), suggesting that the processes of adoption and adaptation relevant to each craft were determined individually by each community (Cutler Reference Cutler2011; Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012; Reference Cutler, Gorogianni, Pavúk and Girella2016). However, some differences in the presently documented chronologies of adoption of loom weights also reflect the degree of investigation of Middle Bronze Age strata at each site.

Fig. 3. Map of the Aegean, showing the main locations mentioned in the text. Drawing: J. Cutler, T. Whitelaw.

The discoid form of loom weight is the only type present at most of these southern Aegean settlements. The absence of loom weights of any type in earlier levels at the majority of these sites further indicates that, in most cases, the appearance of the discoid weights represents the introduction of the warp-weighted loom, and therefore the adoption of a new weaving technology. Prior to the adoption of the warp-weighted loom, another type of loom is likely to have been used. It has been suggested that longitudinally pierced ‘spools’ present in Middle Bronze Age contexts at sites on the Greek mainland, as well as at Ayia Irini on Kea and Phylakopi on Melos, may be associated with setting the warp on the horizontal ground loom, although this identification is uncertain (Carington Smith Reference Carington Smith1975, 404–10; Davis Reference Davis, Hägg and Marinatos1984, 63; Pavúk Reference Pavúk, Nosch and Laffineur2012, pl. XXIVa). Experimental research indicates that spools could have been used as loom weights (Olofsson, Andersson Strand and Nosch Reference Olofsson, Andersson Strand, Nosch, Strand and and Nosch2015, 92–5), but the specific shapes and weights of the Cycladic examples have not been analysed for their suitability. If used as loom weights, they were employed alongside discoid loom weights at Ayia Irini and Phylakopi (Cutler Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012), though not at Akrotiri on Thera (Vakirtzi Reference Vakirtzi and Nikolakopoulou2019, 500).

LEARNING TECHNICAL SKILLS: COMMUNITIES OF PRACTICE AND EMBODIED TECHNIQUES

The adoption of a new technology involving new knowledge and practices, such as weaving on the warp-weighted loom, necessitates the acquisition of new technical skills. Contemporary approaches to the transmission of technical practices emphasise that skills are learnt in particular cultural contexts (Dietler and Herbich Reference Dietler, Herbich, Binder and Courtin1994; Gosselain Reference Gosselain and Stark1998; Minar and Crown Reference Minar and Crown2001; Stark, Bowser and Horne Reference Stark, Bowser and Horne2008; Wendrich Reference Wendrich2012a). Craft practitioners who share a common body of knowledge and the same technical practices and attendant social relations constitute what social learning theorists Lave and Wenger have called ‘communities of practice’ (Lave and Wenger Reference Lave and Wenger1991; Wenger Reference Wenger2006). Novices gain the knowledge and skills necessary to become a full member of a given community by actively engaging in the practices of the community, developing a sense of identity in relation to the group in the process (Lave and Wenger Reference Lave and Wenger1991).

Observation and imitation of an expert craftsperson are not sufficient on their own to enable a learner to master complex skills such as weaving; long-term interaction between the novice and a skilled practitioner is required in order for the novice to achieve competency. The apprentice learns to master increasingly difficult technical elements of the production sequence, with the expert craftsperson assisting as necessary, until the learner can work independently (a process known as scaffolding) (Minar and Crown Reference Minar and Crown2001, 372; Tehrani and Reide Reference Tehrani and Reide2008). For example, novices learning to weave on the horizontal ground loom in Iranian and Central Asian tribal communities are mainly taught through a combination of demonstration, collaborative weaving and intervention, as they master increasingly complex techniques (Tehrani and Collard Reference Tehrani and Collard2009, 289; Tehrani and Reide Reference Tehrani and Reide2008, 320–1). Rug designs in the pile-weaving technique are learnt by ‘filling in’ part of a design already partly woven by a skilled weaver, with the apprentice working on progressively larger and more complicated design sections (Tehrani and Reide Reference Tehrani and Reide2008, 321). In Zinacantec Maya communities, the teacher often demonstrates various elements of the process of weaving on the backstrap loom before the novice weaver attempts them and helps with the parts of the process that the learner has not yet fully mastered, providing more assistance with the more difficult elements than for the easier to execute components (Greenfield Reference Greenfield, Rogoff and Lave1984; Maynard and Greenfield Reference Maynard, Greenfield, Maynard and Martini2005, 85). Greenfield found that when there was a shift to weaving for commercial purposes, learning to weave became more of an independent trial and error process, but novice weavers still needed to ask for help when necessary (Greenfield Reference Greenfield and Derevenski2000). Learning to weave in the Sirwa mountains, in southern Morocco, where a type of vertical two-beam loom is used, is similarly scaffolded (Naji Reference Naji2012, 373).

In non-industrial societies, technical knowledge and skills are usually passed on from parents to children of the same gender, with learning taking place as part of everyday activities (Shennan and Steele Reference Shennan, Steele, Box and Gibson1999; Wendrich Reference Wendrich and Wendrich2012b, 11). Children become familiar with the body positions associated with carrying out a particular task at a very young age, through observation and imitation of those around them (Bril Reference Bril, Bril and Roux2002). The body postures required are often reinforced by wider cultural practices. For example, when weaving on the backstrap loom (a type of loom consisting of two bars, with one bar attached to a fixed object and the other attached to a strap around the weaver's waist), the body is part of the loom, and the weaver needs good balance, limited motor activity (especially upper-body stillness) and the ability to kneel for long periods (Maynard, Greenfield and Childs Reference Maynard, Greenfield and Childs1999, 381–5). Maynard, Greenfield and Childs have observed that, in the case of Zinacantec backstrap loom weavers (who are female), all of these capabilities are fostered through the cultural environment that children grow up in (Maynard, Greenfield and Childs Reference Maynard, Greenfield and Childs1999, 381–5). Babies are swaddled and encouraged to nurse at the slightest sign of movement, thus reducing their motor activity; as they get older, girls see their mothers and other women using restricted body movements in their everyday comportment, carrying firewood by balancing it on their heads, and kneeling to perform routine tasks such as cooking and making tortillas – in their turn they learn to do the same (Maynard, Greenfield and Childs Reference Maynard, Greenfield and Childs1999, 384–5). Maynard, Greenfield and Childs suggest that the low level of motor activity of the mother is also likely to have a prenatal influence on the unborn child.

Weaving on the horizontal ground loom and the vertical two-beam loom, on the other hand, requires the weaver to sit for prolonged periods of time. Naji (who undertook an apprenticeship as part of field research among Sirwan female carpet weavers) noted that, because Sirwan furniture only consisted of carpets or blankets and low tables, ‘people's joints, bones and musculature have been trained over generations to afford the specific body techniques of sitting on the ground’ (Naji Reference Naji2009, 54). In everyday activities, Sirwan female body techniques are much more controlled than those of men, providing a foundation for the constrained posture required when weaving (Naji Reference Naji2009, 54–5). In contrast to the backstrap loom, horizontal ground loom and vertical two-beam loom, weaving on the warp-weighted loom requires the weaver to stand for long periods, therefore necessitating a different body posture to the kneeling or sitting positions used when weaving on these other loom types.

In the same way that they learn the requisite body techniques through enculturation and socialisation, children in traditional communities that practise weaving become familiar with the loom and the weaving process from infancy, through everyday experience. In Sirwan households, for example, children play around the loom from the time they learn to walk, with toddlers sometimes trying to mimic the action of knotting a thread around a warp thread; as they get older, boys begin to distance themselves from the loom, however (Naji Reference Naji2009, 52–3). Young children frequently start to learn to weave by play weaving, often on a toy loom. Zinacantec girls begin to weave on miniature backstrap looms at the age of three or four years, and generally start to weave on a full-size loom at the age of eight or nine, although they sometimes begin as young as five (Greenfield Reference Greenfield, Rogoff and Lave1984, 120; Maynard, Greenfield and Childs Reference Maynard, Greenfield and Childs1999, 386–7). The Codex Mendoza indicates that Aztec girls learnt to weave at a similar age (Hendon Reference Hendon2006, 363). Children often learn to spin before they begin to weave on a full-size loom (see for example Hendon Reference Hendon2006, 363; Tehrani and Collard Reference Tehrani and Collard2009, 289). In Nigeria, Akwete girls begin to play at weaving on upturned stools by the age of three, and may start weaving on the upright two-beam loom as young as five (Aronson Reference Aronson and Coy1989, 151). Girls in Iranian tribal communities begin to learn weaving techniques at the ages of nine or ten, sometimes as young as six, initially practicing rug knots on miniature looms (Tehrani and Collard Reference Tehrani and Collard2009, 289). Today, girls in Sirwan families start to learn to weave between the age of 13 and 17, but previous generations traditionally started earlier, at age 10 or younger (Naji Reference Naji2012, 373). In the Bronze Age southern Aegean, the Linear B texts dating to the fourteenth–thirteenth centuries bc from Knossos on Crete and Pylos on the southern Greek mainland list children of both sexes in ration lists alongside the large numbers of female textile workers recorded, and it is likely that they assisted in/were learning the women's craft (once they reach a certain age, the boys are no longer recorded with the female workers) (Chadwick Reference Chadwick, Olivier and Palaima1988; Killen Reference Killen, Shelmerdine and Palaima1984, 52; Reference Killen, Gillis and Nosch2007, 54). Based on Near Eastern parallels, Nosch has estimated that children would begin to work in the work groups at the age of five to six years (Nosch Reference Nosch, Lovén and Stömberg2003, 16). Females classified as adult who were being taught (di-da-ka-re) are also mentioned in the Knossos tablets (Killen Reference Killen, Shelmerdine and Palaima1984, 55; Reference Killen, Gillis and Nosch2007, 54, 58 n. 11; Nosch Reference Nosch, Lovén and Stömberg2003, 17).

Because of the physical demands of weaving, children can only become completely independent weavers once they have developed sufficient strength and reach to operate the loom without assistance. On all types of traditional loom, weavers working on their own need sufficient reach to be able to pass the weft thread from one side of the shed to the other unaided, and sufficient strength to beat the weft into place (Foxhall Reference Foxhall, Marincola, Llewellyn-Jones and Maciver2012, 187–8). Zinacantec novice weavers need help passing the weft thread through the shed until their arms are long enough to do so unaided (Greenfield Reference Greenfield, Rogoff and Lave1984; Hendon Reference Hendon2006, 364). On the vertical two-beam loom, where the textile is woven from the bottom upwards, once the weaving reaches the upper part, the weaver has to sit on cushions or a stool to be able to continue (younger children would still not have sufficient height), and needs the stamina to be able to work with arms raised for extended periods (Naji Reference Naji2009, 57). On the warp-weighted loom, weaving starts at the top, so to work independently the weaver needs to be tall enough to be able to reach the upper part of the loom; the weft is beaten upwards, against gravity, therefore requiring even more strength than beating in the weft on other types of loom (Foxhall Reference Foxhall, Marincola, Llewellyn-Jones and Maciver2012, 198). Since the warp threads have weights attached to them, it is also hard work to change the shed, especially when weaving wider textiles that require a larger number of loom weights, and/or when weaving coarser textiles requiring heavier weights. Girls in Iranian tribal communities generally start to weave independently when they reach adolescence, and Zinacantec girls have usually become accomplished independent weavers by mid-adolescence (Maynard, Greenfield and Childs Reference Maynard, Greenfield and Childs1999, 386; Tehrani and Collard Reference Tehrani and Collard2009, 289). It is likely that children learning to weave on the warp-weighted loom in the past would not have been strong enough to become independent weavers before adolescence (Foxhall Reference Foxhall, Marincola, Llewellyn-Jones and Maciver2012, 197–8). Certain weaving tasks, such as setting up the warp on the loom and weaving wider textiles, are easier with two people, encouraging some degree of collaborative work even when the weaver has achieved expertise (Barber Reference Barber1991, 105; Reference Barber1994, 86–7). Because setting up the warp is particularly complex, such contexts of collaboration can facilitate support for novices by more experienced weavers.

To become an accomplished weaver requires long-term, repeated practice, and weaving apprenticeships usually last five to ten years (Maynard, Greenfield and Childs Reference Maynard, Greenfield and Childs1999, 386; Tehrani and Reide Reference Tehrani and Reide2008, 321). Warping and complex pattern-weaving (which requires practical mathematical skills) are the hardest techniques to master (Bier Reference Bier2004a; Dilley Reference Dilley and Coy1989, 187; Harris Reference Harris1987; Hendon Reference Hendon2006, 362; Tehrani and Reide Reference Tehrani and Reide2008, 321). In learning a craft skill, each individual element in a complex sequence of movements, such as the component actions executed while weaving, initially requires concentration on the part of the novice practitioner. Through constant repetition over an extended period of time, these separate elements are combined into a single, fluent action, becoming an automatic motor behaviour that can be performed quickly, accurately and consistently without having to think about it (Minar and Crown Reference Minar and Crown2001, 373; Schneider and Fisk Reference Schneider, Fisk and Magill1983). Brain imaging studies have indicated that neural changes occur during the different stages of learning a motor behaviour (an initial fast learning phase, a later slow learning phase, and a period of consolidation, automatisation and retention), with the execution of already acquired motor skills being controlled by different brain circuits than those that are active during the learning process (Doyon and Benali Reference Doyon and Benali2005; Luft and Buitrago Reference Luft and Buitrago2005). Once sequences of actions have become fully automatised they are very conservative. Altering such internalised sequences of actions necessitates returning to a conscious, controlled performance, which is both a great deal slower and more prone to mistakes (Minar and Crown Reference Minar and Crown2001, 373; Schneider and Fisk Reference Schneider, Fisk and Magill1983, 120). The internalisation of motor behaviours can cause ‘large negative transfer effects’, making it more difficult for an individual to modify or change an existing motor habit than it is for a complete novice to learn a motor skill for the first time (Schneider and Fisk Reference Schneider, Fisk and Magill1983, 131–2). Motor behaviours are thus highly resistant to change.

When a new craft technology is adopted, the necessary knowledge and know-how must be acquired from a different community of practice, through horizontal (between individuals of the same generation), rather than vertical (parent to child), transmission (Hosfield Reference Hosfield and Shennan2009, 46). Non-native learners are at a disadvantage, since they do not possess the body techniques and skills that native learners gain through early cultural experience (Maynard, Greenfield and Childs Reference Maynard, Greenfield and Childs1999). When the new technology replaces an existing technology, practitioners must also unlearn or modify previously automatised embodied techniques.

THE ADOPTION OF THE WARP-WEIGHTED LOOM IN SOUTHERN AEGEAN COMMUNITIES

With regard to the adoption of the warp-weighted loom in the southern Aegean, learners previously weaving on another type of loom would have needed to change established body movements and techniques. For example, different body postures are required when weaving on the warp-weighted loom than when weaving on either of the other two known Bronze Age eastern Mediterranean loom types, the horizontal ground loom and the vertical two-beam loom. As noted earlier, the weaver stands when working at the warp-weighted loom, whereas weaving on the other two loom types requires a sitting position. With the warp-weighted loom, since weaving starts at the top, the weft is beaten upwards, whereas on the horizontal ground loom and vertical two-beam loom the weft is beaten downwards, towards the lower beam of the loom and the weaver, so the motor sequences used in the weaving process are not the same. The warp-weighted loom also requires different warping methods to the other two loom types. Additional skills anyone learning to weave on the warp-weighted loom would need to acquire would be the ability to gauge how much tension warp threads of a particular type need on the loom, and the expertise to be able to judge how many threads should be attached to a particular loom weight in order to provide the threads with the correct tension.

If the learner was already adept at weaving on another type of loom, the amount of time needed to achieve proficiency at weaving on the warp-weighted loom is likely to have been reduced, but unlearning or adapting previous automatised practices and acquiring the necessary new knowledge and technical skills would still require prolonged contact between the learner and a skilled practitioner. The adoption of the warp-weighted loom by southern Aegean communities is therefore indicative of the mobility of individuals, with either novices travelling elsewhere to learn the craft or experienced non-local craftspeople spending extended periods of time in the adopting southern Aegean communities. Since weaving in the Aegean Bronze Age was almost certainly a female craft, this therefore suggests that the spread of the new weaving technology was linked to the mobility of women (Cutler Reference Cutler2011, 126–7; Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012, 150).

FEMALE MOBILITY AND THE TRANSMISSION OF TEXTILE CRAFT KNOWLEDGE: ARCHAEOLOGICAL MARKERS AND SOCIAL MECHANISMS

Among the loom weights recovered from southern Aegean settlements are a number that are made of non-local clays. More than 15 percent of the c.1200 loom weights, as well as a number of spindle whorls, recovered from the site of Ayia Irini on Kea are manufactured from clays that are not local to the island; the clay fabrics of additional tools may also be non-local (Cutler Reference Cutler2011, 475; Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012, 149–50). A range of non-local fabrics is present; on the basis of visual comparisons, the likely sources for these fabrics include Crete, other Cycladic islands, various locations in the south-east Aegean, as well as other Aegean locales. A similar percentage of the discoid loom weights from Phylakopi on Melos and Miletus are also non-local, with a range of fabrics again being represented, while lower numbers of non-local loom weights have also been identified at other southern Aegean sites (Cutler Reference Cutler2011, 475; Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012, 149–50).

Loom weights and other textile tools are unlikely to have been items of exchange, since they are objects that are not generally regarded as having any intrinsic value and are relatively easy to make, although they can be of considerable value to the craftspeople who use them (Barber Reference Barber1991, 299). They are therefore unlikely to have been transported from one location to another except as the personal possessions of their users. The non-local loom weights can therefore act as archaeological markers for the networks of connections through which textile craft knowledge and skills are likely to have been transferred. The range of non-local ceramic fabrics, together with differences in the timing of the uptake of the warp-weighted loom at the various southern Aegean sites (Cutler Reference Cutler2011; Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012), suggests that, although the diffusion of warp-weighted loom technology ultimately originated on Crete, some communities may have acquired the necessary skills through weavers from other Aegean centres where the technology had already been introduced and adopted, rather than directly from Cretan craftswomen. At Ayia Irini, non-local loom weights are documented through all phases of the Middle and Late Bronze Age sequence, suggesting that there was continuous mobility of women. Non-local loom weights have also been recovered from all excavated areas of the site, suggesting that this may have been a relatively widespread phenomenon within the community (Cutler Reference Cutler2011, 279). After the initial introduction of the technology, the techniques could then have been passed on to other women within the local community, initially perhaps through horizontal or oblique transmission and subsequently through vertical transmission within families. Weaving on the warp-weighted loom could therefore have become fully local within a generation.

Physical evidence for female mobility in the Bronze Age Aegean has been provided by recent strontium isotope ratio (87Sr/86Sr) analyses of 11 individuals from the sixteenth-century bc Shaft Graves in Grave Circle A at Mycenae, on the southern Greek mainland. Two out of three individuals identified as being non-local are women; these are the only two among the 11 individuals tested to be securely identified as female (Nafplioti Reference Nafplioti2009). One of these women, from Shaft Grave III (which also contained two probable male burials, as well as an infant wrapped in gold, found on the female's chest), was accompanied by a large quantity of rich grave goods, and it has been speculated that the women were present at Mycenae as the result of high status exogamous marriage alliances (Dickinson et al. Reference Dickinson, Papazoglou-Manioudaki, Nafplioti and Prag2012, 181–2; Nafplioti Reference Nafplioti2009, 289; Papazoglou-Manioudaki et al. Reference Papazoglou-Manioudaki, Nafplioti, Musgrave and Prag2010, 160–1). Although the case for exogamy (or formal marriage unions more generally) cannot be proven, intermarriage is highly likely to be foremost among the processes that could account for female mobility in the Bronze Age Aegean (Cutler Reference Cutler2011, 476; Reference Cutler, Nosch and Laffineur2012, 150; Gorogianni, Cutler and Fitzsimons Reference Gorogianni, Cutler, Fitzsimons, Stampolidis, Manger and Kopanias2015).

It is therefore possible that the presence of non-local loom weights and other textile tools at some of the southern Aegean sites may reflect the practice of women taking loom weights, and perhaps also a loom (as well as spinning equipment), with them when they married, possibly as part of a dowry. Preserved Old Babylonian dowry texts demonstrate that spindles, looms and weaving tools, as well as textiles and wool, were sometimes among dowry items given to women in Mesopotamia in the first quarter of the second millennium bc (Dalley Reference Dalley1980). Since the warp-weighted loom was not used in Mesopotamia during the Old Babylonian period, loom weights would not have constituted part of the weaving equipment. Higher-status women involved in exogamous marriages might also have been accompanied by female servants or slaves, bringing their own weaving equipment and practices with them. Women in the Aegean may have travelled with complete sets of loom weights, or may alternatively have only taken a few loom weights with them, to use as templates for making full sets of weights in local clay once they had moved to their new home.

Other possible processes which may have resulted in the dislocation of women are migration as part of a family group, enforced relocation as captives or slaves, or exchange of textile workers between elites. The large number of female textile workers listed in the later Linear B tablets from Pylos include groups of women referred to by ethnonyms that are interpreted as referring to places of origin beyond the Greek mainland, among which are locations that have been identified with the island of Kythera to the south, the south-west Anatolian coastal settlements of Knidos, Miletus and Halikarnassos, and the east Aegean islands of Lemnos and Chios, as well as ‘Asiatic’ (a-*64-ja) women (Chadwick Reference Chadwick, Olivier and Palaima1988; Shelmerdine Reference Shelmerdine, Cline and Harris-Cline1998, 295; Olsen Reference Olsen2014, 95–100). It has alternatively been suggested that the ‘Asiatic’ workers in the Pylos Linear B archives might originate from the same region as ‘Asiatic’ (Levantine) workers recorded in Egyptian texts, many of whom were captive female textile workers (Michailidou and Voutsa Reference Michailidou, Voutsa, Laffineur and Greco2005). The tablets also record ra-wi-ja-ja women, a term that has been interpreted as indicating captives or ‘women taken as booty’ (Chadwick Reference Chadwick, Olivier and Palaima1988, 83; Olsen Reference Olsen2014, 111–13). Some of the dependent women workers listed in the Linear B archives from Knossos are described as ‘slaves’ (do-e-ra) (Michailidou and Voutsa Reference Michailidou, Voutsa, Laffineur and Greco2005, 18–19; Nosch Reference Nosch, Lovén and Stömberg2003, 18–20). The term ‘slave’ may not have had the same meaning in the Late Bronze Age, however; Nosch (Reference Nosch, Lovén and Stömberg2003, 22) has observed that it only seems to be applied to individuals who are privately owned. The capture of craftspeople in the Late Bronze Age Aegean is documented in an early-thirteenth-century bc Hittite text which records a raid on Lazpa (interpreted as the island of Lesbos) by a man called Piyamaradu, in which he kidnapped craftsmen and took them to Millawanda (interpreted as Miletus) (Michailidou and Voutsa Reference Michailidou, Voutsa, Laffineur and Greco2005, 26–7; Niemeier Reference Niemeier and Laffineur1999, 151). Although it cannot be taken as evidence for the Bronze Age, it is also interesting to note that in the Iliad (6.289–95), Alexandros/Paris takes female captives from the Levantine coastal town of Sidon back to Troy to work as skilled weavers (Tzachili Reference Tzachili and Michailidou2001, 173).

Captive women working as weavers are also attested in earlier Bronze Age records from Mesopotamia. Foreign women (the majority of whom are associated with weaving and milling), thought to be captives, are recorded in the late-third-millennium bc Ur III texts (Uchitel Reference Uchitel1984; Wright Reference Wright and Wright1996), while it is documented that Zimri-Lim (who ruled the kingdom of Mari in the eighteenth century bc) sent more than 100 women from the defeated king's harem back to Mari when he conquered Ashlakka, most of whom were set to work as weavers (Sasson Reference Sasson, Aruz, Benzel and Evans2008, 99). In terms of the exchange of workers between rulers/elites, archives from Mari show that Zimri-Lim received 10 women, two boys and two girls from the ruler of Karana, who were set to work in the textile workshop in the palace (Dalley Reference Dalley2002, 153); the Egyptian pharaoh Tuthmosis III also presented 150 Asiatic weavers to one of his chief officials (Hall Reference Hall1986, 18). The exchange of specialist workers such as masons, sculptors and carpenters between eastern Mediterranean rulers in the Late Bronze Age is well documented in the fourteenth-century bc Amarna correspondence and other texts (Zaccagnini Reference Zaccagnini1983, 249–50). There is also evidence for a textile craft specialist being lent by one merchant to another; an eighteenth-century bc document from the Old Assyrian trading colony of Alishar in Anatolia records that a fuller had been sent on a temporary basis to a trader residing there by a business partner in another town (Dercksen Reference Dercksen and van Soldt2001, 43–4, 62). The evidence from the wider eastern Mediterranean and Middle East thus indicates that the mobility of craftspeople in the region, including weavers, was manifest through more than a single mechanism, and this is also likely to have been the case in the Bronze Age southern Aegean.

WEAVING THREADS OF CONNECTION

Cycladic wall paintings contemporary with the Cretan Neopalatial period, notably those from Akrotiri on Thera, show individuals wearing Cretan-style dress (Abramovitz Reference Abramovitz1980; Doumas Reference Doumas1999; Morgan and Cameron Reference Morgan, Cameron, Renfrew, Brodie, Morris and Scarre2007). The adoption of warp-weighted loom technology and the associated discoid loom weights in the southern Aegean may therefore have been at least partially driven by a desire to produce textiles of Cretan type. Many of the costumes depicted in the wall-paintings are elaborately patterned. Textile patterns are more likely than textile techniques to be transferred through horizontal transmission between members of the same generation (Hosfield Reference Hosfield and Shennan2009, 46; Tehrani and Collard Reference Tehrani and Collard2009, 289). Knowledge of new patterns can also be transferred through the circulation of the textiles themselves, although since pattern-weaving is done during the weaving process – thus being integral to the textile – and can be mechanised through the use of extra sheds, knowledge of the pattern-weaving technique used and skill in pattern-weaving on the same type of loom would be prerequisites for the successful adoption of new designs (Bier Reference Bier2004a, 181–2; Reference Bier and Bier2004b, 148; Tehrani and Collard Reference Tehrani and Collard2009, 289).

In considering how pattern-weaving techniques were transmitted, and how widely, it is necessary to take into account how many people are likely to have had the knowledge and skills necessary to weave the Cretan-style patterned textiles seen in the Akrotiri frescoes (which would have required carefully prepared, high-quality raw material, the dyeing of at least some fibres and knowledge of more complex weaving techniques). While both on Crete and in the wider southern Aegean the warp-weighted loom was in use in household contexts, there is of course likely to have been a considerable difference in the nature and, potentially, production processes of the textiles used by the general populace and those used by elites. In addition, through iconography on Crete and in the Akrotiri frescoes, it is clear that some particularly elaborate clothing was ritually significant, for offerings to, or for a robing ceremony involving, a goddess or her human or inanimate proxies (Crowley Reference Crowley, Nosch and Laffineur2012; Warren Reference Warren1988). However, while textiles as religious offerings are identified in the later Linear B documents, it is not clear whether any distinct contexts, personnel or procedures were involved in the production of these textiles (Nosch and Perna Reference Nosch, Perna, Laffineur and Hägg2001).

By the beginning of the Late Bronze Age, the warp-weighted loom was widely in use across the southern Aegean. Since mastering a complex craft requires sustained interaction between the novice and a skilled craftsperson, the adoption of the new loom technology would have necessitated the movement of people across the region. The differences in the timing, contexts and associations of the first appearance of loom weights at the various sites suggests that, in some cases, knowledge of Cretan weaving techniques may have been filtered through craftswomen from other, previously ‘Minoanised’ southern Aegean communities. The identification of non-local textile tools at a number of settlements provides a tangible means of exploring the patterns of mobility that are likely to have fostered the extended female networks of teaching, learning and craft practice through which the new technology and associated techniques are likely to have spread.

While textile production was almost certainly a female craft (Costin Reference Costin and Wright1996; Reference Costin and Bolger2013), use of the potter's wheel cross-culturally is associated with male potters (Kramer Reference Kramer1985, 79–80; Knappett and Nikolakopoulou Reference Knappett and Nikolakopoulou2008, 38–9). Wheel-made ceramics are not produced at the domestic level (Roux Reference Roux1990, 146), though weaving is a craft that can be practised both to meet domestic needs and to produce textiles for exchange. Whereas skill in use of the potter's wheel is likely to have been restricted to a limited number of individuals within a community, warp-weighted loom technology was often more widely employed. Both the introduction and use of these technologies are therefore likely to have involved different processes, and they were not likely to have been adopted together as a ‘package’ which can be taken to represent a single ‘Minoanisation’ process. Comparison of the evidence for the two crafts can therefore provide different perspectives on the mechanisms of the adoption, use and development of Cretan technologies in the wider southern Aegean.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The author completed this article before her terminal illness. At her request, Margarita Gleba and Todd Whitelaw edited it and guided it through the publication process. They would like to thank Flora Michelaki for translating the abstract, and the two anonymous reviewers and the editors for identifying points for clarification.

References

REFERENCES

Abell, N. and Hilditch, J. 2016. ‘Adoption and adaptation in pottery production practices: investigating Cycladic community interactions through the ceramic record of the second millennium bc’, in Gorogianni, E., Pavúk, P. and Girella, L. (eds), Beyond Thalassocracies: Understanding Processes of Minoanisation and Mycenaeanisation in the Aegean (Oxford), 155–71.Google Scholar
Abramovitz, K. 1980. ‘Frescoes from Ayia Irini, Keos, parts II–IV’, Hesperia 49, 5785.Google Scholar
Andersson Strand, E. 2015. ‘The basics of textile tools and textile technology – from fibre to fabric’, in Andersson Strand, E. and Nosch, M.-L.B. (eds), Tools, Textiles and Contexts: Investigating Textile Production in the Aegean and Eastern Mediterranean Bronze Age (Ancient Textiles Series Vol. 21; Oxford), 3960.Google Scholar
Andersson Strand, E. and Nosch, M.-L.B. 2015. ‘Summary of results and conclusions’, in Andersson Strand, E. and Nosch, M.-L.B. (eds), Tools, Textiles and Contexts: Investigating Textile Production in the Aegean and Eastern Mediterranean Bronze Age (Ancient Textiles Series Vol. 21; Oxford), 351–83.Google Scholar
Aronson, L. 1989. ‘To weave or not to weave: apprenticeship rules among the Akwete Igbo of Nigeria and the Baule of the Ivory Coast’, in Coy, M.W. (ed.), Apprenticeship: From Theory to Method and Back Again (Albany, NY), 149–62.Google Scholar
Barber, E.W. 1991. Prehistoric Textiles. The Development of Cloth in the Neolithic and Bronze Ages with Special Reference to the Aegean (Princeton, NJ).Google Scholar
Barber, E.W. 1994. Women's Work: The First 20,000 Years. Women, Cloth, and Society in Early Times (London).Google Scholar
Bier, C. 2004a. ‘Patterns in time and space: technologies of transfer and the cultural transmission of mathematical knowledge across the Indian Ocean’, Ars Orientalis 34, 172–94.Google Scholar
Bier, C. 2004b. ‘Pattern power: textiles and the transmission of knowledge’, in Bier, C. (ed.), Textile Society of America Symposium Proceedings (Oakland, CA), 144–53.Google Scholar
Bril, B. 2002. ‘L'apprentissage de gestes techniques: ordre de contraintes et variations culturelles’, in Bril, B. and Roux, V. (eds), Le geste technique. Réflexions méthodologiques et anthropologiques (Revue d'Anthropologie des Connaissances Vol. 14.2), 231–41.Google Scholar
Broodbank, C. 2004. ‘Minoanisation’, PCPS 50, 4691.Google Scholar
Carington Smith, J. 1975. ‘Spinning, weaving and textile manufacture in prehistoric Greece’ (unpublished PhD thesis, University of Tasmania).Google Scholar
Carington Smith, J. 1992. ‘Spinning and weaving equipment’, in McDonald, W.A. and Wilkie, N.C. (eds), Excavations at Nichoria in Southwest Greece, vol. 2: The Bronze Age Occupation (Minneapolis, MN), 674711.Google Scholar
Chadwick, J. 1988. ‘The women of Pylos’, in Olivier, J. and Palaima, T.G. (eds), Texts, Tablets and Scribes. Studies in Mycenaean Epigraphy and Economy Offered to Emmett L. Bennett, Jr. (Minos Supp. Vol. 10; Salamanca), 4395.Google Scholar
Costin, C. 1996. ‘Exploring the relationship between gender and craft in complex societies: methodological and theoretical issues in gender attribution’, in Wright, R.P. (ed.), Gender and Archaeology (Philadelphia, PA), 111–40.Google Scholar
Costin, C. 2013. ‘Gender and textile production in prehistory’, in Bolger, D. (ed.), A Companion to Gender Prehistory (Chichester), 180202.Google Scholar
Crowley, J. 2012. ‘Prestige clothing in the Bronze Age Aegean’, in Nosch, M.-L.B. and Laffineur, R. (eds), KOSMOS. Jewellery, Adornment and Textiles in the Aegean Bronze Age (Aegaeum Vol. 33; Liège), 231–9.Google Scholar
Cutler, J. 2011. ‘Crafting Minoanisation: textiles, crafts production and social dynamics in the Bronze Age southern Aegean’ (unpublished PhD thesis, University College London).Google Scholar
Cutler, J. 2012. ‘Ariadne's thread: the adoption of Cretan weaving technology in the wider southern Aegean in the mid-second millennium bc’, in Nosch, M.-L.B. and Laffineur, R. (eds), KOSMOS. Jewellery, Adornment and Textiles in the Aegean Bronze Age (Aegaeum Vol. 33, Liège), 145–54.Google Scholar
Cutler, J. 2016. ‘Fashioning identity: weaving technology, dress and cultural change in the Middle and Late Bronze Age southern Aegean’, in Gorogianni, E., Pavúk, P. and Girella, L. (eds), Beyond Thalassocracies: Understanding Processes of Minoanisation and Mycenaeanisation in the Aegean (Oxford), 172–85.Google Scholar
Cutler, J. forthcoming. Crafting Minoanisation: Textiles, Crafts Production and Social Dynamics in the Bronze Age Southern Aegean (Ancient Textiles Series Vol. 33; Oxford).Google Scholar
Dalley, S. 1980. ‘Old Babylonian dowries’, Iraq 42.1, 5374.Google Scholar
Dalley, S. 2002. Mari and Karana. Two Old Babylonian Cities, 2nd edn (Piscataway, NJ).Google Scholar
Davis, J.L. 1984. ‘Cultural innovation and the Minoan Thalassocracy at Ayia Irini, Keos’, in Hägg, R. and Marinatos, N. (eds), The Minoan Thalassocracy: Myth and Reality: Proceedings of the Third International Symposium at the Swedish Institute in Athens, 31 May through 5 June, 1982 (Skrifter utgivna av Svenska institutet i Athen Vol. 40; Stockholm), 159–66.Google Scholar
Davis, J.L. 2008. ‘Minoan Crete and the Aegean islands’, in Shelmerdine, C.W. (ed.), The Cambridge Companion to the Aegean Bronze Age (Cambridge), 186208.Google Scholar
Dercksen, J.G. 2001. ‘“When we met in Hattuš”: trade according to Old Assyrian texts from Alishar and Boğazköy’, in van Soldt, W.H. (ed.), Veenhof Anniversary Volume: Studies Presented to Klaas R. Veenhof on the Occasion of his Sixty-Fifth Birthday (Uitgaven van het Nederlands Historisch-Archaeologisch Instituut te İstanbul Vol. 89; Leiden), 3966.Google Scholar
Dickinson, O.T.P.K., Papazoglou-Manioudaki, L., Nafplioti, A. and Prag, A.J.N.W. 2012. ‘Mycenae revisited part 4: assessing the new data’, BSA 107, 161–88.Google Scholar
Dietler, M. and Herbich, I. 1994. ‘Ceramics and ethnic identity: ethnoarchaeological observations on the distribution of pottery styles and the relationship between the social contexts of production and consumption’, in Binder, D. and Courtin, J. (eds), Terre cuite et société: la céramique, document technique, economique, culturel (Juan-les-Pins), 459–72.Google Scholar
Dilley, R.M. 1989. ‘Secrets and skills: apprenticeship among Tukolor weavers’, in Coy, M.W. (ed.), Apprenticeship: From Theory to Method and Back Again (Albany, NY), 181–9.Google Scholar
Doumas, C. 1999. The Wall-Paintings of Thera, 2nd edn (Athens).Google Scholar
Doyon, J. and Benali, H. 2005. ‘Reorganisation and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills’, Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15.2, 161–7.Google Scholar
Foxhall, L. 2012. ‘Family time: temporality, gender and materiality in ancient Greece’, in Marincola, J., Llewellyn-Jones, L. and Maciver, C. (eds), Greek Notions of the Past in the Archaic and Classical Eras: History Without Historians (Edinburgh), 183206.Google Scholar
Gorogianni, E., Cutler, J. and Fitzsimons, R. 2015. ‘Something old, something new: non-local brides as catalysts for cultural exchange at Ayia Irini, Kea?’, in Stampolidis, N.C., Manger, Ç. and Kopanias, K. (eds), Nostoi: Indigenous Culture, Migration and Integration in the Aegean Islands and Western Anatolia During the Late Bronze Age and Early Iron Age (Istanbul), 889922.Google Scholar
Gorogianni, E., Pavúk, P. and Girella, L. (eds) 2016. Beyond Thalassocracies: Understanding Processes of Minoanisation and Mycenaeanisation in the Aegean (Oxford).Google Scholar
Gosselain, O. 1998. ‘Social and technical identity in a clay crystal ball’, in Stark, M.T. (ed.), The Archaeology of Social Boundaries (Washington, DC), 78106.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P.M. 1984. ‘A theory of the teacher in the learning activities of everyday life’, in Rogoff, B. and Lave, J. (ed.), Everyday Cognition: Its Development in Social Context (Cambridge), 117–38.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P.M. 2000. ‘Children, material culture and weaving: historical change and developmental change’, in Derevenski, S.J. (ed.), Children and Material Culture (London), 7286.Google Scholar
Hägg, R. and Marinatos, N. (eds) 1984. The Minoan Thalassocracy: Myth and Reality: Proceedings of the Third International Symposium at the Swedish Institute in Athens, 31 May through 5 June, 1982 (Skrifter utgivna av Svenska institutet i Athen Vol. 40; Stockholm)Google Scholar
Hall, R. 1986. Egyptian Textiles (Oxford).Google Scholar
Harris, M. 1987. ‘An example of traditional women's work as a mathematics resource’, For the Learning of Mathematics 7.3, 26–8.Google Scholar
Hendon, J.A. 2006. ‘Textile production as craft in Mesoamerica: time, labour and knowledge’, Journal of Social Archaeology 6.3, 354–78.Google Scholar
Hosfield, R. 2009. ‘Modes of transmission and material culture patterns in craft skills’, in Shennan, S.J. (ed.), Pattern and Process in Cultural Evolution (Berkeley, CA), 4560.Google Scholar
Killen, J.T. 1984. ‘The textile industries at Pylos and Knossos’, in Shelmerdine, C. and Palaima, T. (eds), Pylos Comes Alive: Industry and Administration in a Mycenaean Palace (New York), 4963.Google Scholar
Killen, J.T. 2007. ‘Cloth production in Late Bronze Age Greece: the documentary evidence’, in Gillis, C. and Nosch, M.-L.B. (eds), Ancient Textiles: Production, Craft and Society (Oxford), 50–8.Google Scholar
Knappett, C. and Nikolakopoulou, I. 2008. ‘Colonialism without colonies? A Bronze Age case study from Akrotiri, Thera’, Hesperia 77, 142.Google Scholar
Kramer, C. 1985. ‘Ceramic ethnoarchaeology’, Annual Review of Anthropology 14, 77102.Google Scholar
Lave, J. and Wenger, E. 1991. Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation (Cambridge).Google Scholar
Luft, A.R. and Buitrago, M.M. 2005. ‘Stages of motor skill learning’, Molecular Neurobiology 32.3, 205–16.Google Scholar
Macdonald, C.F., Hallager, E. and Niemeier, W.-D. (eds) 2009. The Minoans in the Central, Eastern and Northern Aegean – New Evidence (Monographs of the Danish Institute at Athens Vol. 8; Aarhus).Google Scholar
Maynard, A.E. and Greenfield, P.M. 2005. ‘An ethnomodel of teaching and learning: apprenticeship of Zinacantec Maya women's tasks’, in Maynard, A.E. and Martini, M.I. (eds), Learning in Cultural Context: Family, Peers and School (New York), 75103.Google Scholar
Maynard, A.E., Greenfield, P.M. and Childs, C.P. 1999. ‘Culture, history, biology, and body: native and non-native acquisition of technological skill’, Ethos 27.3, 379402.Google Scholar
Michailidou, A. and Voutsa, K. 2005. ‘Humans as a commodity in Aegean and Oriental societies’, in Laffineur, R. and Greco, E. (eds), EMPORIA: Aegeans in the Central and Eastern Mediterranean (Aegaeum Vol. 25; Liège), 1728.Google Scholar
Minar, C.J. and Crown, P.L. 2001. ‘Learning and craft production: an introduction’, Journal of Anthropological Research 57.4, 369–80.Google Scholar
Morgan, L. and Cameron, M. 2007. ‘The painted plasters and their relation to the wall paintings of the Pillar Crypt’, in Renfrew, C., Brodie, N., Morris, C. and Scarre, C. (eds), Excavations at Phylakopi in Melos 1974–77 (BSA Supp. Vol. 42; London), 371–99.Google Scholar
Nafplioti, A. 2009. ‘Mycenae revisited part 2. Exploring the local versus non-local geographical origin of the individuals from Grave Circle A: evidence from strontium isotope ratio (87Sr/86Sr) analysis’, BSA 104, 279–91.Google Scholar
Naji, M. 2009. ‘Gender and materiality in-the-making: the manufacture of Sirwan femininities through weaving in southern Morocco’, Journal of Material Culture 14.1, 4773.Google Scholar
Naji, M. 2012. ‘Learning to weave the threads of honor: understanding the value of female schooling in southern Morocco’, Anthropology and Education Quarterly 43.4, 372–84.Google Scholar
Niemeier, W.-D. 1999. ‘Mycenaeans and Hittites in war in western Asia Minor’, in Laffineur, R. (ed.), POLEMOS: le contexte guerrier en Égée à l’âge du Bronze (Aegaeum Vol. 19; Liège), 141–55.Google Scholar
Nikolakopoulou, I. and Knappett, C. 2016. ‘Mobilities in the Neopalatial southern Aegean: the case of Minoanisation’, in Kiriatzi, E. and Knappett, C. (eds), Human Mobility and Technological Transfer in the Prehistoric Mediterranean (BSA Studies in Greek Antiquity; Cambridge), 102–15.Google Scholar
Nosch, M.-L.B. 2003. ‘The women at work in the Linear B tablets’, in Lovén, L.L. and Stömberg, A. (eds), Gender, Cult, and Culture in the Ancient World from Mycenae to Byzantium (Studies in Mediterranean Archaeology Pocket Book Vol. 166; Sävedalen), 1226.Google Scholar
Nosch, M.-L.B. and Perna, M. 2001, ‘Cloth in the cult’, in Laffineur, R. and Hägg, R. (eds), Potnia. Deities and Religion in the Bronze Age Aegean (Aegaeum Vol. 22; Liège), 471–7.Google Scholar
Olofsson, L., Andersson Strand, E. and Nosch, M.-L.B. 2015. ‘Experimental testing of Bronze Age textile tools’, in Strand, Andersson, and Nosch, E., M.-L.B. (eds), Tools, Textiles and Contexts: Investigating Textile Production in the Aegean and Eastern Mediterranean Bronze Age (Ancient Textiles Series Vol. 21; Oxford), 75100.Google Scholar
Olsen, B. 2014. Women in Mycenaean Greece: The Linear B Tablets from Pylos and Knossos (London).Google Scholar
Papazoglou-Manioudaki, L., Nafplioti, A., Musgrave, J.H. and Prag, A.J.N.W. 2010. ‘Mycenae revisited part 3. The human remains from Grave Circle A at Mycenae. Behind the masks: a study of the bones of Shaft Graves I–IV’, BSA 105, 157224.Google Scholar
Pavúk, P. 2012. ‘Of spools and discoid loom weights: Aegean-style weaving at Troy revisited’, in Nosch, M.-L.B. and Laffineur, R. (eds), KOSMOS. Jewellery, Adornment and Textiles in the Aegean Bronze Age (Aegaeum Vol. 33; Liège), 121–30.Google Scholar
Roux, V. 1990. ‘The psychological analysis of technical activities: a contribution to the study of craft specialisation’, Archaeological Review from Cambridge 9, 142–53.Google Scholar
Sasson, J.M. 2008. ‘Text, trade, and travellers’, in Aruz, J., Benzel, K. and Evans, J.M. (eds), Beyond Babylon: Art, Trade and Diplomacy in the Second Millennium bc (New York), 95100.Google Scholar
Schneider, W. and Fisk, A.D. 1983. ‘Attention theory and mechanisms for skilled performance’, in Magill, R.A. (ed.), Memory and Control of Action (Amsterdam), 119–44.Google Scholar
Shelmerdine, C.W. 1998. ‘Where do we go from here? And how can the Linear B tablets help us get there?’, in Cline, E. and Harris-Cline, D. (eds), The Aegean and the Orient in the Second Millennium. Proceedings of a Conference Held at the University of Cincinnati 18–20 April 1997 (Aegaeum Vol. 18; Liège), 291–9.Google Scholar
Shennan, S.J. and Steele, J. 1999. ‘Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioural ecological approach’, in Box, H.O. and Gibson, K.R. (eds), Mammalian Social Learning: Comparative and Ecological Perspectives (Cambridge), 367–88.Google Scholar
Stark, M.T., Bowser, B.J. and Horne, L. (eds) 2008. Cultural Transmission and Material Culture: Breaking Down Boundaries (Tucson, AZ).Google Scholar
Tehrani, J.J. and Collard, M. 2009. ‘On the relationship between interindividual cultural transmission and population-level cultural diversity: a case study of weaving in Iranian tribal populations’, Evolution and Human Behavior 30.4, 286300.Google Scholar
Tehrani, J.J. and Reide, F. 2008. ‘Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions’, WorldArch 40.3, 316–31.Google Scholar
Tzachili, I. 2001. ‘Circulation of textiles in the Late Bronze Age Aegean’, in Michailidou, A. (ed.), Manufacture and Measurement: Counting, Measuring and Recording Craft Items in Early Aegean Societies (Meletemata Vol. 33; Athens), 167–75.Google Scholar
Uchitel, A. 1984. ‘Women at work: Pylos and Knossos, Lagash and Ur’, Historia 33, 257–82.Google Scholar
Vakirtzi, S. 2019. ‘Spindle whorls and loomweights’, in Nikolakopoulou, I. (ed.), Akrotiri, Thera. Middle Bronze Age Pottery and Stratigraphy, vol. 2 (Archaeological Society of Athens Library Series Vol. 318; Athens), 479500.Google Scholar
Warren, P. 1972. Myrtos: An Early Bronze Age Settlement in Crete (BSA Supp. Vol. 7; London).Google Scholar
Warren, P. 1988. Minoan Religion as Ritual Action (Studies in Mediterranean Archaeology Pocket Book Vol. 72; Gothenburg).Google Scholar
Wendrich, W. (ed.) 2012a. Archaeology and Apprenticeship: Body Knowledge, Identity, and Communities of Practice (Tucson, AZ).Google Scholar
Wendrich, W. 2012b. ‘Archaeology and apprenticeship: body knowledge, identity, and communities of practice’, in Wendrich, W. (ed.), Archaeology and Apprenticeship: Body Knowledge, Identity, and Communities of Practice (Tucson, AZ), 119.Google Scholar
Wenger, E. 2006. Communities of Practice: Learning, Meaning and Identity (Cambridge).Google Scholar
Wright, R.P. 1996. ‘Technology, gender and class: worlds of difference in Ur III Mesopotamia’, in Wright, R.P. (ed.), Gender and Archaeology (Philadelphia, PA), 79110.Google Scholar
Zaccagnini, C. 1983. ‘Patterns of mobility among ancient Near Eastern craftsmen’, JNES 42.4, 245–64.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. The warp-weighted loom. Drawing: Annika Jeppsson © Annika Jeppsson and the Centre for Textile Research, University of Copenhagen.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Examples of discoid loom weights: a) discoid rounded; b) discoid elliptical. Photographs: J. Cutler.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Map of the Aegean, showing the main locations mentioned in the text. Drawing: J. Cutler, T. Whitelaw.