Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-9nwgx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-20T16:35:19.579Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does Aid Reduce Anti-refugee Violence? Evidence from Syrian Refugees in Lebanon

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 June 2020

M. CHRISTIAN LEHMANN*
Affiliation:
University of Brasilia
DANIEL T. R. MASTERSON*
Affiliation:
Stanford University
*
M. Christian Lehmann Assistant Professor, Department of Economics, University of Brasilia, clehmann@unb.br.
Daniel T. R. Masterson Postdoctoral Fellow, Immigration Policy Lab, Department of Political Science, Stanford University, dmasters@stanford.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Anti-refugee violence often accompanies refugee migration, but the factors that fuel or mitigate that violence remain poorly understood, including the common policy response in such settings of humanitarian aid. Existing theory and policy debates predict that aid to refugees exacerbates anti-refugee violence by increasing hosts’ resentment toward refugees. In contrast, however, aid may reduce violence in ways such as increasing host communities’ well-being through more demand for local goods and services and refugees sharing aid. We test for the sign and mechanisms of this relationship. Evidence from original survey data and a regression discontinuity design suggests that cash transfers to Syrian refugees in Lebanon did not increase anti-refugee violence, and if anything they reduced violence. Exploring why aid does not increase hostility, we find evidence that aid allows recipients to indirectly compensate locals through higher demand for local goods and services, directly benefit locals by offering help and sharing aid, and reduce contact with potential aggressors.

Type
Letter
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the American Political Science Association

Introduction

In recent years, high levels of refugee migration have led to social tensions, political conflict, and right-wing political backlash (Alrababa’h et al. Reference Alrababa’h, Dillon, Williamson, Hainmueller, Hangartner and Weinstein2019; Bansak, Hainmueller, and Hangartner Reference Bansak, Hainmueller and Hangartner2016; Braithwaite et al. Reference Braithwaite, Chu, Curtis and Ghosn2019; Ghosn, Braithwaite, and Chu Reference Ghosn, Braithwaite and Chu2019; Hangartner et al. Reference Hangartner, Dinas, Marbach, Matakos and Xefteris2019; Marbach and Ropers Reference Marbach and Ropers2018). We explore international humanitarian aid as a potential mechanism by which these tensions can be reduced, studying how aid affects violence targeting refugees. Although aid is often portrayed as increasing hostility from host-country nationals, we present quasi-experimental evidence that cash transfers to refugees do not increase, and may in fact reduce, anti-refugee violence.

What should we expect from cash transfers to refugees? Existing literature and our fieldwork raise concerns that humanitarian aid exacerbates violence targeting refugees. The humanitarian community is concerned about potential increases in resentment among host-country nationals that refugees receive assistance and that the aid is “unfairly” distributed (UNDP 2019; World Bank 2013). Aid may exacerbate hostility through distributional concerns, negatively affecting natives’ perceived relative well-being or activating concerns over resource scarcity (Bobo and Hutchings Reference Bobo and Hutchings1996; Dahlberg, Edmark, and Lundqvist Reference Dahlberg, Edmark and Lundqvist2012; Dancygier Reference Dancygier2010; Dustmann, Vasiljeva, and Damm Reference Dustmann, Vasiljeva and Damm2018). We also know that increased exposure to refugee populations and camps, with their concomitant humanitarian aid responses, may increase hostility toward refugees (Hangartner et al. Reference Hangartner, Dinas, Marbach, Matakos and Xefteris2019; Zhou Reference Zhou2018). However, the role of aid specifically in causing this hostility has not been explored.

Despite predictions that humanitarian aid exacerbates anti-refugee violence, a number of factors suggest reasons why it may not do so. If aid mitigates the perceived adverse economic consequences of refugees for host communities, then natives may be less likely to use violence as a strategic tool (to impede refugees from causing economic harm, to extract transfers, or to enact vengeance) or as an instinctual emotional reaction (caused by anger). In this case aid may reduce hostility by increasing refugees’ capacity to appease hosts through offering help (e.g., sharing aid) and through buying locally and boosting demand for local goods and services. Furthermore, aid may allow refugees to both avoid contact with potential aggressors (e.g., by substituting public with private transport) and increase positive contact with locals.Footnote 1

To study how international humanitarian aid affects anti-refugee hostility, we collected survey data from over 1,300 Syrian refugee households in Lebanon, a country with a population of 4.5 million that has seen the arrival of more than a million refugees since the outbreak of the Syrian civil war in 2011. As is typical for most refugees today, with only one third of the world’s refugees living in camps, the vast majority of Syrian refugees (and all respondents of our survey) live in rented apartments, rooms, and improvised shelters in Lebanese towns.

For our empirical test, we leverage the fact that approximately half of the refugees in our sample were as-if randomly assigned to receive cash transfers from the UN refugee agency (UNHCR): only refugees in communities at or above 500 meters altitude were eligible for these transfers because UNHCR used an altitude-based criterion to target refugees residing in colder climates during the winter months. Hence we estimate a regression discontinuity design (RD), comparing refugees in communities slightly below 500 meters altitude (control group) to those living in communities slightly above 500 meters altitude (treatment group).

Our RD estimates suggest that cash transfers to refugees did not increase anti-refugee violence and may have reduced it. Exploring why aid did not exacerbate anti-refugee hostility, we find evidence of possible mechanisms including that aid allows recipients to indirectly compensate locals through higher demand for local goods and services, directly benefit locals by offering help and sharing aid, and reduce contact with potential aggressors. We contribute to the literature on the causes of anti-refugee violence (Benček and Strasheim Reference Benček and Strasheim2016; Falk, Kuhn, and Zweimueller Reference Falk, Kuhn and Zweimueller2011; Jäckle and König Reference Jäckle and König2017; Koopmans Reference Koopmans1996; Onoma Reference Onoma2013), which has previously examined a range of causes including political and economic drivers, by examining the role of international humanitarian aid.

Methods

In this section we provide a brief summary of the cash transfer program and our empirical strategy. In our web appendix, we provide a detailed description of the aid program and survey. We also report full descriptive statistics and correlates of anti-refugee violence as well as conventional tests of the internal validity of our RD.

The Cash Transfer Program

In November 2013, the UN started a new aid program for almost a hundred thousand Syrian refugee families in Lebanon. The goal of the program was to help refugees stay warm, dry, and healthy during the cold, wet winter months. Eligible refugee households received an ATM card from the UN. Between November 2013 and March 2014, the UN transferred about $US100 per month to a bank account linked to each ATM card, $US575 in total over the course of six months (which is roughly $US1,000 in PPP terms). Since the program was only intended for the winter, it ended in April 2014.

Regression Discontinuity Design

We rely on a criterion that only refugees in communities at or above 500 meters altitude were eligible for transfers. According to a senior UNHCR official we interviewed, the goal of the program was to target refugees in colder climates, and they selected a round number (500) as the altitude cutoff subject to their program’s budget constraint. Within communities above 500 meters altitude, all Syrian households that met a number of proxy poverty criteria received the transfers. Hence, we estimate an RD, comparing refugees in eligible “poor” households slightly above 500 meters altitude (treatment group) to otherwise eligible “poor” households living in communities slightly below 500 meters altitude (control group).

We sought to collect survey data on all 1,851 “poor” refugee households residing between 450 and 550 meters altitude, and we successfully interviewed 1,358 households (73% response rate, 74.1% in control, and 72.7% in treatment).Footnote 2 Our survey was administered in April and May 2014—that is, about six months after the start of the program.

Treated and control communities cover the entire mountain range along the Lebanese coast.Footnote 3 Formal statistical tests do not suggest systematic differences between treated and control communities in terms of geography (latitude/longitude), demography (number of Lebanese and refugees), climate (temperature and precipitation), economy, and religious sect.Footnote 4 We thus have little reason to believe that culture and attitudes toward strangers, which are presumably correlated with the variables used in our balance tests, would vary discontinuously around the cutoff. Finally, there is no evidence that other aid programs vary discontinuously around the cutoff.Footnote 5

Following Imbens and Lemieux (Reference Imbens and Lemieux2008), we implement the RD by running local regressions of the form

(1)$$ {Y}_i=\alpha +\beta \mathbf{1}\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\kern.50em \ge \kern.50em 500\right)+f\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\right)+{\epsilon}_{i\kern1.em }\forall 450\kern.40em \le \kern.40em {A}_i\kern.40em \le \kern.40em 550, $$

where Ai is the altitude where household i resides. The term f(Ai) is a polynomial function of Ai. We follow the suggestion of Imbens and Lemieux (Reference Imbens and Lemieux2008) and use linear and quadratic functional forms for f(Ai), and allow for different slopes of the regression function on both sides of the cutoff. The parameter $ \beta$ measures the local average treatment effect (LATE) of aid on outcome Y i at A i = 500.

Descriptive Statistics

The average household in our sample is composed of three children and two adults. A total of 28% of heads have no schooling or did not complete primary schooling, while 33% completed primary school, 30% middle school, and 9% secondary school or higher. A total of 89% of households rent rooms; the remainder lives in improvised shelters. Household monthly food and nonfood consumption is $US320 and $US460, respectively.Footnote 6 Household members worked a total of 11.8 days during the past 4 weeks, with a labor income of $US182. A total of 76% of respondents report casual labor in shops, agriculture, or construction as their main occupation. The ratio of Syrian to Lebanese households is 60:282 in treatment communities and 58:283 in control communities.

Measurement of Hostility

We asked survey respondents whether Lebanese in the community had been physically aggressive to household members in the past six months. In a separate question, we asked about verbal abuse perpetrated by Lebanese community members in the past six months. We did not ask about other acts of violence committed by Lebanese.

The precise wording of the questions was

  • “In the last six months, have Lebanese living in this community been physically aggressive with members of your household often, sometimes, rarely, or never?”

  • “In the last six months, have Lebanese living in this community said things to insult or hurt members of your household often, sometimes, rarely, or never?”

Our fieldwork suggests that refugees do not generally trust Lebanese authorities enough to report crimes like assault, hence self-reported data is perhaps the most reliable data source, even with its limitations.Footnote 7

A total of 5.2% of respondents report verbal assault, and 1.3% report physical assault by Lebanese community members. We code respondents who answer “often” or “sometimes” as having experienced assault. Results are robust to including “rarely” responses as incidences of violence, as shown in the appendix.Footnote 8 We inquired about the reason for the verbal or physical assault by asking respondents the following question:

  • “What do you view as the reasons for the verbal or physical attack?”

Response options included:

  1. a) “She/he/they think that my household takes away his/her/their jobs” (40% of respondents)

  2. b) “My household receives assistance (e.g., food or money) from organizations” (17% of respondents)

  3. c) “She/he/they think that my household’s presence has increased prices” (16% of respondents)

  4. d) “She/he/they do not like the way I and/or members of my household dress” (8% of respondents)

  5. e) “She/he/they think that my household’s presence has increased crime” (7% of respondents)

  6. f) “She/he/they think that I and/or members of my household are lazy” (4% of respondents)

  7. g) Other reason (7% of respondents)

Respondents believe that economic consequences of the refugee influx for host communities are the primary drivers of violence, with (a) higher unemployment and (c) inflation accounting for more than half (56%) of the hostility that refugees experience. Respondents’ perception that their effect on unemployment is the primary cause for violence aligns with correlational analysis of our survey data: we find a statistically significant negative correlation between the daily wage rate of agricultural labor and the number of refugees in a community, controlling for the number of Lebanese residents. A 1% increase in the size of the refugee population is associated with a 0.05% decrease in local wages. Furthermore, we find that a refugee’s labor supply (hours worked) is a statistically significant predictor of violence in our data.

In contrast, a non-negligible share of respondents (17%) stated that receiving humanitarian aid was one of the reasons for anti-refugee violence (reason b). During fieldwork and discussions with government and nongovernmental organizations we often encountered the view that humanitarian aid for refugees is further fueling hostility in host communities. Allegedly, members of host communities are furious that Syrians receive humanitarian aid, while host communities receive little compensation for the supposed negative impact of the Syrian refugee crisis on Lebanese wages.Footnote 9

Results and Discussion

Figure 1 suggests that cash transfers did not increase hostility toward refugees, and if anything they may have reduced it. Point estimates are consistently negative and confidence intervals rule out large positive effects. As shown in Figure 2 and the appendix, results are robust to numerous modeling choices. A linear regression modelFootnote 10 produces our most conservative estimates, and we also present results dropping the 490–499-meter altitude bin with relatively high incidences of violence. The RD estimates with (and without) the outlier bin show a decrease of 2.7 (0.7) percentage points in physical violence and 7.5 (4.7) percentage points in verbal assault. Table 1 presents these results and also shows results from a quadratic model,Footnote 11 which produces even larger negative effect estimates.Footnote 12Figure 2 shows results across a range of bandwidths when we restrict the sample to observations closer to the cutoff. Point estimates remain consistently negative and confidence intervals refute any meaningful increase in violence.

Table 1. Aid and Anti-refugee Violence

Note: This table reports ordinary least square estimates of $ \beta$ in Equation 1 with a linear polynomial (columns 1 and 2) and quadratic polynomial (columns 3 and 4), and in parentheses are robust standard errors clustered at the community level. In Panels I, II, III(i.), and III(ii.), columns 1 and 3 (2 and 4), the dependent variable is a 1/0 dummy: 1 if Lebanese have “sometimes” or “often” been physically (verbally) aggressive to the respondent or members of the household in the past six months and 0 if “rarely” or “never.” In panel III(iii.), columns 1 and 3 (2 and 4), the dependent variable is a 1/0 dummy: 1 if Syrians in the community have “sometimes” or “often” been physically (verbally) aggressive to the respondent or members of the household in the past six months and 0 if “rarely” or “never.” * p < .10, ** p < .05, *** p < .01.

Note: Graphs show parametric and nonparametric regressions on both sides of the 500-meter eligibility cutoff. The dependent variable in the left (right) graphs is a 1/0 dummy: 1 if Lebanese have “sometimes” or “often” been verbally (physically) aggressive to the respondent or members of the household in the past six months and 0 if “rarely” or “never.” The dots show the mean of the dependent variable by altitude bins of 10 meters. The size of each dot reflects the number of observations in that particular bin. The number of observations in the upper graphs is 1,356. The lower graphs exclude observations between 490 and 499 meters altitude, and the number of observations in that case is 1,335.

Figure 1. Aid and Anti-refugee Violence

Note: The solid line shows ordinary least squares estimates of β in Equation 1 with a linear polynomial, for successively smaller bandwidths around the cutoff. Light (dark) gray areas indicate 95 (90)% confidence intervals. The dependent variable in the left (right) graphs is a 1/0 dummy: 1 if Lebanese have “sometimes” or “often” been verbally (physically) aggressive to the respondent or members of the household in the past six months and 0 if “rarely” or “never.” The graphs present results for all bandwidths from 50 meters (our full data, from 450 to 550 meters altitude) to 20 meters (from 480 to 520 meters altitude). We exclude bandwidths smaller than 20 meters, which subset the data to fewer than 100 observations on either side of the cutoff.

Figure 2. Aid and Anti-refugee Violence: Smaller Bandwidth

Placebo tests support these findings. The first test consists of using 475 and 525 meters altitude as pseudo-eligibility cutpoints, which yield precise point estimates close to zero. In the second placebo test, we replace the dependent variable with verbal or physical assault from other Syrian refugees (as opposed to Lebanese locals). According to our theoretical predictions about the drivers of anti-refugee violence, we would not necessarily expect any effect of aid on hostility between refugees, and in line with this reasoning we get precise point estimates close to zero.Footnote 13 We provide further robustness tests in the appendix.Footnote 14

Why is it that humanitarian aid does not exacerbate anti-refugee hostility? One possibility is that aid allowed refugees to take steps that mitigate their risk of violence, such as indirectly benefiting locals through higher demand for local goods and services, directly benefiting locals by offering help and sharing aid, and reducing contact with potential aggressors (e.g., by substituting public with private means of transport). Our data provide evidence of these mechanisms: for example, we asked respondents, “Do members of your household provide help to Lebanese living in this community such as help looking after their children, help when they are sick, help with housework, or giving money?” and find a large increase in the propensity to help hosts at 500 meters altitude (see Figure 3). Furthermore, food consumption (in the past 30 days) jumps by $US70 at 500 meters altitude, suggesting that aid recipients spend roughly 70%Footnote 15 of their aid (UNHCR transferred roughly $US100 in the 30 days prior to data collection) in the local economy.Footnote 16 The contact with refugees on these occasions may also have increased hosts’ acceptance. Ghosn, Braithwaite, and Chu (Reference Ghosn, Braithwaite and Chu2019), for example, find that Lebanese who have had contact with Syrians are significantly more likely to support hosting refugees.Footnote 17 Finally, Figure 3 suggests that aid may have allowed refugees to avoid contact with potential aggressors by substituting public with private transport. More rigorous tests of these hypotheses remain on the agenda for future research.

Note: Graphs show parametric and nonparametric regressions on both sides of the 500-meter eligibility cutoff. The dots show the mean of the dependent variable by altitude bins of 10 meters. The size of each dot reflects the number of observations in that particular bin. In the appendix we report ordinary least squares estimates of β for these and other dependent variables.

Figure 3. Possible Mechanisms

A caveat of our research design is external validity. Our results are confined to poor refugees in host communities around 500 meters altitude in winter, which is not a representative population or season. If anti-refugee violence is generally lower during winter (e.g., because refugees and hosts have less contact as both stay at home more) and cash transfers indeed reduce violence, then we should expect the treatment effect in nonwinter months to be larger than what we report in this article.Footnote 18 Furthermore, the effect of cash transfers on anti-refugee violence may change as a refugee crisis wears on and as the baseline rate of intergroup tensions changes.Footnote 19

To draw policy implications for cash transfers and anti-refugee violence in a given situation, we must consider policy constraints and alternatives. In some contexts market capacity or infrastructure will be important constraints. Our evidence, confined to Syrian refugees in Lebanon residing between 450 and 550 meters altitude, suggests that these local economies adjusted well to refugees receiving cash transfers: we find large positive effects on food consumption of recipients and no effects on local prices; that is, market supply accommodated the increased demand of cash transfer recipients. In other contexts, however, widespread programming could cause inflation (if market supply is unable to accommodate large shifts in demand) and may thereby increase resentment/hostility toward refugees. Further research should study alternative humanitarian interventions to understand conditions under which aid may have heterogeneous effects on hostility. If humanitarian aid mitigates anti-refugee hostility because of positive spillover effects on host communities, further research should consider how to realize these benefits, such as delivering aid directly to host communities (e.g., cash transfers to the local Lebanese population, hospitals, schools, etc.), possibly explicitly linked to the community’s role in hosting refugees. In Lebanon, a number of organizations provide assistance for host communities with the goal of reducing tensions. Testing for the effects of such direct transfers to hosts, including whether they generate reductions in anti-refugee sentiments or have adverse effects, is, we believe, a fruitful avenue for future research.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0003055420000349.Replication materials can be found on Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/5LAZ54.

Footnotes

We benefited from conversations, comments, and suggestions of Peter Aronow, Dominik Hangartner, Dean Karlan, David Laitin, Jason Lyall, Chris Udry, Elisabeth J. Wood, and several anonymous referees. We thank the International Rescue Committee (IRC) and The UN Refugee Agency (UNHCR) for collaboration and Chris Blattman and Juliette Seban for their support in the early stages of this project. Part of this work was completed while Lehmann was visiting the Yale Economic Growth Center and while Masterson was visiting the American University of Beirut Issam Fares Institute for Public Policy, which the authors thank for their hospitality. We are grateful for funding through DFID grant agreement number 204007-111. We declare that we have no relevant or material financial interests that relate to the research described in this paper. Replication files are available at the American Political Science Review Dataverse: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/5LAZ54.

1 Ghosn, Braithwaite, and Chu (Reference Ghosn, Braithwaite and Chu2019), for example, find that Lebanese who have had contact with Syrian refugees are significantly more likely to support hosting refugees.

2 We chose this particular 450–550-meter altitude bandwidth based on available funding for the household survey, i.e., limited funding prevented us from collecting data on more than 1,800 households.

3 See appendix for a map of treated and control communities.

4 See appendix. We thank Lama Mourad for generously sharing her sect data with us. See Mourad (Reference Mourad2019).

5 There were many other aid agencies delivering aid apart from UNHCR. We conducted interviews with more than a dozen major aid organizations in Lebanon regarding their aid programs for Syrians or for Lebanese host communities, and none of these programs used a 500-meter altitude criterion, i.e., other aid programming will be balanced at the cutoff.

6 The most important items being rent ($US208), bread ($US64), vegetables ($US52), health care ($US46), oils ($US34), meat ($US34), cleaning products ($US26), electricity ($US23), tobacco ($US23), and public transportation ($US22).

7 If refugees do not trust Lebanese authorities, would they trust Lebanese enumerators? We believe yes, for our enumerators introduced themselves as working on behalf of academic researchers and a humanitarian aid organization (see consent script in appendix), which refugees have arguably less reason to mistrust. Even if there is mistrust, this should be balanced at the cutoff given our quasi-experimental research design.

8 To highlight the magnitude of the verbal or physical assault rates from our survey data, consider that in the FBI’s 2016 annual report on violent crime in the USA, they report an estimated 386.3 violent crimes per 100,000 inhabitants in 2016—a rate of 0.38%, less than 1%. Unfortunately we do not have data on the rate of verbal or physical assault for the general population in Lebanon. The rates of anti-refugee violence in our data are much higher than rates seen in data from police and media used by studies that focus on Western Europe. In addition to the possibility that the rates are much higher in Lebanon, or that Syrians overreport violence in our survey, is the possibility that European police/media data underreport violence (e.g., some assaulted refugees do not report violence out of fear of retaliation). In Germany, Benček and Strasheim (Reference Benček and Strasheim2016) and Jäckle and König (Reference Jäckle and König2017) use data from the Amadeu Antonio Foundation, which registers assaults based on news reports. The Foundation’s and official police data yield similar assault figures (see Bundesministerium des Innern 2014; 2015). In 2014, when Germany hosted roughly 200,000 refugees (source: http://popstats.unhcr.org), the police registered 512 physical assaults on refugees, i.e., a rate of 0.2%. This figure is 1.3% in our household survey with Syrian refugees in Lebanon (also conducted in 2014), i.e., roughly six times larger.

9 Although survey data from Lebanese community members could have provided numerous important insights, funding constraints prevented us from collecting such data. Recent work explores the Lebanese community’s perceptions of refugees, e.g., Ghosn, Braithwaite, and Chu (Reference Ghosn, Braithwaite and Chu2019).

10 $ f\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\right)={\gamma}_1\left({\mathrm{A}}_i-500\right)+{\gamma}_21\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\ge 500\right)\times \left({\mathrm{A}}_i-500\right)$ in Equation 1.

11 $ f\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\right)={\theta}_1\left({\mathrm{A}}_i-500\right)+{\theta}_21\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\ge 500\right)\times \left({\mathrm{A}}_i\kern.20em -\kern.30em 500\right)+{\theta}_3{\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\kern.50em -\kern.50em 500\right)}^2+{\theta}_41\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\kern.50em \ge \kern.50em 500\right)\kern.50em \times \kern.50em {\left({\mathrm{A}}_i\kern.50em -\kern.50em 500\right)}^2$ in Equation 1.

12 The relatively high incidences of violence between 490 and 499 meters altitude do not seem to be driven by systematic pre-program differences between respondents residing there and the rest of our sample: in the appendix we report results of regressing an outlier dummy (if respondent resides between 490 and 499 meters altitude) on a vector of exogenous household and community characteristics, which yields point estimates close to zero for most household and community characteristics. The only statistically significant difference is for the variable “Can a truck drive to your village center?” to which respondents residing between 490 and 499 meters altitude are less likely to answer affirmatively, suggesting that these communities are isolated or that they suffered some shock (e.g., landslide) that limited access.

13 In an alternative interpretation of this test, the aid program could plausibly increase hostility between refugees who benefit from the program and non-recipients, but we find no evidence of this (at least for Syrian non-recipients). None of the Lebanon’s 12 Palestinian camps, where a majority of the country’s Palestinian refugees live, is located in our 450–550-meter altitude range. Therefore, our study does not speak to whether aid affects tensions between refugee populations from different countries of origin. This is an important question as qualitative evidence suggests that some Palestinians in Lebanon feel that they have suffered as a result of the Syrian refugee crisis in Lebanon and the humanitarian community’s attention to Syrians.

14 Results are generally robust to: (i) adding control variables, (ii) alternative discrete choice models such as probit or ordered logit, (iii) to excluding the few possibly contaminated control group households who moved from below to above the cutoff between November 2013 (program launch) and April 2014 (our survey), and (iv) to alternative clustering of standard errors.

15 This interpretation assumes no difference in labor supply or income between treatment and control group. Table A12 in the appendix provides support for this assumption, showing no statistically significant treatment effect for employment variables.

16 Respondents report that they rarely go to other communities to buy food (see descriptive statistics in the appendix). Casual labor in agriculture is a main source of employment in host communities, hence local shop owners likely obtain food items from local producers, who therefore also benefit, and part of these benefits may trickle down to casual laborers that local producers employ (e.g., demand for labor increases, implying more days of work, and hence more income for casual laborers).

17 Figure 3, for example, suggests an increase (albeit small) in the propensity of respondents to talk with Lebanese sometimes or often.

18 A relevant question is if the Lebanese population differs in winter and summer, thereby affecting the refugee to local ratio. Given our quasi-experimental design, we expect seasonal migration to be balanced around the cutoff, hence not affect the internal validity of our research design. Indeed, using data collected for each of our study sites on the size of the refugee population and the Lebanese population, we do not find any discontinuity at the cutoff in the ratio of refugees to locals. Seasonal migration, even though balanced around the cutoff, may affect the external validity of our treatment effects though: if the Lebanese population differs in winter and summer, our treatment effects for anti-refugee violence (which are confined to the winter months) are perhaps smaller than they would be during summer when there are more locals hence potential aggressors around. Our fieldwork provides qualitative evidence that during the summer months, some Lebanese from urban areas spend weekends in the mountains rather than the cities. With a larger Lebanese population in our study sites in the summer, the potential for contact between refugees and locals may be higher than in the winter months.

19 UNHCR has delivered winter cash programming every year since our study, intended to assist Syrians in Lebanon facing pronounced needs during winter. Largely due to variable annual funding, the target population has varied from 87,700 (winter 2013–2014), to 75,000 households (winter 2014–2015) to 175,000 (2017–2018) and 150,000 (2018–2019). There is journalistic evidence that today, after more than eight years of hosting Syrian refugees (and five years after our survey), Lebanese host communities are “out of patience” (e.g., https://foreignpolicy.com/2019/07/31/lebanon-is-sick-and-tired-of-syrian-refugees/) such that, for example, even a boost to the local economy caused by cash transfers to refugees may not be sufficient to reduce anti-refugee sentiments (i.e., the 2018–2019 cash transfer program, for example, may have had a smaller effect on reducing anti-refugee violence than our 2013–2014 program).

References

Alrababa’h, Ala’, Dillon, Andrea, Williamson, Scott, Hainmueller, Jens, Hangartner, Dominik, and Weinstein, Jeremy M.. 2019. “Attitudes toward Migrants in a Highly-impacted Economy: Evidence from the Syrian Refugee Crisis in Jordan.” IPL Working Paper No.19-01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bansak, Kirk, Hainmueller, Jens, and Hangartner, Dominik. 2016. “How Economic, Humanitarian, and Religious Concerns Shape European Attitudes toward Asylum Seekers.” Science 354 (6309): 217222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benček, David, and Strasheim, Julia. 2016. “Refugees Welcome? A Dataset on Anti-refugee Violence in Germany.” Research & Politics 3 (4): 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bobo, Lawrence, and Hutchings, Vincent L.. 1996. “Perceptions of Racial Group Competition: Extending Blumer’s Theory of Group Position to a Multiracial Social Context.” American Sociological Review 61 (6): 951972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braithwaite, Alex, Chu, Tiffany S., Curtis, Justin, and Ghosn, Faten. 2019. “Violence and the Perception of Risk Associated with Hosting Refugees.” Public Choice 178 (3): 473492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bundesministerium des Innern 2014. Verfassungsschutzbericht 2014, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Bundesministerium des Innern 2015. Verfassungsschutzbericht 2015, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Dahlberg, Matz, Edmark, Karin, and Lundqvist, Heléne. 2012. “Ethnic Diversity and Preferences for Redistribution.” Journal of Political Economy 120 (1): 4176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, Rafaela M. 2010. Immigration and Conflict in Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dustmann, Christian, Vasiljeva, Kristine, and Damm, Anna Piil. 2018. “Refugee Migration and Electoral Outcomes.” The Review of Economic Studies 86 (5): 20352091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, Armin, Kuhn, Andreas, and Zweimueller, Josef. 2011. “Unemployment and Right-wing Extremist Crime.” Scandinavian Journal of Economics 113 (2): 260285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghosn, Faten, Braithwaite, Alex, and Chu, Tiffany S.. 2019. “Violence, Displacement, Contact, and Attitudes toward Hosting Refugees.” Journal of Peace Research 56 (1): 118133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hangartner, Dominik, Dinas, Elias, Marbach, Moritz, Matakos, Konstantinos, and Xefteris, Dimitrios. 2019. “Does Exposure to the Refugee Crisis Make Natives More Hostile?American Political Science Review 113 (2): 442455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imbens, Guido W., and Lemieux, Thomas. 2008. “Regression Discontinuity Designs: A Guide to Practice.” Journal of Econometrics 142 (2): 615635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jäckle, Sebastian, and König, Pascal D.. 2017. “The Dark Side of the German ‘Welcome Culture’: Investigating the Causes behind Attacks on Refugees in 2015.” West European Politics 40 (2): 223251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koopmans, Ruud. 1996. “Explaining the Rise of Racist and Extreme Right Violence in Western Europe: Grievances or Opportunities?European Journal of Political Research 30 (2): 185216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marbach, Moritz, and Ropers, Guido. 2018. “Not in My Backyard: Do Increases in Immigration Cause Political Violence?” IPL Working Paper No. 18-02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mourad, Lama. 2019. “The Local Politics of Refugee Crises: Fragmentation and the Lebanese Response to the Syrian Refugee Crisis.” PhD diss. University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Onoma, Ato Kwamena. 2013. Anti-refugee Violence and African Politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UNDP (United Nations Development Programme). 2019. “Regular Perceptions Survey of Social Tensions throughout Lebanon, Wave VI.” UNDP and The ARK Group.Google Scholar
World Bank. 2013. Lebanon: Economic and Social Impact Assessment of the Syrian Conflict. Washington, D.C.: World Bank.Google Scholar
Zhou, Yang-Yang. 2018. Refugee Proximity and Support for Citizenship Exclusion in Africa. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association. https://www.yangyangzhou.com/s/Zhou_CitiExclusion.pdf.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Aid and Anti-refugee Violence

Figure 1

Figure 1. Aid and Anti-refugee Violence

Note: Graphs show parametric and nonparametric regressions on both sides of the 500-meter eligibility cutoff. The dependent variable in the left (right) graphs is a 1/0 dummy: 1 if Lebanese have “sometimes” or “often” been verbally (physically) aggressive to the respondent or members of the household in the past six months and 0 if “rarely” or “never.” The dots show the mean of the dependent variable by altitude bins of 10 meters. The size of each dot reflects the number of observations in that particular bin. The number of observations in the upper graphs is 1,356. The lower graphs exclude observations between 490 and 499 meters altitude, and the number of observations in that case is 1,335.
Figure 2

Figure 2. Aid and Anti-refugee Violence: Smaller Bandwidth

Note: The solid line shows ordinary least squares estimates of β in Equation 1 with a linear polynomial, for successively smaller bandwidths around the cutoff. Light (dark) gray areas indicate 95 (90)% confidence intervals. The dependent variable in the left (right) graphs is a 1/0 dummy: 1 if Lebanese have “sometimes” or “often” been verbally (physically) aggressive to the respondent or members of the household in the past six months and 0 if “rarely” or “never.” The graphs present results for all bandwidths from 50 meters (our full data, from 450 to 550 meters altitude) to 20 meters (from 480 to 520 meters altitude). We exclude bandwidths smaller than 20 meters, which subset the data to fewer than 100 observations on either side of the cutoff.
Figure 3

Figure 3. Possible Mechanisms

Note: Graphs show parametric and nonparametric regressions on both sides of the 500-meter eligibility cutoff. The dots show the mean of the dependent variable by altitude bins of 10 meters. The size of each dot reflects the number of observations in that particular bin. In the appendix we report ordinary least squares estimates of β for these and other dependent variables.
Supplementary material: Link

Lehmann and Masterson Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: PDF

Lehmann and Masterson supplementary material

Online Appendix

Download Lehmann and Masterson supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.7 MB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.