Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T10:25:43.886Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The association of social support networks and loneliness with negative perceptions of ageing: evidence from the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 December 2017

ZIGGI IVAN SANTINI*
Affiliation:
The Danish National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Copenhagen, Denmark.
AI KOYANAGI
Affiliation:
Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Universitat de Barcelona, Fundació Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Spain.
STEFANOS TYROVOLAS
Affiliation:
Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Universitat de Barcelona, Fundació Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Spain.
JOSEP MARIA HARO
Affiliation:
Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Universitat de Barcelona, Fundació Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Spain.
VIBEKE KOUSHEDE
Affiliation:
The Danish National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Copenhagen, Denmark.
*
Address for correspondence: Ziggi Santini, The Danish National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Oester Farimagsgade 5A, 1353 Copenhagen, Denmark E-mail: ziggi.santini@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

It is well known that negative ageing perceptions have various detrimental effects on indicators of successful ageing, but less is known about the role of social support networks and loneliness in ageing perceptions. The objective of this study was therefore to assess the association of social networks, relationship quality and loneliness with negative ageing perceptions in late life. Cross-sectional data on 6,912 adults aged ⩾50 years from the first wave of the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA) were analysed. Ageing perceptions were assessed with the Brief Ageing Perceptions Questionnaire. Information on social support networks, loneliness and socio-demographics were obtained using standard questions. Depressive symptoms were assessed with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale. Multivariable linear regression was conducted to assess the associations. Social isolation, poor relationship quality (with spouse, children, other family members or friends) and loneliness were all significantly associated with negative ageing perceptions even after adjustment for all potential confounders including depressive symptoms. Our study indicates that targeting integration into social support networks and improving relationship quality may potentially reduce the extent to which older individuals adopt negative ageing perceptions. Future studies with prospective design are warranted to understand the temporal direction and causal association of social support networks and loneliness with negative ageing perceptions.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Introduction

Both in Europe and overall globally, rapid ageing is currently taking place due to a combination of increasing life expectancies and declining birth rates (Rechel et al. Reference Rechel, Doyle, Grundy and McKee2009; United Nations 2015). This trend in demographic ageing is associated with increases in morbidity and disability, which in turn leads to increases in demand for health care and social services, while the declining working-age population will generate less income to ensure the sustainability of existing health systems (Rechel et al. Reference Rechel, Doyle, Grundy and McKee2009; World Health Organization 2011). It has thus become a top priority in European health policy to respond to this challenge by focusing strategies on successful ageing and ultimately increasing the number of years spent in good health (Rechel et al. Reference Rechel, Grundy, Robine, Cylus, Mackenbach, Knai and McKee2013). Evidence is increasing that such strategies increase healthy life expectancies, postpone health expenditure and have wider economic benefits (Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Doblhammer, Rau and Vaupel2009; Doyle, Mc Kee and Sherriff Reference Doyle, Mc Kee and Sherriff2012; Doyle et al. Reference Doyle, McKee, Rechel and Grundy2009; Gandjour Reference Gandjour2009; Gandjour and Lauterbach Reference Gandjour and Lauterbach2005).

A key component to successful ageing pertains to the subjective elements of the ageing process (Doyle, McKee and Sherriff Reference Doyle, Mc Kee and Sherriff2012; Wurm et al. Reference Wurm, Warner, Ziegelmann, Wolff and Schuz2013) such as ageing perceptions. Ageing perceptions are defined as the personal evaluation of one's own ageing process (Moser, Spagnoli and Santos-Eggimann Reference Moser, Spagnoli and Santos-Eggimann2011). Research has increasingly demonstrated that ageing perceptions have implications for physical and mental health (Levy Reference Levy2003). Importantly, ageing perceptions may involve a ‘self-fulfilling prophecy’ where a more positive outlook towards ageing tends to promote successful ageing and longevity, while negative appraisals of ageing appear to accelerate the ageing process considerably (Wurm et al. Reference Wurm, Warner, Ziegelmann, Wolff and Schuz2013). Specifically, positive perceptions of ageing have been found to be a predictor of engagement in preventive health behaviours (Levy and Myers Reference Levy and Myers2004), recovery from disability (Levy et al. Reference Levy, Slade, Murphy and Gill2012), and a protective factor in terms of functional health and longevity (Levy, Slade and Kasl Reference Levy, Slade and Kasl2002; Levy et al. Reference Levy, Slade, Kunkel and Kasl2002). In contrast, negative perceptions of ageing have been reported to be associated with various psychiatric outcomes, particularly depressive symptoms (Wurm and Benyamini Reference Wurm and Benyamini2014).

There is a dearth of studies on social context factors in relation to ageing perceptions in general. In particular, to our knowledge, very few studies report on associations between social relationships and ageing perceptions, despite the fact that social support networks and loneliness may theoretically be related to ageing perceptions. For example, one study has shown that being married or in a relationship is related to more positive attitudes to ageing as compared to those who do not have a partner (Bryant et al. Reference Bryant, Bei, Gilson, Komiti, Jackson and Judd2012). Another study showed that older adults who are less socially active also score higher on expressing negative ageing perceptions (Sanchez Palacios, Trianes Torres and Blanca Mena Reference Sanchez Palacios, Trianes Torres and Blanca Mena2009). Being better integrated in social networks may promote feelings of being socially active and having responsibilities for others, which in turn may produce positive feelings towards getting older (Ranzijn et al. Reference Ranzijn, Keeves, Luszcz and Feather1998; Stevens Reference Stevens1993). In addition, engaging in diverse social networks and feeling socially integrated (as opposed to feeling lonely) may result in greater resilience against prevalent negative ageing stereotypes that depict older people as socially isolated, impaired or dependent (Braithwaite Reference Braithwaite1986; Nuessel Reference Nuessel1982; Ory et al. Reference Ory, Kinney Hoffman, Hawkins, Sanner and Mockenhaupt2003; Scholl and Sabat Reference Scholl and Sabat2008). Finally, being embedded in more supportive networks may generate optimism or feelings of control, which could also have implications for how people interpret their own ageing process (Karademas Reference Karademas2006; Levy, Slade and Kasl Reference Levy, Slade and Kasl2002; VanderZee, Buunk and Sanderman Reference VanderZee, Buunk and Sanderman1997; Wurm and Benyamini Reference Wurm and Benyamini2014).

Furthermore, it is also important to consider the role of depression in this association. Depressive disorders are some of the most common disorders among older adults (Byers et al. Reference Byers, Yaffe, Covinsky, Friedman and Bruce2010), and strong associations of depression with both ageing perceptions (Chachamovich et al. Reference Chachamovich, Fleck, Laidlaw and Power2008; Richardson et al. Reference Richardson, Friedman, Podgorski, Knox, Fisher, He and Conwell2012; Wurm and Benyamini Reference Wurm and Benyamini2014) and social relationships (Santini et al. Reference Santini, Koyanagi, Tyrovolas, Mason and Haro2015b) have been reported. Depression may also be a common risk factor for both poor social support networks and negative ageing perceptions. For example, while depressive attitudes and thought patterns relate to a cynical or pessimistic outlook on life in general, it can be argued that negative perceptions of ageing involve similar cynical or pessimistic attitudes and beliefs focused specifically on one's ageing process.

Given the detrimental effects of negative perceptions of ageing on health and longevity, it is essential to reduce such perceptions in the population of older adults. One way that negative perceptions of ageing are activated may be through poor social relationships. However, to date, there are surprisingly few studies on the association between social relationships and ageing perceptions. Thus, using a large-scale, Irish nationally representative sample, we examined associations between relationship quality, social network integration, loneliness and negative perceptions of ageing among older adults. Based on the aforementioned literature documenting potential links between social relationships and negative ageing perceptions, we hypothesised that: (a) better relationship quality (from all sources) and greater social network integration would be inversely associated with negative perceptions of ageing; (b) loneliness would be associated with negative perceptions of ageing; and (c) depressive symptoms will at least partially explain these associations.

Methods

Study design and sample

The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA) is a nationally representative population-based survey of older adults residing in Ireland. The current analysis was based on Wave 1 conducted between October 2009 and February 2011. Full details of the surveys and their sampling procedure have been described elsewhere (Cronin et al. Reference Cronin, O'Regan, Finucane, Kearney and Kenny2013; Kearney et al. Reference Kearney, Cronin, O'Regan, Kamiya, Savva, Whelan and Kenny2011; Nolan et al. Reference Nolan, O'Regan, Dooley, Wallace, Hever, Cronin, Hudson and Kenny2014; Whelan and Savva Reference Whelan and Savva2013). In brief, the first wave comprised 8,175 people aged 50 years and above, along with an additional 329 spouses or partners younger than 50 years. The first wave excluded institutionalised individuals, anyone with known dementia or anyone unable to personally provide written informed consent to participate due to severe cognitive impairment.

Face-to-face interviews were conducted by trained interviewers using Computer Assisted Personal Interviewing. The survey also included a self-completion questionnaire that was returned after the visit. All respondents were asked to complete the self-completion questionnaire. The overall response rate for the first wave was 62 per cent and the response rate for the self-completion questionnaire was 84 per cent (Kearney et al. Reference Kearney, Cronin, O'Regan, Kamiya, Savva, Whelan and Kenny2011; Whelan and Savva Reference Whelan and Savva2013). Our analysis restricted the sample to: (a) participants aged 50 years and above; and (b) participants who completed the self-completion questionnaire, which included items for ageing perceptions and loneliness. The sample size after restriction to these individuals was 6,912. Ethical approval was obtained from the ethics committee of Trinity College Dublin. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Measures

Negative perceptions of ageing

Negative perceptions of ageing were assessed with the Brief Ageing Perceptions Questionnaire (B-APQ) (Sexton et al. Reference Sexton, King-Kallimanis, Morgan and McGee2014). This scale specifically measures internalised and subjective perceptions of one's own ageing process, such as ‘I always classify myself as old’, ‘Getting older makes me less independent’ or ‘As I get older I do not cope as well with problems that arise’. The scale consists of 17 items rated on a five-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), with six items reverse coded. Reverse-coded items were recoded so that all items were based on the same scale. Scores were summed to create a scale that ranged from 17 to 85, with higher scores indicating more negative perceptions of ageing. The B-APQ has been psychometrically validated for use with the Irish population aged ⩾50 years, and has been found to have good internal consistency and construct validity (Sexton et al. Reference Sexton, King-Kallimanis, Morgan and McGee2014). For the B-APQ , Cronbach's α = 0.83. The exact 17-item questionnaire and details regarding its development can be found in a previous publication (Sexton et al. Reference Sexton, King-Kallimanis, Morgan and McGee2014). The B-APQ can be seen in the Appendix.

Social networks

Social networks were assessed with the Berkman–Syme Social Network Index (SNI). The SNI is a validated self-report questionnaire (Berkman and Syme Reference Berkman and Syme1979) that assesses a person's degree of social integration by: marital/partnership status (married/with partner versus not), sociability (number and frequency of contact with children, close relatives and close friends), and church group membership or membership of community organisations. The composite score ranged from 0 to 4 and was categorised according to the standard categorisation described by Berkman and Syme (Reference Berkman and Syme1979) as 0–1 (most isolated) (reference category), 2 (moderately isolated), 3 (moderately integrated) and 4 (most integrated). Further information about the psychometric properties and evidence for the predictive validity of the SNI is provided elsewhere (Berkman and Breslow Reference Berkman and Breslow1983).

Relationship quality

Relationship quality was assessed in terms of participants’ experiences of social support and social strain from four sources: spouse, children, friends and other family members (brothers, sisters, parents, cousins, etc.) (Schuster, Kessler and Aseltine Reference Schuster, Kessler and Aseltine1990). Social support was measured with three items: ‘How much does he/she/they really understand the way you feel about things?’, ‘How much can you rely on him/her/them if you have a serious problem?’ and ‘How much can you open up to him/her/them if you need to talk about your worries?’ Social strain was measured with four items: ‘‘How much does he/she/they make too many demands on you?’, ‘‘How much does he/she/they criticise you?’, ‘‘How much does he/she/they let you down when you are counting on him/her?’’ and ‘How much does he/she/they get on your nerves?’ Each respondent was asked to answer the questions separately in regards to spouse, children, other family members and friends. Responses for social support and social strain were coded in the following way: 1 (not at all), 2 (a little), 3 (some) and 4 (a lot). The scores for social support and social strain from each source were summed separately to create scales ranging from 3 to 12 and 4 to 16, respectively. For the sake of comparability between social support and social strain, all scales were converted into scales ranging from 0 to 10, with higher scores indicating greater levels of social support or social strain (Santini et al. Reference Santini, Koyanagi, Tyrovolas and Haro2015a). Thus, the end result was two scales (social support and social strain) for each of the four sources (spouse, children, other family members, friends), resulting in eight scales in total. The alpha values were as follows: spouse support 0.86, spouse strain 0.78; children support 0.79, children strain 0.75; family support 0.86, family strain 0.76; and friends support 0.85, friends strain 0.74.

Loneliness

The short form of the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale was used to assess feelings of loneliness (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo2004; Russell, Peplau and Cutrona Reference Russell, Peplau and Cutrona1980). The short-form UCLA Loneliness Scale, which assesses subjective feelings of social isolation, is a commonly used measure in loneliness research. The dominant factor underlying the UCLA Loneliness Scale is ‘perceived social isolation’ (Austin Reference Austin1983; Russell Reference Russell1996). The UCLA three-item scale is comprised of three negatively worded questions relating to feelings of isolation, feeling left out and companionship. The three response options are coded as 1 (hardly ever), 2 (some of the time) and 3 (often). Scores are summed to create a total score that runs from 3 to 9, with higher scores indicating a greater degree of loneliness (Cronbach's α = 0.81). Previous research has indicated that this scale has an acceptable degree of reliability and has both concurrent and discriminant validity (Hughes et al. Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo2004).

Control variables

Socio-demographic characteristics included age, gender, education, financial circumstances and place of residence. Age was operationalised as a continuous variable. Place of residence was dichotomised into urban (Dublin city or county/another town or city) and rural. Education was classified as: primary (some primary/not complete, primary or equivalent) (reference category); secondary (intermediate/junior/group certificate or equivalent, leaving certificate or equivalent); and tertiary (diploma/certificate, primary degree, postgraduate/higher degree). Current employment situation was categorised as: employed (employed and self-employed, including farming) (reference category); retired; or unemployed, permanently sick or disabled, looking after home or family, and in education or training.

The number of chronic medical conditions was assessed by the question ‘has a doctor ever told you that you have any of the conditions on this card?’ Reponses included 17 conditions: high blood pressure or hypertension; angina; heart attack (including myocardial or coronary thrombosis); congestive heart failure; diabetes or high blood sugar; stroke (cerebral vascular disease); ministroke or transient ischemic attack; high cholesterol; heart murmur; abnormal heart rhythm; any other heart trouble; chronic lung disease such as chronic bronchitis or emphysema; asthma; arthritis (including osteoarthritis or rheumatism); osteoporosis; cancer or a malignant tumour (including leukaemia or lymphoma but excluding minor skin cancers); cirrhosis or serious liver damage. The total number of chronic medical conditions was calculated and operationalised as a continuous variable.

Difficulties with six types of activities of daily living (ADL; dressing, walking, bathing, eating, getting in or out of bed, and using the toilet; Katz et al. Reference Katz, Ford, Moskowitz, Jackson and Jaffe1963) were assessed by asking participants to indicate whether they had difficulty performing these activities. ADL disability was defined as having difficulty with at least one of these ADLs.

The scale used for depressive symptoms was the 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff Reference Radloff1977), which assesses symptoms experienced in the last week before the survey. The 20 items were scored on scales from 0 (rarely or none of the time, less than one day in the week) to 3 (most or all of the time, five to seven days in the week) with four items reverse coded. Reverse-coded items were recoded so that all items were based on the same scale. Scores were summed to create a scale that ranged from 0 to 60, with higher scores indicating more depressive symptoms (Cronbach's α = 0.88). The validity of the CES-D scale as a screening tool for depression in community-dwelling older adults has been well-documented (Hertzog et al. Reference Hertzog, Van Alstine, Usala, Hultsch and Dixon1990; Lewinsohn et al. Reference Lewinsohn, Seeley, Roberts and Allen1997).

Statistical analysis

We analysed data with Stata version 13.1 (StataCorp 2013). The mean ± standard deviation (SD) of the negative ageing perception score is presented by sample characteristics. The between-group difference of the negative ageing perception score was tested by Student's t-tests. Multivariable linear regression analyses were performed to assess the association between social networks, relationship quality from four sources or loneliness (exposure variables), and negative perceptions of ageing (outcome variable). Separate models were constructed for each exposure variable [i.e. social networks; relationship quality from each source (spouse, children, other family members, friends); loneliness]. Each analysis was therefore restricted to the participants to whom the variable applied (e.g. analyses pertaining to relationship quality with children included only those participants who had children).

In order to assess how the inclusion of different covariates influence the association between social networks, relationship quality or loneliness, and negative ageing perceptions, we conducted hierarchical analysis by constructing three models: Model 1 – adjusted for age, gender, place of residence, education, current employment situation, disability and number of chronic medical conditions; and Model 2 – adjusted for covariates in Model 1 and depressive symptoms. The selection of these variables was based on previous literature [relating to predictors of ageing perceptions (Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, Kotter-Gruhn and Smith Reference Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, Kotter-Gruhn and Smith2008; Kruse and Schmitt Reference Kruse and Schmitt2006; Westerhof and Barrett Reference Westerhof and Barrett2005) and social support networks (Broese van Groenou and Van Tilburg Reference Broese van Groenou and Van Tilburg2003; Shumaker and Hill Reference Shumaker and Hill1991; Smith and Christakis Reference Smith and Christakis2008; Uchino Reference Uchino2006)]. In particular, we were interested in assessing the effect of depressive symptoms in this association for its theoretically strong link with both the exposure and outcome variables (Chachamovich et al. Reference Chachamovich, Fleck, Laidlaw and Power2008; Richardson et al. Reference Richardson, Friedman, Podgorski, Knox, Fisher, He and Conwell2012; Santini et al. Reference Santini, Koyanagi, Tyrovolas, Mason and Haro2015b; Wurm and Benyamini Reference Wurm and Benyamini2014). With the exception of age, number of medical conditions and depressive symptoms, all variables were included in the models as categorical variables. Sampling weights were generated with respect to age, sex and educational attainment to the Quarterly National Household Survey (Central Statistics Office 2010). The sample weighting and the complex study design were taken into account in all analyses to generate nationally representative estimates (Barrett et al. Reference Barrett, Savva, Timonen and Kenny2011). Results are expressed as coefficients and their 95 per cent confidence intervals. A p-value less than 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

The mean (SD) age of the sample was 63.6 (9.3) years and 52.1 per cent of the sample were women. Male sex, lower education, not being employed, rural residence, ADL disability and higher depressive symptoms were all significantly associated with higher negative ageing perception scores (Table 1). Finally, in terms of the variables on social relationships, significantly higher levels of negative ageing perceptions were observed among those who were more socially isolated and lonely, and those with relationships characterised by more social strain and less social support (Table 2). The associations between social networks, relationship quality, loneliness and negative perceptions of ageing estimated by multivariable linear regression are shown in Table 3. All predictors were significantly associated with negative perceptions of ageing even in the fully adjusted model where higher levels of social network integration and social support from all sources were inversely associated with negative ageing perceptions, while social strain and loneliness were positively associated with negative ageing perceptions. When depressive symptoms were added to the adjusted model, there was a moderate to large attenuation in the coefficients for all exposure variables, although the statistical significance remained unchanged, with the exception of two categories of the SNI.

Table 1. Negative ageing perception scores by socio-demographic characteristics and health-related variables

Notes: The scale used for negative ageing perceptions ranged from 17 to 85, with higher scores indicating more negative perceptions of ageing. Sampling weights were used for the calculation of means and standard deviations (SD); p-values are based on Student's t-tests. ADL: activity of daily living.

Table 2. Negative ageing perceptions scores by level of social networks, relationship quality and loneliness

Notes: The scale used for negative ageing perceptions ranged from 17 to 85, with higher scores indicating more negative perceptions of ageing. Sampling weights were used for the calculation of means and standard deviations (SD); p-values are based on Student's t-tests.

Table 3. Association of social networks, relationship quality and loneliness (independent variables) with negative ageing perceptions scores (dependent variable) among older adults estimated by linear regression

Notes: The scale used for negative ageing perceptions ranged from 17 to 85, with higher scores indicating more negative perceptions of ageing. Model 1: adjusted for age, gender, place of residence, education, current employment situation, disability and number of chronic medical conditions. Model 2: adjusted for covariates in Model 1 and depressive symptoms. Ref.: reference category.

Significance levels: * p < 0.01, ** p < 0.001.

Discussion

The current study investigated the associations of social support networks, relationship quality and loneliness with negative perceptions of ageing among older adults aged 50 and above in Ireland. Greater social network integration was inversely associated with negative perceptions of ageing. Similarly, better relationship quality with social ties (spouse, children, other family members, friends) was also associated with perceptions of ageing, where greater social support and higher levels of social strain were related to lower and higher levels of negative ageing perceptions, respectively. Finally, loneliness was positively associated with negative perceptions of ageing. Our findings demonstrate that poor social support networks and loneliness are associated with negative perceptions of ageing. While the coefficients of these associations were attenuated after the inclusion of depressive symptoms in the model, it did not fully explain the associations.

Contextualisation of findings

Although knowledge on the mechanisms linking objective and subjective measures of social support networks and negative ageing perceptions is scarce due to little prior research on this topic, several hypotheses may be proposed. First, as mentioned previously, the action of internalising ageing stereotypes into constructs of one's self-perceptions of ageing has been postulated to result in a ‘self-fulfilling prophecy’ of the stereotype (Levy, Slade and Kasl Reference Levy, Slade and Kasl2002; Levy et al. Reference Levy, Slade, Kunkel and Kasl2002; Wurm et al. Reference Wurm, Warner, Ziegelmann, Wolff and Schuz2013). Older adults are often perceived as being boring, weak, mournful, debilitated, cognitively impaired, demented, dependent, helpless or incompetent (Braithwaite Reference Braithwaite1986; Nuessel Reference Nuessel1982; Ory et al. Reference Ory, Kinney Hoffman, Hawkins, Sanner and Mockenhaupt2003; Scholl and Sabat Reference Scholl and Sabat2008). This can lead to negative self-stereotyping, which occurs when these stereotypes are internalised (Kruse and Schmitt Reference Kruse and Schmitt2006; Levy Reference Levy2009). It is possible that the adoption of such negative stereotypes is facilitated and reinforced under adverse social circumstances. It may be that more socially isolated individuals over time tend to more readily ascribe their situation to negative ageing stereotypes, perceiving that loneliness and social isolation are simply necessary consequences of getting older (Sanchez Palacios, Trianes Torres and Blanca Mena Reference Sanchez Palacios, Trianes Torres and Blanca Mena2009). It may also be that older adults who consistently experience poor relationship quality with their social ties at some point begin to attribute this to the fact that they are transitioning into older adulthood. Hence, in line with negative ageing stereotypes, little support or high strain may become interpreted as part of the normal ageing process where older people are given less priority or they are seen as being more difficult to deal with (e.g. in the case of cognitive impairment or dementia) (Braithwaite Reference Braithwaite1986; Nuessel Reference Nuessel1982; Ory et al. Reference Ory, Kinney Hoffman, Hawkins, Sanner and Mockenhaupt2003; Scholl and Sabat Reference Scholl and Sabat2008).

Second, the internalisation of negative ageing stereotypes, such as being ‘alone and lonely’, ‘dependent’ or ‘inactive’ is potentially further reinforced by several naturally occurring psycho-social factors as people transition into older adulthood and old age, such as declining social roles (Dykstra and de Jong Gierveld Reference Dykstra and de Jong Gierveld2004; Dykstra, van Tilburg and de Jong Gierveld Reference Dykstra, van Tilburg and de Jong Gierveld2005), the loss of network members due to bereavement (Ha Reference Ha2008; Utz et al. Reference Utz, Carr, Nesse and Wortman2002), and increasing dependency on others for formal and informal care (Ovseiko Reference Ovseiko2007). Such factors may contribute to the extent to which individuals ascribe to negative ageing stereotypes, and by extension, adopt negative perceptions of their own ageing process. Thus, another self-fulfilling prophecy may occur, where diminishing social support networks and perceived social isolation facilitate the adoption of negative perceptions of ageing, which ultimately confirm negative ageing stereotypes by further isolating individuals and reinforcing dependency. For example, research has previously reported that both social isolation/withdrawal and adoption of negative ageing stereotypes can lead to dependency and action inhibition (Barder, Slimmer and LeSage Reference Barder, Slimmer and LeSage1994; Coudin and Alexopoulos Reference Coudin and Alexopoulos2010; Rodin Reference Rodin1989). Conversely, more supportive social networks have been linked to optimism and perceived control, and ultimately psychological wellbeing (Karademas Reference Karademas2006; VanderZee, Buunk and Sanderman Reference VanderZee, Buunk and Sanderman1997). Optimism and perceived control have previously been shown to buffer against the detrimental effects of negative ageing perceptions (Levy, Slade and Kasl Reference Levy, Slade and Kasl2002; Wurm and Benyamini Reference Wurm and Benyamini2014). Thus, it is possible that supportive relationships facilitate more perceived control or a more optimistic outlook, which in turn reinforces a more positive view of one's ageing process and ultimately decelerates the ageing process.

Last, having poor social relationships has been reported to be an independent risk factor for developing various adverse physical and mental health outcomes among older adults (Holt-Lunstad, Smith and Layton Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith and Layton2010; Kuiper et al. Reference Kuiper, Zuidersma, Oude Voshaar, Zuidema, van den Heuvel, Stolk and Smidt2015; Santini et al. Reference Santini, Koyanagi, Tyrovolas, Mason and Haro2015b; Smith and Christakis Reference Smith and Christakis2008; Uchino Reference Uchino2009). Hence, it can be argued that the protective effects of being embedded in good social support networks imply a deceleration of the ageing process, which may effectively reduce the likelihood of adopting negative attitudes to getting older. Further, it may also be important to consider not only the size of social networks or the quality of relationships, but also the characteristics of the people to whom individuals are socially connected. Recent literature has highlighted the role of social comparison on ageing perceptions among older people (Bennett and Gaines Reference Bennett and Gaines2010; Horton et al. Reference Horton, Baker, Côté and Deakin2008a, Reference Horton, Baker, Pearce and Deakin2008b). For example, the lack of intergenerational interaction between the old and the young has been cited as one essential factor contributing to the adoption of negative ageing perceptions (Ory et al. Reference Ory, Kinney Hoffman, Hawkins, Sanner and Mockenhaupt2003; Weiss, Sassenberg and Freund Reference Weiss, Sassenberg and Freund2013). In other words, norms of age segregation imply that older adults only socialise and associate themselves with other older adults, which in turn may perpetuate negative ageing stereotypes and cause individuals to interpret their own ageing in a negative light.

Our findings demonstrate a link between ageing perceptions among older individuals and their integration into social networks or the quality of their existing relationships. If our findings are confirmed by longitudinal studies, it may suggest that strategies to increase social network size or improve relationship quality could have a positive impact on ageing perceptions among the older population. While there are no studies on the effect of targeting social support networks to increase positive ageing perceptions, ageing-related cognitions and the extent to which individuals ascribe to negative stereotypes of ageing can be modified under experimental conditions (Levy Reference Levy1996, Reference Levy2003; Weiss, Sassenberg and Freund Reference Weiss, Sassenberg and Freund2013). Further, interventions targeting the social support networks of older adults can effectively reduce social isolation and loneliness, promote healthy social interaction and facilitate community participation, which in turn have various beneficial effects on mental health and psychological wellbeing (Alves, Maia and Nardi Reference Alves, Maia and Nardi2014; Cattan et al. Reference Cattan, White, Bond and Learmouth2005). It is likely that such interventions, by increasing social participation and integration into social support networks, for example, could also have favourable effects on how individuals perceive their own ageing process and the extent to which they view themselves as confirming or disconfirming negative ageing stereotypes.

It should also be noted that online social network platforms, such as Facebook, can be powerful in terms of influencing general perceptions of ageing (Levy et al. Reference Levy, Chung, Bedford and Navrazhina2014). Given the increasing use of online media among older adults (Zickuhr and Madden Reference Zickuhr and Madden2012), intervening within the context of such social media sites could potentially be an effective tool to reduce negative ageing stereotypes and to promote further more positive ageing perceptions among the general public or specifically through the online social networks of older adults.

A number of different ways exist to intervene in social support networks, e.g. by improving social skills, enhancing social support, increasing opportunities for social contact or addressing maladaptive social cognition (e.g. therapeutic interventions aimed at altering thought processes related to social interaction) (Heaney and Israel Reference Heaney, Israel, Glanz, Rimer and Viswanath2008; Masi et al. Reference Masi, Chen, Hawkley and Cacioppo2011). A meta-analysis covering 30 social network interventions designed to reduce social isolation and loneliness among older adults found that educational and social activity group interventions were effective, whereas the effectiveness of home visiting and befriending schemes remained unclear (Cattan et al. Reference Cattan, White, Bond and Learmouth2005). In terms of reducing loneliness specifically, Masi et al. (Reference Masi, Chen, Hawkley and Cacioppo2011) conducted a meta-analysis across 50 intervention studies and found a larger effect size for interventions that addressed maladaptive social cognition than for interventions designed to improve social skills, enhance support or increase opportunities for social interaction.

Finally, it is worth considering that research has established that core social support networks are generally formed in young adulthood, which ultimately has implications for late-life emotional wellbeing (English and Carstensen Reference English and Carstensen2014). Similarly, perceptions of ageing are usually entrenched earlier in life and can have consequences for health and functioning later in life (Levy Reference Levy2003; Levy, Slade and Kasl Reference Levy, Slade and Kasl2002; Levy et al. Reference Levy, Zonderman, Slade and Ferrucci2009). This means that it may be possible to assess both social support networks and perceptions of ageing at a time considerably before the transition into older adulthood, which in turn could prevent various adverse physical and mental health outcomes occurring years, perhaps even decades, into the future.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study include the use of data from a large nationally representative sample of the Irish population, and a validated scale to assess self-perceptions of ageing. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to assess associations of social networks, relationship quality from four different sources (including both social support and social strain) and loneliness with negative perceptions of ageing. Our results contribute to the existing gerontology literature and could effectively inform policy makers about how interpersonal relationships and social context factors influence ageing perceptions in the general population. However, the results of our study should be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, these findings were based on cross-sectional data, which precludes the possibility of making firm conclusions about directions of causality. For example, social isolation could also be the result of negative perceptions of ageing. Future studies are needed to establish causal relationships between these variables using prospective data. Second, since the data were based on self-report, reporting bias may exist. Third, other factors may mediate the relationship between social relationships and ageing perceptions, such as optimism or locus of control. We did not have proper measures available to test such mediating effects, and future studies testing these pathways are therefore relevant. Finally, residual confounding may exist due to the omission of potential confounders such as personality type due to lack of data (Bryant et al. Reference Bryant, Bei, Gilson, Komiti, Jackson and Judd2016; Moor et al. Reference Moor, Zimprich, Schmitt and Kliegel2006). Thus, their independent and confounding effects remain unknown.

Conclusion

Our findings show that social support networks and perceived social isolation are associated with ageing perceptions. Specifically, social isolation, poor relationship quality with social ties and loneliness are all related to negative perceptions of ageing in older Irish adults. It is possible that public health interventions can effectively reduce the extent to which older individuals adopt negative appraisals of their ageing process by targeting their integration into social support networks. Longitudinal studies are warranted to investigate the predictive validity of social support networks and loneliness on self-perceptions of ageing over time, and further, to explore the extent to which such links could become essential in decelerating the ageing process and ultimately promote successful ageing.

Acknowledgements

The TILDA data have been co-funded by the Government of Ireland through the Office of the Minister for Health and Children, by Atlantic Philanthropies and by Irish Life, and have been collected under the Statistics Act, 1993, of the Central Statistics Office. The project has been designed and implemented by the TILDA Study Team © Department of Health and Children. Copyright and all other intellectual property rights relating to the data are vested in TILDA. A. Koyanagi's work was supported by the Miguel Servet contract financed by the CP13/00150 project, integrated into the National R + D + I and funded by the ISCIII – General Branch Evaluation and Promotion of Health Research – and the European Regional Development Fund.

Appendix

Questions included in the Brief Ageing Perceptions Questionnaire (B-APQ)

Answer options were: ‘strongly disagree’, ‘disagree’, ‘neither agree nor disagree’, ‘agree’ and ‘strongly agree’.

  1. 1. I always classify myself as old.

  2. 2. I am always aware of the fact that I am getting older.

  3. 3. I feel my age in everything that I do.

  4. 4. As I get older I get wiser.

  5. 5. As I get older I continue to grow as a person.

  6. 6. As I get older I appreciate things more.

  7. 7. I get depressed when I think about how ageing might affect the things that I can do.

  8. 8. The quality of my social life in later years depends on me.

  9. 9. The quality of my relationships with others in later life depends on me.

  10. 10. Whether I continue living life to the full depends on me.

  11. 11. Getting older makes me less independent.

  12. 12. As I get older I can take part in fewer activities.

  13. 13. As I get older I do not cope as well with problems that arise.

  14. 14. Slowing down with age is not something I can control.

  15. 15. I have no control over the effects which getting older has on my social life.

  16. 16. I worry about the effects that getting older may have on my relationships with others.

  17. 17. I feel angry when I think about getting older.

References

Alves, V. d. M., Maia, A. C. C. d. O. and Nardi, A. E. 2014. Suicide among elderly: a systematic review. MedicalExpress, 1, 1, 913.Google Scholar
Austin, B. A. 1983. Factorial structure of the UCLA Loneliness Scale. Psychological Reports, 53, 3, 883–89.Google Scholar
Barder, L., Slimmer, L. and LeSage, J. 1994. Depression and issues of control among elderly people in health care settings. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 20, 4, 597604.Google Scholar
Barrett, A., Savva, G., Timonen, V. and Kenny, R. A. (eds) 2011. Fifty Plus in Ireland 2011: First Results from The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA). The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing, Dublin.Google Scholar
Bennett, T. and Gaines, J. 2010. Believing what you hear: the impact of aging stereotypes upon the old. Educational Gerontology, 36, 5, 435–45.Google Scholar
Berkman, L. F. and Breslow, L. 1983. Health and Ways of Living. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Berkman, L. F. and Syme, S. L. 1979. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. American Journal of Epidemiology, 109, 2, 186204.Google Scholar
Braithwaite, V. A. 1986. Old age stereotypes: reconciling contradictions. Journal of Gerontology, 41, 3, 353–60.Google Scholar
Broese van Groenou, M. and Van Tilburg, T. G. 2003. Network size and support in old age: differentials by socio-economic status in childhood and adulthood. Ageing & Society, 23, 5, 625–45.Google Scholar
Bryant, C., Bei, B., Gilson, K., Komiti, A., Jackson, H. and Judd, F. 2012. The relationship between attitudes to aging and physical and mental health in older adults. International Psychogeriatriatrics, 24, 10, 1674–83.Google Scholar
Bryant, C., Bei, B., Gilson, K. M., Komiti, A., Jackson, H. and Judd, F. 2014. Antecedents of attitudes to aging: a study of the roles of personality and well-being. Gerontologist, 56, 2, 256–65.Google Scholar
Byers, A. L., Yaffe, K., Covinsky, K. E., Friedman, M. B. and Bruce, M. L. 2010. High occurrence of mood and anxiety disorders among older adults: the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 5, 489–96.Google Scholar
Cattan, M., White, M., Bond, J. and Learmouth, A. 2005. Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: a systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing & Society, 25, 1, 4167.Google Scholar
Central Statistics Office 2010. Quarterly National Household Survey. Central Statistics Office, Cork, Republic of Ireland.Google Scholar
Chachamovich, E., Fleck, M., Laidlaw, K. and Power, M. 2008. Impact of major depression and subsyndromal symptoms on quality of life and attitudes toward aging in an international sample of older adults. Gerontologist, 48, 5, 593602.Google Scholar
Christensen, K., Doblhammer, G., Rau, R. and Vaupel, J. W. 2009. Ageing populations: the challenges ahead. Lancet, 374, 9696, 1196–208.Google Scholar
Coudin, G. and Alexopoulos, T. 2010. ‘Help me! I'm old!’ How negative aging stereotypes create dependency among older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 14, 5, 516–23.Google Scholar
Cronin, H., O'Regan, C., Finucane, C., Kearney, P. and Kenny, R. A. 2013. Health and aging: development of the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing health assessment. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 61, supplement 2, S26978.Google Scholar
Doyle, Y., McKee, M., Rechel, B. and Grundy, E. 2009. Meeting the challenge of population ageing. BMJ, 339, b3926.Google Scholar
Doyle, Y. G., Mc Kee, M. and Sherriff, M. 2012. A model of successful ageing in British populations. European Journal of Public Health, 22, 1, 71–6.Google Scholar
Dykstra, P. A. and de Jong Gierveld, J. 2004. Gender and marital-history differences in emotional and social loneliness among Dutch older adults. Canadian Journal of Aging, 23, 2, 141–55.Google Scholar
Dykstra, P. A., van Tilburg, T. G. and de Jong Gierveld, J. 2005. Changes in older adult loneliness: results from a seven-year longitudinal study. Research on Aging, 27, 725–47.Google Scholar
English, T. and Carstensen, L. L. 2014. Selective narrowing of social networks across adulthood is associated with improved emotional experience in daily life. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 2, 195202.Google Scholar
Gandjour, A. 2009. Aging diseases – do they prevent preventive health care from saving costs? Health Economics, 18, 3, 355–62.Google Scholar
Gandjour, A. and Lauterbach, K. W. 2005. Does prevention save costs? Considering deferral of the expensive last year of life. Journal of Health Economics, 24, 4, 715–24.Google Scholar
Ha, J. 2008. Changes in support from confidants, children, and friends following widowhood. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70, 306–18.Google Scholar
Heaney, C. A. and Israel, B. A. 2008. Social networks and social support. In Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K. and Viswanath, K. (eds), Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice, pp. 189–207. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Hertzog, C., Van Alstine, J., Usala, P. D., Hultsch, D. F. and Dixon, R. 1990. Measurement properties of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) in older populations. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2, 1, 6472.Google Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B. and Layton, J. B. 2010. Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7, 7, 120.Google Scholar
Horton, S., Baker, J., Côté, J. and Deakin, J. M. 2008 a. Understanding seniors’ perceptions and stereotypes of aging. Educational Gerontology, 34, 11, 9971017.Google Scholar
Horton, S., Baker, J., Pearce, G. W. and Deakin, J. M. 2008 b. On the malleability of performance: implications for seniors. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 27, 4, 446–65.Google Scholar
Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C. and Cacioppo, J. T. 2004. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging, 26, 6, 665–72.Google Scholar
Karademas, E. C. 2006. Self-efficacy, social support and well-being: the mediating role of optimism. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 6, 1281–90.Google Scholar
Katz, S., Ford, A. B., Moskowitz, R. W., Jackson, B. A. and Jaffe, M. W. 1963. Studies of illness in the aged. The Index of ADL: a standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. Journal of the American Medical Association, 185, 914–9.Google Scholar
Kearney, P. M., Cronin, H., O'Regan, C., Kamiya, Y., Savva, G. M., Whelan, B. and Kenny, R. 2011. Cohort profile: the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing. International Journal of Epidemiology, 40, 4, 877–84.Google Scholar
Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn, A., Kotter-Gruhn, D. and Smith, J. 2008. Self-perceptions of aging: do subjective age and satisfaction with aging change during old age? Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63B, 6, P37785.Google Scholar
Kruse, A. and Schmitt, E. 2006. A multidimensional scale for the measurement of agreement with age stereotypes and the salience of age in social interaction. Ageing & Society, 26, 393411.Google Scholar
Kuiper, J. S., Zuidersma, M., Oude Voshaar, R. C., Zuidema, S. U., van den Heuvel, E. R., Stolk, R. P. and Smidt, N. 2015. Social relationships and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Research Reviews, 22, 3957.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R. 1996. Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 6, 1092–107.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R. 2003. Mind matters: cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58B, 4, P20311.Google Scholar
Levy, B. 2009. Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging. Current Direction in Psychological Science, 18, 6, 332–6.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R., Chung, P. H., Bedford, T. and Navrazhina, K. 2014. Facebook as a site for negative age stereotypes. Gerontologist, 54, 2, 172–6.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R. and Myers, L. M. 2004. Preventive health behaviors influenced by self-perceptions of aging. Preventive Medicine, 39, 3, 625–9.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D. and Kasl, S. V. 2002. Longitudinal benefit of positive self-perceptions of aging on functional health. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, 5, P40917.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Kunkel, S. R. and Kasl, S. V. 2002. Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 2, 261–70.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Murphy, T. E. and Gill, T. M. 2012. Association between positive age stereotypes and recovery from disability in older persons. Journal of the American Medical Association, 308, 19, 1972–3.Google Scholar
Levy, B. R., Zonderman, A. B., Slade, M. D. and Ferrucci, L. 2009. Age stereotypes held earlier in life predict cardiovascular events in later life. Psychological Science, 20, 3, 296–8.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., Roberts, R. E. and Allen, N. B. 1997. Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) as a screening instrument for depression among community-residing older adults. Psychology and Aging, 12, 2, 277–87.Google Scholar
Masi, C. M., Chen, H. Y., Hawkley, L. C. and Cacioppo, J. T. 2011. A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 3, 219–66.Google Scholar
Moor, C., Zimprich, D., Schmitt, M. and Kliegel, M. 2006. Personality, aging self-perceptions, and subjective health: a mediation model. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 63, 3, 241–57.Google Scholar
Moser, C., Spagnoli, J. and Santos-Eggimann, B. 2011. Self-perception of aging and vulnerability to adverse outcomes at the age of 65–70 years. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B, 6, 675–80.Google Scholar
Nolan, A., O'Regan, C., Dooley, C., Wallace, D., Hever, A., Cronin, H., Hudson, E. and Kenny, R. A. (eds), 2014. The Over 50s in a Changing Ireland: Economic Circumstances, Health, and Well-being. The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing, Dublin.Google Scholar
Nuessel, F. H. Jr. 1982. The language of ageism. Gerontologist, 22, 3, 273–6.Google Scholar
Ory, M., Kinney Hoffman, M., Hawkins, M., Sanner, B. and Mockenhaupt, R. 2003. Challenging aging stereotypes: strategies for creating a more active society. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 25, 3 supplement 2, 164–71.Google Scholar
Ovseiko, P. 2007. Long-term care for older people. Aging Horizons Brief, 114. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.1.1018.0647.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S. 1977. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.Google Scholar
Ranzijn, R., Keeves, J., Luszcz, M. and Feather, N. T. 1998. The role of self-perceived usefulness and competence in the self-esteem of elderly adults: confirmatory factor analyses of the Bachman revision of Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 53B, 2, P96104.Google Scholar
Rechel, B., Doyle, Y., Grundy, E. and McKee, M. 2009. How Can Health Systems Respond to Population Ageing? World Health Organization, Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Rechel, B., Grundy, E., Robine, J. M., Cylus, J., Mackenbach, J. P., Knai, C. and McKee, M. 2013. Ageing in the European Union. Lancet, 381, 9874, 1312–22.Google Scholar
Richardson, T. M., Friedman, B., Podgorski, C., Knox, K., Fisher, S., He, H. and Conwell, Y. 2012. Depression and its correlates among older adults accessing aging services. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20, 4, 346–54.Google Scholar
Rodin, J. 1989. Sense of control: potentials for intervention. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 503, 2942.Google Scholar
Russell, D. W. 1996. UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 1, 2040.Google Scholar
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A. and Cutrona, C. E. 1980. The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 3, 472–80.Google Scholar
Sanchez Palacios, C., Trianes Torres, M. V. and Blanca Mena, M. J. 2009. Negative aging stereotypes and their relation with psychosocial variables in the elderly population. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 48, 3, 385–90.Google Scholar
Santini, Z. I., Koyanagi, A., Tyrovolas, S. and Haro, J. M. 2015 a. The association of relationship quality and social networks with depression, anxiety, and suicidal ideation among older married adults: findings from a cross-sectional analysis of the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA). Journal of Affective Disorders, 179, 134–41.Google Scholar
Santini, Z. I., Koyanagi, A., Tyrovolas, S., Mason, C. and Haro, J. M. 2015 b. The association between social relationships and depression: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175, 5365.Google Scholar
Scholl, J. M. and Sabat, S. R. 2008. Stereotypes, stereotype threat and ageing: implications for the understanding and treatment of people with Alzheimer's disease. Ageing & Society, 28, 103–30.Google Scholar
Schuster, T. L., Kessler, R. C. and Aseltine, R. H. Jr. 1990. Supportive interactions, negative interactions, and depressed mood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 3, 423–38.Google Scholar
Sexton, E., King-Kallimanis, B. L., Morgan, K. and McGee, H. 2014. Development of the brief ageing perceptions questionnaire (B-APQ): a confirmatory factor analysis approach to item reduction. BMC Geriatrics, 14, 44.Google Scholar
Shumaker, S. A. and Hill, D. R. 1991. Gender differences in social support and physical health. Health Psychology, 10, 2, 102–11.Google Scholar
Smith, K. P. and Christakis, N. A. 2008. Social networks and health. Annual Review of Sociology, 34, 405–29.Google Scholar
StataCorp. 2013. Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. StataCorp LP College Station, Texas.Google Scholar
Stevens, E. S. 1993. Making sense of utility: an avenue towards satisfaction in later life. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 37, 313–25.Google Scholar
Uchino, B. N. 2006. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 4, 377–87.Google Scholar
Uchino, B. N. 2009. Understanding the links between social support and physical health: a life-span perspective with emphasis on the separability of perceived and received support. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 236–55.Google Scholar
United Nations 2015. World Population Ageing 2015. Department of Economics and Social Affairs, Population Division, United Nations, New York.Google Scholar
Utz, R. L., Carr, D., Nesse, R. and Wortman, C. B. 2002. The effect of widowhood on older adults’ social participation: an evaluation of activity, disengagement, and continuity theories. Gerontologist, 42, 4, 522–33.Google Scholar
VanderZee, K. I., Buunk, B. R. and Sanderman, R. 1997. Social support, locus of control, and psychological well-being. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 20, 1842–59.Google Scholar
Weiss, D., Sassenberg, K. and Freund, A. M. 2013. When feeling different pays off: how older adults can counteract negative age-related information. Psychology and Aging, 28, 4, 1140–6.Google Scholar
Westerhof, G. J. and Barrett, A. E. 2005. Age identity and subjective well-being: a comparison of the United States and Germany. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B, 3, S12936.Google Scholar
Whelan, B. J. and Savva, G. M. 2013. Design and methodology of the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 61, supplement 2, S2658.Google Scholar
World Health Organization 2011. Global Health and Aging. World Health Organization, Geneva.Google Scholar
Wurm, S. and Benyamini, Y. 2014. Optimism buffers the detrimental effect of negative self-perceptions of ageing on physical and mental health. Psychology & Health, 29, 7, 832–48.Google Scholar
Wurm, S., Warner, L. M., Ziegelmann, J. P., Wolff, J. K. and Schuz, B. 2013. How do negative self-perceptions of aging become a self-fulfilling prophecy? Psychology and Aging, 28, 4, 1088–97.Google Scholar
Zickuhr, K. and Madden, M. 2012. Older Adults and Internet Use. Pew Research Center, Washington DC.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Negative ageing perception scores by socio-demographic characteristics and health-related variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Negative ageing perceptions scores by level of social networks, relationship quality and loneliness

Figure 2

Table 3. Association of social networks, relationship quality and loneliness (independent variables) with negative ageing perceptions scores (dependent variable) among older adults estimated by linear regression