Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:42:02.078Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory impairment in chronic pain patients and the related neuropsychological mechanisms: a review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2013

Xianhua Liu
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Science, Hengyang Normal University, Hengyang, Hunan, China
Li Li
Affiliation:
Psychiatry Institute, Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, Hunan, China Psychological Counseling Centre, Guizhou Finance and Economics University, Guiyang, Guizhou, China
Fanggui Tang
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Science, Hengyang Normal University, Hengyang, Hunan, China
Siwei Wu
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Science, Hengyang Normal University, Hengyang, Hunan, China
Yiqiu Hu*
Affiliation:
Science Technology Normal College, Hunan Agriculture University, Changsha, Hunan, China
*
Yiqiu Hu, Science Technology Normal College, Hunan Agriculture University, Changsha, Hunan 410128, P. R. China. Tel: +13 574 828338; Fax: +86 734 8484945; E-mail: yiqiuhu@yahoo.cn
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

This study provides a comprehensive review of the literature on memory impairment and the potential effective factors in patients with chronic pain.

Methods

A literature search of databases PubMed, EMBASE, SpringerLink, and PsycINFO until September 2012 was conducted using the keywords ‘memory’ and ‘chronic pain’. The study emphasises on publications over the past 20 years.

Results

Memory impairment in chronic pain patients is substantial, but the aspects of memory (e.g. working memory, long-term memory, and autobiographical memory) in chronic pain patients and the potentially related factors (e.g. age, level of education, pain conditions, emotion, neural network, and use of analgesics) are modest. Memory impairment is interpreted with the attention-narrowing hypothesis and the capacity-reduction hypothesis.

Conclusions

The currently available data and theory have explained memory impairment in chronic pain patients, but many controversies remain. Future research should focus on the subclinical characteristics of chronic pain, enlarging the sample size, and emphasise on the experimental intervention method and the cognitive neuroscience method.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 2013 

Summations

  • The findings in the literature show impaired memory functions in chronic pain patients.

  • The findings suggest that memory dysfunction in chronic pain patients is partially attributable to depressive mood and pain intensity, whereas other potential effective factors remain controversial.

  • Most of the existing studies on chronic pain have consistently shown that pain and memory share a common central nervous pathway, including the insula, anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex, hypothalamus, amygdala, and hippocampus.

Considerations

  • The subtypes of chronic pain and the variations of pain conditions are complex and inadequately characterised.

  • Large-scale trials in a wide variety of chronic pain patients are needed.

Introduction

Chronic pain is a common health-care problem. Breivik et al. (Reference Breivik, Collett, Ventafridda, Cohen and Gallacher1) had a survey conducted in 15 European countries and Israel involving 46 000 people, and found that 19% of the people had pain lasting more than 6 months. In an Australian epidemiological survey, 21% of the 17 543 adults were suffering from chronic pain (Reference Blyth, March, Brnabic and Cousins2). An epidemiological survey in 17 countries showed that the prevalence of chronic pain lasting more than 12 months in developing countries was 37.3%, whereas in the developed countries the prevalence was 41.1% (Reference Tsang, Von Korff and Lee3).

As defined by the International Society for the Study of Pain, chronic pain means the feeling and experience of actual or potential tissue damage, or the patient's subjective description. Chronic pain is a non-malignant pain that persists in the healing tissue (Reference Merskey and Bogduk4). A time of 3 months is a common demarcation point distinguishing acute and chronic pain. However, a time of 6 months is often selected for research purposes (Reference Merskey and Bogduk4).

Chronic pain negatively affects a patient's physical activity, daily communication, cognitive function, and mood. Memory loss is one of the main complaints in chronic pain patients. In 1957, Jones (Reference Jones5) found that chronic pain patients can hardly recall the episodic memory that is associated with the pain. Then research concerning the memory of pain started. There were few studies on this issue during the following 20 years. From the 1980s, many findings about memory impairment in chronic pain patients have been reported (Reference Jamison, Sbrocco and Parris6Reference Jongsma, Postma and Souren12). Studies on the memory of chronic pain patients are important because of the following reasons. First, it is helpful for diagnosis and treatments, because diagnosis and treatment rely on the patient's memory. If the memory is distorted, precise diagnosis and treatment evaluation would be difficult. Second, it is helpful for the study of the theory of memory in chronic pain patients.

This review analyses memory function and the potentially related factors in chronic pain patients. The databases PubMed, EMBASE, SpringerLink, and PsycINFO were searched using the keywords ‘memory’ and ‘chronic pain’. Publications until September 2012 were included and analysed, with an emphasis on the empirical research published over the past 20 years.

Research on memory complaints of chronic pain patients

Memory complaint is one of the chief complaints in chronic pain patients with cognitive deficits. Memory complaint is an important source of anxiety in chronic pain patients. For example, (i) they often complain about their ‘bad memory’ of forgetting the once familiar phone number and a friend's name; (ii) they have thinking interruptions and often forget the topic of conversation a moment ago; (iii) they forget the appointment time and where to go (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13). This brought a lot of trouble to their daily lives, and therefore their family and friends often complained. There is not enough related research on the memory complaints, although it is a very common issue. For example, Jorge et al. (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13) reported that 73–81% of chronic pain patients complained of memory decline. The earliest research was conducted in 1988 by Jamison et al. (Reference Jamison, Sbrocco and Parris6). They divided 363 chronic pain patients into high- and low-memory complaint groups by the score of two questions on the SCL-90: (i) the extent to which you find it difficult to concentrate and (ii) the extent to which you find it difficult to remember things. There were no significant differences in age, gender, education level, marital status, drug use, long pain, and pain intensity between these two groups. It is found that patients in the high-memory complaint group suffered from emotional problems (such as depression, anger, irritability, and tension), lack of family support, dissatisfaction of social activities, and sexual life significantly more than those of the low-memory complaint group (Reference Jamison, Sbrocco and Parris6). Schnurr and MacDonald (Reference Schnurr and MacDonald7) found that memory complaint in chronic pain patients is significantly more than that in mental patients or patients receiving psychotherapy. The degrees of depression and chronic pain are related to the number of memory complaints (Reference Schnurr and MacDonald7). Although patients often attribute memory complaint to codeine or psychoactive drug use, they are not related (Reference Schnurr and MacDonald7). McCracken and Iverson (Reference McCracken and Iverson14) found that 23.4% of the 275 patients with chronic persistent pain complained of forgetfulness. Memory complaint is moderately correlated with anxiety, depression, pain intensity, and antidepressants, but it is not related to opioid (Reference McCracken and Iverson14). Muñoz and Esteve (Reference Muñoz and Esteve15) found that of the 149 patients with chronic benign pain, 61% cannot remember movie and book contents. Of them, 44% have forgetfulness, 38% are forgetful in dealing with the day-to-day affairs, and 38% forget conversation topics. Memory complaint is mainly associated with depression and anxiety, and not related to benzodiazepine, age, and pain duration (Reference Muñoz and Esteve15). Of the three components (rumination, helplessness, and magnification), only rumination is positively correlated with memory complaint (Reference Muñoz and Esteve15).

Studies on memory complaint have shown that the complaints of memory decline are prevalent in chronic pain patients. The memory complaints are closely related to negative emotions (depression and anxiety). However, the relationship between memory complaints and other factors, such as pain and drug, needs to be further studied. Analyses in the currently available studies of memory complaints are based on the subjective experiences of patients, which are not objective measurements. In addition, although most studies exclude traumatic brain injury and stroke history, potential brain damages cannot be completely ruled out, which may affect the results.

Research on the working memory of chronic pain patients

Working memory plays an essential role in the cognitive function and has an important impact on other cognitive functions. Working memory is also closely associated with other memory. For example, the autobiographical memory theory greatly emphasises on the role of working memory in the specific autobiographical memory recall (Reference Williams, Barnhofer, Hermans, Raes, Watkins and Dalgleish16). It indicates that the impaired working memory makes the resources required to retrieve specific autobiographical event tasks to be not enough, which leads to generalisation of autobiographical memory retrieval and response delay. This point has been supported by a lot of studies on autobiographical memory of patients with depression, schizophrenia, and post-traumatic stress disorder (Reference Williams17Reference Warren and Haslam19).

However, there were few studies on working memory of chronic pain patients. Etherton et al. (Reference Etherton, Bianchini, Ciota, Heinly and Greve20) found in two studies that chronic pain patients usually have attention and working memory impairments, which is further supported by Jorge and Dick (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13,Reference Dick and Rashiq21). However, Sjøgren et al. (Reference Sjøgren, Christrup, Petersen and Højsted22) found that there was no significant difference in working memory between the chronic pain patient group and the healthy control group. Although brain injury, neurological diseases, mental illness, and cancers were excluded from these studies, drug types, doses, and duration used in these studies are inconsistent. Various measurement tools used for working memory in these studies, such as Working Memory Index (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13,Reference Etherton, Bianchini, Ciota, Heinly and Greve20), Paced Auditory Serial Addition the Task (Reference Sjøgren, Christrup, Petersen and Højsted22), Reading Span Test (Reference Dick and Rashiq21), and Spatial Span Test (Reference Dick and Rashiq21), may result in inconsistency.

The factors related to the working memory impairment in chronic pain patients are as follows. (1) Pain conditions: Dick and Rashiq (Reference Dick and Rashiq21) found that the impaired memory and pain are closely related to the interference of the memory trace. They believe that pain may interfere with the retention of memory traces, and memory trace is important for information processing and long-term memory storage (Reference Dick and Rashiq21). Etherton et al. (Reference Etherton, Bianchini, Ciota, Heinly and Greve20) found that working memory impairment and pain intensity were not related in chronic pain patients. Jorge et al. (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13) also found that there was no significant relationship between the degree of memory impairment and duration of pain. However, Sjøgren et al. (Reference Sjøgren, Christrup, Petersen and Højsted22) found that pain intensity and working memory scores are significantly negatively correlated. (2) Emotions: Jorge et al. (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13) found a positive correlation between the intensity of anxiety and the degree of memory impairment. (3) Analgesic drugs: Sjøgren et al. (Reference Sjøgren, Christrup, Petersen and Højsted22) explored the impact of drug therapy on the working memory of chronic pain patients, and found that working memory of patients without drug treatment was significantly better than that of the patients who had oral opioid therapy. (4) Age: Dick and Rashiq (Reference Dick and Rashiq21) found there was no significant relationship between age and working memory of chronic pain patients, which is further supported by Oosterman et al. (Reference Oosterman, Derksen, van Wijck, Veldhuijzen and Kessels23). (5) Other psychosocial factors: Jorge et al. (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13) found that working memory impairments in chronic pain patients were affected by the environment and individual effort, whereas Dick and Rashiq (Reference Dick and Rashiq21) did not find any obvious relationship between this impairment and sleep problems or psychological pressure. Altogether, because of the relatively few studies on the working memory of chronic pain patients, influencing factors and mechanisms of the working memory impairment are still worth further exploring because of the important role of working memory in cognitive functions.

Research on the long-term memory in chronic pain patients

Long-term memory impairment is also found in chronic pain patients by various studies. Landro et al. (Reference Landro, Stiles and Sletvold24) found that it was more difficult for fibromyalgia patients to complete the long-term memory recall task requiring sustained efforts (e.g. the Randt Memory Test, the Code Memory Test, the Word Fluency task, the Kimura Recurring Recognition Figures Test). Grisart (Reference Grisart, Van der Lindenb and Bastin25) found that the recognition operations were significantly more difficult for chronic pain patients than the control group and that the chronic pain patients had more ‘understanding’ reactions, whereas the control group had more ‘remember’ reactions. Oosterman (Reference Oosterman, Derksen, van Wijck, Veldhuijzen and Kessels23) investigated the visual and verbal episodic memory in chronic pain patients, and found that their episodic memory scores were significantly lower than the control group. Busch (Reference Busch, Montgomery, Melin and Lundberg26) adopted the computerised memory experiment to test chronic pain patients: he found that the long-term memory in chronic pain patients was not all reduced but was closely related to the stimulus materials.

Thus far, there have been some studies on the long-term memory impairments in chronic pain patients, showing several kinds of influencing factors. (1) Pain conditions: Thomas et al. (Reference Thomas, Iezzi, Melanie, Duckworth, Archibald and Klinck27) found that the more serious the pain is, the worse is the memory of chronic pain patients. Iezzi et al. (Reference Iezzi, Duckworth, Vuong, Archibald and Klinck28) also found that the severity of the pain really predicted the memory performance. Hart et al. (Reference Hart, Martelli and Zasler29) believed that pain interfered with memory process rather than memory-encoding process. However, Grisart et al. (Reference Grisart, Van der Lindenb and Bastin25) believed that there was no relationship between recognition results and the pain intensity in the ‘remember’ reaction of the chronic pain patients. Jorge (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13) thought that the long-term memory was not influenced by pain intensity and pain duration. This inconsistency may result from sampling differences, because samples in the studies of Thomas and Iezzi included patients with different subtypes of chronic pain (Reference Thomas, Iezzi, Melanie, Duckworth, Archibald and Klinck27,Reference Iezzi, Duckworth, Vuong, Archibald and Klinck28), whereas Jorge (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13) only studied patients with chronic low back pain. (2) Emotions: Iezzi (Reference Iezzi, Archibald, Barnett, Klinck and Duckworth8) found that the more emotional stress patients have, the more difficulties they have to recall the word materials or the non-word materials. Landro (Reference Landro, Stiles and Sletvold24) found that when depression was controlled in fibromyalgia patients, only the patients with a history of depression showed worse long-term memory grades than the healthy control group. Hart (Reference Hart, Martelli and Zasler29) believed that similar to the impacts of pain on memory, emotion influenced the recall process, rather than the memory-encoding process. Grisart (Reference Grisart, Van der Lindenb and Bastin25) found that the recognition grade was not related to anxiety and depression. (3) Analgesic drugs: Iezzi et al. (Reference Iezzi, Archibald, Barnett, Klinck and Duckworth8) investigated the relationship between the analgesic drugs and long-term memory in chronic pain patients, and found no obvious relationship between the drugs and the memory impairment, which was further supported by Grisart et al. (Reference Grisart, Van der Lindenb and Bastin25). (4) Attention: Jorge et al. (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13) found that the decrease in the long-term memory was closely related to attention. Oosterman (Reference Oosterman, Derksen, van Wijck, Veldhuijzen and Kessels23) also believed that the decrease in the episodic memory scores may be due to the impaired attention of chronic pain patients. (5) Other psychosocial factors: It is found that long-term memory in chronic pain patients is associated with education time, post-traumatic stress, psychological pressure, and study skills (Reference Jorge, Gerard and Revel13,Reference Thomas, Iezzi, Melanie, Duckworth, Archibald and Klinck27,Reference Iezzi, Duckworth, Vuong, Archibald and Klinck28). In conclusion, the long-term memory impairment in chronic pain patients has been widely recognised and analysed. However, specific aspects of the long-term memory, such as word logical memory, scene memory, and the memory of feeling-channel effects, needed to be further studied.

Research on the autobiographical memory of chronic pain patients

Since the late 1980s, study on the autobiographical memory became a hot spot of memory research, especially favoured by clinical scientists, including our lab (Reference Williams, Barnhofer, Hermans, Raes, Watkins and Dalgleish16,Reference Liu, Li, Xiao, Yang and Jiang30). The autobiographical memory status of chronic pain patients also attracted the interests of researchers.

It is believed that the pain experience will be influenced by negative or pain-related autobiographical memory (Reference Christianson and Safer31). Morley (Reference Morley32) used questionnaires and interviews to assess autobiographical memory in chronic pain patients, and found the close relationship between the pain experience (e.g. distress, pain intensity) and vivid original pain events.

It was also found that when chronic pain patients extract autobiographical events, emotions and pain response appeared. Patients intended to extract negative emotions and pain-related autobiographical events, rather than positive emotions or related autobiographical events. Wright and Morley (Reference Wright and Morley33) found that there was no difference in the number of recalled autobiographical events to neutral and pain-related cue words between the patients and the control group; however, chronic pain patients recalled more memories associated with body pain and recalled pain events significantly faster than the non-pain event memories. Oosterman (Reference Oosterman, de Vries, Dijkerman, de Haan and Scherder34) found a positive correlation between memory and the self-reported pain experience, regardless of the pain intensity and memory. Kelly (Reference Kelly, Lloyd, Nurmikko and Roberts35) found that chronic pain patients recalled more pain-related autobiographical events, which activated more in the anterior cingulate cortex and left inferior frontal gyrus.

One of the main concerns in this field is whether chronic pain patients accurately retrieve their pain-event memories. Feine (Reference Feine, Lavigned, Dao, Morin and Lund36) found that the chronic pain patients had an inaccurate memory of the pre-treatment pain. Moreover, the longer time there is, the more errors there are in memories (Reference Feine, Lavigned, Dao, Morin and Lund36). Memory accuracy of the pre-treatment pain depends on the degree of pain. Patients with slight pain may exaggerate pain before treatment, whereas patients with severe pain may underestimate the pain before treatment. Regardless of the actual decreased degree of pain, most patients tend to overestimate the levels of pain relief (Reference Feine, Lavigned, Dao, Morin and Lund36). However, Salovey et al. (Reference Salovey, Smith, Turk, Jobe and Willis37) found that patients had an accurate memory of the pain event. Matera et al. (Reference Matera, Morell, La Grua, Sassu, Santagostino and Prioreschi38) found that the chronic pain patients tend to overestimate the intensity of their pre-treatment pain. Bryant (Reference Bryant39) had the 6-week treatments of 40 chronic pain patients, where the patients recorded their sensory and affective pain intensity, anxiety, and depression intensity before treatments, and they were re-evaluated at the end of the treatments. They found that the patients who report increased pains and depressions tend to overestimate their pain and depression before treatment (Reference Bryant39). McNamara (Reference McNamara, Benson, McGeeney, Brown and Albert40) found that the self-report of pain intensity of ‘scene mode’ (from the first-person perspective to re-experience the event memory) was higher than the ‘observer mode’ (from the perspective of the observer), using the Edingurgh Handedness Inventory.

In conclusion, the research on autobiographical memory in chronic pain patients is mainly related to the accuracy of pain event, extraction number, and extraction response tendencies, as well as the factors affecting autobiographical memory retrieval. Studies in this area are still in a preliminary stage. There is no consensus conclusion about the accuracy of pain as the subtypes of chronic pain patients in each study are different. In addition, they used different pain assessment tools. Thus, further research in this field is necessary.

Neuropsychological mechanisms of memory deficits in chronic pain patients

It is shown that pain and memory share a common central nervous pathway (Reference Prakash and Golwala41). The nervous pathway of memory includes the insula, anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex, hypothalamus, amygdala, and hippocampus (Reference Prakash and Golwala41). The prefrontal cortex is considered to be the main structure of the nerve pain and neural network of memory. The insula is the pain perception hub, which is involved in maintaining memory trace. The hippocampus is generally considered to be a central nervous tissue of memory. Recently, some neuroimaging studies have shown that the structure of the hippocampus has changed in chronic pain patients, suggesting that pain and memory may have intrinsic neural connections. In addition, there exists a partial overlapping between the neural network systems of pain and memory. Most neuroimaging studies on pain and memory have shown that these regions are activated during the pain and the memory processes (Reference Jongsma, Postma and Souren12). Some regions of the limbic system, such as the amygdala, are closely related to persistent pain (Reference Eccleston42).

The psychological mechanism of memory deficits of the chronic pain patients can be explained by ‘attentional resources’. There are mainly two theories. One is the narrow attentional resources hypothesis, and the other is the diminished capacity hypothesis. The narrow attentional resources hypothesis assumes that attention is focused on the feeling or emotion of pain (Reference Grisart and Van der Linden9). The attentional resources required by the task of processing is limited, resulting in decreased memory operation performance. Grisart et al. found that, compared with the healthy control group, the awareness in the patient group significantly reduced and the unconscious memory did not change significantly. It shows that the weakened memory attention control is a predisposing factor of memory efficiency. At the same time, the fear of pain and catastrophising beliefs can better explain the influences on the intention memories than the socio-demographic characteristics and feeling of pain (Reference Grisart and Van der Linden9). The diminished capacity hypothesis assumes that the memory deficits in chronic pain patients are the results of the overall decrease in attention resources (Reference Eccleston42). It is suggested that the chronic pain interferes with the cognitive operations and increases the workload of task attention, resulting in the lack of general attention resources, thus affecting the memory task (Reference Eccleston42). However, the assumption has been challenged by Christianson et al. and Kelly et al., as they found that the self-related memory content is better than the neutral content in chronic pain patients (Reference Christianson and Safer31,Reference Kelly, Lloyd, Nurmikko and Roberts35). In addition, on the basis of the antagonism of the self-related processing promotion (cognitive biases) and the cognitive interference on neutral information processing (cognitive impairment), a general pattern of cognitive function can be drawn. However, it still remains unknown whether the basic ability of the attention task is limited by the chronic pain, or chronic pain decreases spontaneous initiative of the controlling processing.

Limitations and prospective of studies on the memory of chronic pain patients

Chronic pain patients are from a special group, with common symptoms of cognitive deficits. Therefore, the studies of memory in chronic pain patients help in understanding the etiology of chronic pain, promote the clinical diagnosis and treatment, and improve memory theory. The currently available data and theory have explained memory impairment in chronic pain patients, but many controversies remain.

First, because the reason of chronic pain is very complex, the homogeneity of samples is relatively poor. Chronic nociceptive pain (caused by tissue damage or inflammation), chronic neuropathic pain (caused by nerve damage or disease), composite pain (nociceptive pain and neuropathic pain combined), and chronic psychological pain have different pathological mechanisms and different impacts on memories. Large-scale studies usually collected different subtypes of chronic pain patients with a great difference in the sample size of subtypes, resulting in inconsistent results. For small-scale studies, single subtype is preferred, such as chronic low back pain patients. For the large-scale sample study, the number of each subtype should be roughly equal, which would enhance the validity of research and facilitate the investigation of differences between the various subtypes of chronic pains.

Second, because of the individual measurement, sampling is more difficult. Most cross-sectional studies are with small samples, which greatly limit the validity of results. For example, some studies found no relationships between memory and pain duration that may result from the type II statistical error. Therefore, a large-scale sample is necessary for effective parameter analysis, which can study the psychological factors and the potential impacts on memory function of chronic pain patients.

Third, the influence of pain intensity on memory function in chronic pain patients is a controversial issue. Some experiments determined the relationship between pain intensity and memory function by experimentally inducing pains. Despite its convenience (such as the pain intensity spectrum from light to serious), there are many differences in experimentally induced pains and the natural pains, especially for chronic pains. Natural pain involves the context, persistent or chronic, related muscles, internal tissues, and organs, rather than just skins. In clinical studies, patients with light chronic pains may not come to look for medical care, and patients with serious pain are unwilling or unable to complete a memory task. Therefore, in the study, there are mainly patients with medium or partial chronic pains. Thus, the linear relationship between the pain intensity and the memory performance may be masked. In the intervention study, the speed of memory function recovery and the speed of pain reduction may be different. Therefore, in the repeated measurements, the latter timing is an important influencing factor. Therefore, future research, in addition to outpatient sampling, should also include community sampling. Improved evaluation method is also necessary in order for patients with serious pains to complete the memory test, so that the chronic pain intensity changes linearly from light to severe. In the intervention study, the repeated measurement tracking design is recommended to overcome the deficiencies of the experimental design.

Fourth, there are limited studies on the influence of analgesic drugs on memory function of chronic pain patients. Analgesic drugs relieve pain, thereby improving patient's memory function. However, analgesic drugs often have side effects for the potential impact on patient's memory function. Meanwhile, there are considerable differences between the peripheral antipyretic analgesic drug and the central analgesic drug. In addition, conditions such as dosages, time (taking/stopping taking), administration (oral/injection), and concomitant medications should all be considered. These factors make it more complex and difficult, often resulting in inconsistent results in previous studies. Therefore, future research should include these factors during studies on the memory function of chronic pain patients.

In addition, despite the shared central nervous pathway between pain and memory, it is not sufficient to show the neuropsychological mechanisms of memory defects in chronic pain patients. For example, there is no specific neuropsychological study on the mechanisms and interaction of pain and working memory, long-term memory, and autobiographical memory, respectively. Thus, cognitive neuroscience is important for understanding the neuropsychological mechanisms of chronic pain patients with memory deficits. Future research should focus on the subclinical characteristics of chronic pain, enlarge the sample size, and emphasise the experimental intervention method and cognitive neuroscience method.

Acknowledgement

This study was supported by Hunan Provincial Education Sciences Planning of China (Grant No. XJK011BXL002), Hunan Social Science Fund Project of China (Grant No. 11YBB194), and Science Foundation of Hengyang Normal University (Grant No. 10B64).

Authors’ contributions

Xianhua Liu and Li Li contributes equally to this work. All authors read the final manuscript, and all have given final approval of the version submitted for publication.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

1. Breivik, H, Collett, B, Ventafridda, V, Cohen, R, Gallacher, D. Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. Eur J Pain 2006;10:287333.Google Scholar
2. Blyth, FM, March, LM, Brnabic, AJ, Cousins, MJ. Chronic pain and frequent use of health care. Pain 2004;111:5158.Google Scholar
3. Tsang, A, Von Korff, M, Lee, S et al. Common chronic pain conditions in developed and developing countries: gender and age differences and comorbidity with depression-anxiety disorders. J Pain 2008;9:883891.Google Scholar
4. Merskey, H, Bogduk, N. Classification of chronic pain. Descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Prepared by the International Association for the Study of Pain, Subcommittee on Taxonomy. Pain Suppl 1986;3:S1S226.Google Scholar
5. Jones, E. Pain. Int J Psychoanal 1957;38:255257.Google Scholar
6. Jamison, RN, Sbrocco, T, Parris, WC. The influence of problems with concentration and memory on emotional distress and daily activities in chronic pain patients. Int J Psychiatry Med 1988;18:183191.Google Scholar
7. Schnurr, RF, MacDonald, MR. Memory complaints in chronic pain. Clin J Pain 1995;11:103111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Iezzi, T, Archibald, Y, Barnett, P, Klinck, A, Duckworth, M. Neurocognitive performance and emotional status in chronic pain patients. J Behav Med 1999;22:205216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Grisart, JM, Van der Linden, M. Conscious and automatic uses of memory in chronic pain patients. Pain 2001;94:305313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Grisart, JM, Van der Linden, M, Masquelier, E. Controlled processes and automaticity in memory functioning in fibromyalgia patients: relation with emotional distress and hypervigilance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002;24:9941009.Google Scholar
11. Jonsson, T, Christrup, LL, Højsted, J et al. Symptoms and side effects in chronic non-cancer pain: patient report vs. systematic assessment. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 2011;55:6974.Google Scholar
12. Jongsma, MLA, Postma, SAE, Souren, P et al. Neurodegenerative properties of chronic pain: cognitive decline in patients with chronic pancreatitis. PLoS One 2011;6:e23363.Google Scholar
13. Jorge, LL, Gerard, C, Revel, M. Evidences of memory dysfunction and maladaptive coping in chronic low back pain and rheumatoid arthritis patients: challenges for rehabilitation. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med 2009;45:469477.Google Scholar
14. McCracken, LM, Iverson, GL. Predicting complaints of impaired cognitive functioning in patients with chronic pain. J Pain Symptom Manage 2001;21:392396.Google Scholar
15. Muñoz, M, Esteve, R. Reports of memory functioning by patients with chronic pain. Clin J Pain 2005;21:287291.Google Scholar
16. Williams, JMG, Barnhofer, T, Hermans, D, Raes, F, Watkins, E, Dalgleish, T. Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol Bull 2007;133:122148.Google Scholar
17. Williams, JMG. Capture and rumination, functional avoidance, and executive control (CaRFAX): three processes that underlie overgeneral memory. Cogn Emot 2006;20:548568.Google Scholar
18. Raes, F, Hermans, D, Williams, JMG et al. Is overgeneral autobiographical memory an isolated memory phenomenon in major depression? Memory 2006;14:584594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Warren, Z, Haslam, C. Overgeneral memory for public and autobiographical events in depression and schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 2007;12:301321.Google Scholar
20. Etherton, JL, Bianchini, KJ, Ciota, MA, Heinly, MT, Greve, KW. Pain, malingering and the WAIS-III Working Memory Index. Spine J 2006;6:6171.Google Scholar
21. Dick, BD, Rashiq, S. Disruption of attention and working memory traces in individuals with chronic pain. Anesth Analg 2007;104:12231229.Google Scholar
22. Sjøgren, P, Christrup, LL, Petersen, MA, Højsted, J. Neuropsychological assessment of chronic non-malignant pain patients treated in a multidisciplinary pain centre. Eur J Pain 2005;9:453462.Google Scholar
23. Oosterman, JM, Derksen, LC, van Wijck, AJ, Veldhuijzen, DS, Kessels, RP. Memory functions in chronic pain: examining contribution of attention and age to test performance. Clin J Pain 2011;27:7075.Google Scholar
24. Landro, NI, Stiles, TC, Sletvold, H. Memory functioning in patients with primary fibromyalgia and major depression and healthy controls. J Psychosom Res 1997;42:297306.Google Scholar
25. Grisart, J, Van der Lindenb, M, Bastin, C. The contribution of recollection and familiarity to recognition memory performance in chronic pain patients. Behav Res Ther 2007;45:10771084.Google Scholar
26. Busch, H, Montgomery, W, Melin, B, Lundberg, U. Visuospatial and verbal memory in chronic pain patients: an explorative study. Pain Pract 2006;6:179185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Thomas, S, Iezzi, T, Melanie, P, Duckworth, MP, Archibald, Y, Klinck, A. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and general activity level in the prediction of neurocognitive performance in chronic pain patients. Int J Rehabil Health 2000;5:3142.Google Scholar
28. Iezzi, T, Duckworth, MP, Vuong, LN, Archibald, YM, Klinck, A. Predictors of neurocognitive performance in chronic pain patients. Int J Behav Med 2004;11:5661.Google Scholar
29. Hart, RP, Martelli, MF, Zasler, ND. Chronic pain and neuropsychological functioning. Neuropsychol Rev 2000;10:131149.Google Scholar
30. Liu, X, Li, L, Xiao, J, Yang, J, Jiang, X. Abnormalities of autobiographical memory of patients with depressive disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Psychother Theor Res Pract 2012.Google Scholar
31. Christianson, SA, Safer, MA. Emotional events and emotions in autobiographical memories. In: Rubin DC, editor. Remembering our past: studies in autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1995, p. 218243.Google Scholar
32. Morley, S. Vivid memory for ‘everyday’ pains. Pain 1993;55:5562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Wright, J, Morley, S. Autobiographical memory and chronic pain. Br J Clin Psychol 1995;34:255265.Google Scholar
34. Oosterman, JM, de Vries, K, Dijkerman, HC, de Haan, EH, Scherder, EJ. Exploring the relationship between cognition and self-reported pain in residents of homes for the elderly. Int Psychogeriat 2009;21:157163.Google Scholar
35. Kelly, S, Lloyd, D, Nurmikko, T, Roberts, N. Retrieving autobiographical memories of painful events activates the anterior cingulate cortex and inferior frontal gyrus. J Pain 2007;8:307314.Google Scholar
36. Feine, JS, Lavigned, GJ, Dao, TT, Morin, C, Lund, JP. Memories of chronic pain and perceptions of relief. Pain 1998;77:137141.Google Scholar
37. Salovey, P, Smith, AF, Turk, DC, Jobe, JB, Willis, GB. The accuracy of memory for pain: not so bad most of the time. Pain Soc J 1993;2:184191.Google Scholar
38. Matera, D, Morell, M, La Grua, M, Sassu, B, Santagostino, G, Prioreschi, G. Memory distortion during acute and chronic pain recalling. Minarva Anestesiol 2003;69:775783.Google ScholarPubMed
39. Bryant, RA. Memory for pain and affect in chronic pain patients. Pain 1993;54:347351.Google Scholar
40. McNamara, P, Benson, E, McGeeney, B, Brown, A, Albert, ML. Modes of remembering in patients with chronic pain: relation to current pain. J Nerv Ment Dis 2005;193:5357.Google Scholar
41. Prakash, S, Golwala, P. Phantom headache: pain-memory-emotion hypothesis for chronic daily headache? J Headache Pain 2011;12:281286.Google Scholar
42. Eccleston, C. Chronic pain and distraction: an experimental investigation into the role of sustained and shifting attention in the processing of chronic persistent pain. Behav Res Ther 1995;33:391405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed