Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T18:49:45.783Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Long-term effects of ageing and ovariectomy on aversive and recognition memory and DNA damage in the hippocampus of female rats

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2013

Daniela Dimer Leffa*
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular and Cellular Biology (LABIM), Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
Adriani Paganini Damiani
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular and Cellular Biology (LABIM), Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
Daiane Dal Col Damazio
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular and Cellular Biology (LABIM), Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
Naiana Pereira Guerra
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Neuroscience, Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
Morgana Moretti
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Neuroscience, Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
Geovana Gomes da Silva de Brito
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Neuroscience, Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
Carina Rodrigues Boeck
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Neuroscience, Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
Elaine Cristina Gavioli
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Behavioural Pharmacology, Postgraduate Programme of Behavioural Neuroscience, Centre for Biosciences, University Campus, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, RN, Brazil
Vanessa Moraes de Andrade
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular and Cellular Biology (LABIM), Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, Criciúma, SC, Brazil
*
Daniela Dimer Leffa, Laboratory of Molecular and Cellular Biology, Postgraduate Programme of Health Sciences, Department of Health Sciences, University of Southern Santa Catarina, 888.06-000, Criciúma, SC, Brazil. Tel: +55 (48) 3431 2757; Fax: +55 (48) 3431 2671; E-mail: daniela_leffa@yahoo.com.br
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

This study investigated the influence of ageing – in particular the decrease of gonadal hormone levels during the ageing process – on the memory and the levels of DNA damage in the hippocampus of female rats.

Methods

Three groups of female Wistar rats were investigated: Group I consisted of non-ovariectomised, adult animals (6 months old); Group II consisted of non-ovariectomised, aged animals (18 months old); and Group III consisted of ovariectomised, aged animals (18 months old). The memory of the animals in these groups was examined via novel object recognition and inhibitory avoidance tests. The hippocampus tissue samples of all animals were obtained via biopsy and used to quantify the DNA damage using a Comet Assay.

Results

According to our findings, the process of ageing results in a change during the behavioural tests. To prevent genotoxic damage to the hippocampus caused by the ageing process, lowered hormone levels seem to be part of a protective biochemical mechanism in the body of rats. Animals that were previously submitted to an ovariectomy adapted better to these lower levels of hormones.

Conclusion

Our results indicate that ovariectomy can provide beneficial long-term effects on the memory. However, this could be specific to the kind of memory examined, as the aversive memory deficits caused by ageing were not affected by ovariectomy.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 2013 

Significant outcomes

  • The object recognition tests indicated an impaired memory in non-ovariectomised aged rats.

  • The inhibitory avoidance tests suggested an affected aversive memory in the aged rats independent of the performed ovariectomy.

  • Non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats showed lower concentrations of 17β-oestradiol in the blood when compared with adult rats.

  • A Comet Assay showed significantly increased levels of DNA damage in the hippocampus of non-ovariectomised aged rats compared with adult rats.

Limitations

  • Non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats showed similar 17β-oestradiol levels in the blood.

Introduction

Ageing is a normal and an inevitable phenomenon, which depends on the interaction of several genetic, environmental and lifestyle factors (Reference King, Bristow-Craig, Gillespie and Barnett1). It can be described as a general decline of the physiological bodily functions, which initially leads to morbidity and ultimately to mortality (Reference Masoro2,Reference Sohal and Dubey3). This process is caused by numerous factors, all of which result in an irreversible damage to the biological macromolecules (DNA, proteins, lipids), cells and organs (Reference Ivancsits, Diem, Jahn and Rüdiger4). Gonadal hormones are extremely important for proper functioning of the body, and even small hormonal imbalances can result in severe macroscopic dysfunctions of the body (Reference Kankofer, Radzki, Bieńko and Albera5). Oestrogen, a female steroid, is synthesised inside the ovaries using blood cholesterol and acetyl coenzyme A. Only three types of oestrogen are present in the plasma of women in significant quantities: β-oestradiol, estrona and estriol. The most important oestrogen is β-oestradiol, as it is 12 times more potent than estrona and 80 times more potent than estriol (Reference Pilšáková, Riečanský and Jagla6). Oestrogens cause proliferation and growth of specific cells, which are responsible for the development of the secondary sexual characteristics. Non-pregnant women produce oestrogens in significant quantities only in the ovaries, albeit lower quantities are also produced via the adrenal cortex. During pregnancy, however, the placenta also releases significant amounts of oestrogens. Later, in the climacteric period, the levels of oestrogens start to decrease as a result of the decreased functionality of the ovaries (Reference Greendale, Lee and Arriola7). Oestrogen is also believed to be able to act as an antioxidant. The neuroprotective activity thereby arises from inhibited lipid peroxidation caused by the presence of a phenolic ring moiety in the steroid scaffold of oestrogen (Reference Kankofer, Radzki, Bieńko and Albera5). Accordingly, the decrease in endogenous oestrogen levels can result in increased levels of free radicals and cause adverse effects in a variety of tissue types in postmenopausal women (Reference Sugioka, Shimosegawa and Nakano8,Reference Green, Gordon and Simpkins9).

Oestrogen can, moreover, influence the learning performance. Learning and memory functions are fundamental to the acquisition and storage of new information. These processes are potentially affected by a change in the hormone levels. Gonadal hormones such as estriol have been shown to exhibit beneficial effects on the buildup of hippocampus-dependent memory (Reference Luine10). When women enter the menopausal period, the productivity of the ovarian follicles decreases, resulting in decreased levels of ovarian steroids and consequently increased levels of free radicals (Reference Persky, Green and Stubley11).

Ovariectomy is a common animal model used to evaluate the function of hormones in the physiological and pathological mechanisms (Reference Kankofer, Radzki, Bieńko and Albera5). The administration of oestradiol to ovariectomised female rats over a short period of time is associated with the acquisition of memory (Reference Gibbs, Gabor, Cox and Johnson12) and an increased cognitive performance compared with ovariectomised animals that did not receive the same hormonal treatment (Reference Luine, Jacome and Maclusky13,Reference Rhodes and Frye14).

Aims of this study

This study investigated the influence of ageing – in particular the decrease of gonadal hormone levels during the ageing process – on the memory and the levels of DNA damage in the hippocampus of female rats.

Methods and materials

Animals

Adult female Wistar rats (12 animals of 6 months and 24 animals of 18 months) were used in this study. It was ensured that none of the animals carried any offspring at any time before the study. The animals were obtained from the Fundação para Ciência e Investigação da Saúde (Porto Alegre, Brazil). They were kept in the University of Southern Santa Catarina for at least 2 weeks before the ovariectomy surgery. The animals were housed in boxes at 23 ± 1°C with access to water and food ad libitum and exposure to alternating bright–dark cycles of 12 h each (start of the bright period at 07:00 a.m.). Behavioural procedures were performed between 08:00 and 12:00 a.m. in a room, where exposure to excess sound and light was kept to a minimum. Animals were allowed to acclimatise to the procedure room 1 h before the tests. All experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Sociedade Brasileira de Neurociências e Comportamento.

Ovariectomy

Ovariectomy procedures were carried out 15 months before the behavioural tests. Surgery was exclusively performed under anaesthesia using ketamine (60 mg/kg; Bristol Laboratories, Syracuse, NY, USA) and xylazine IP injections (12 mg/kg; Miles Laboratories, Shawnee, KS, USA) as described by Robertson et al. (Reference Robertson, Owens, Klindt and Friesen15).

A small incision (1 cm) through the skin and the muscle back wall was made parallel to the bodyline. The ovaries were located and a silk thread was tightly tied around the oviduct including the ovarian blood vessels. The oviduct was sectioned and the ovaries were removed, taking good care to leave the knot intact. The skin and the muscle wall were subsequently sutured with a silk thread. The sham surgery control subjects received the same incisions and were sutured in the same way as the ovariectomised animals; however, their ovaries were only palpated instead of being removed. After the surgical procedure, the animals were treated with the antibiotic enrofloxacino (Flotril®, 2.5%, 0.025 mg/kg, IP injection; Schering-Plough Animal Health, Cotia, Brazil) once a day for 10 consecutive days. The animals were subsequently allowed to convalesce in their boxes (adult rats: 3 months; aged rats: 15 months) before being subjected to the behavioural tests and Comet Assay. Two weeks before the behavioural tests, vaginal smears were collected on a daily basis and analysed with a light microscope.

Vaginal cytology

Vaginal smears were collected using cotton tipped swabs, which were wetted with physiological saline solution of ambient temperature before being inserted into the vaginas of the restrained rats. The swabs were gently turned and rolled against the vaginal wall and then removed. Cells were transferred onto dry glass slides by rolling the swabs across the slides. The slides were then air-dried and stained (ca. 400 μl of Accustain/45 s; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA). The slides were then rinsed with water, overlaid with a cover slip and examined immediately at a 200-fold magnification under bright field illumination. The stage of the oestrous cycle was determined based on the presence or absence of the leukocytes, cornified epithelial and nucleated epithelial cells as described by Marcondes et al. (Reference Marcondes, Bianchi and Tanno16).

To provide a reliable basis for the comparisons, we only considered dioeostrual adult sham rats for the statistical analysis of behavioural tests. This phase was chosen because the blood serum concentrations of oestrogens have been shown to be lower compared with other phases of the oestrous cycle (Reference Marcondes, Miguel, Melo and Spadari-Bratfisch17,Reference Zuluaga, Agrati, Pereira, Uriarte, Fernández-Guasti and Ferreira18). Vaginal smears were collected immediately after the behavioural tests to avoid the effects of human manipulation in the rats’ responses. However, before collecting the blood samples, the vaginal cytology was not checked. Therefore, we cannot ensure that the adult sham rats were in the dioeostrus period when their blood samples were collected. In the aged sham rats, the vaginal smears were collected every day 1 week before starting the behavioural tests. To confirm the complete removal of all the ovarian tissues in the ovariectomised aged rats, vaginal smears were collected directly after the surgical procedure and subsequently every day for a period of 1 week before the behavioural tests.

Open-field test

Tests were carried out on 60 × 40 cm open-field grids, which were surrounded by brown plywood walls (50 cm high) with a front glass wall. The floor of the open field was divided into 12 equal squares by black lines. The animals were gently placed at the centre of the arena and allowed to explore freely for 5 min. The number of crossings and rearings of black lines were recorded to evaluate the spontaneous locomotion of the rats in this unfamiliar environment as described by de Chaves et al. (Reference de Chaves, Moretti and Castro19).

Object recognition test

After a habituation period of 24 h, training sessions were conducted. Individual rats were placed for 5 min in the open field in which two identical objects (A1 and A2; A1 = A2 = cube) were positioned in two adjacent corners (distance to the walls = 10 cm). In a long-term recognition memory test taken 24 h after the initial training, the rats explored the open field for 5 min in the presence of one familiar (A; cube) and one unfamiliar object (B; pyramid with a square-shaped base). All objects had surfaces with similar smooth textures and blue colour. They were also similar in size (weight 150–200 g) but distinct in shape (cube vs. pyramid). From these test sessions, the recognition memory index was calculated for each animal, which is expressed as the ratio: TB/(TA + TB) (TA = time spent exploring the familiar object A; TB = time spent exploring the unfamiliar object B). Between the trials, objects were washed with a 10% ethanol solution. Exploration was defined as sniffing (exploring the object from a distance of 3–5 cm) or touching the object with the nose and/or the forepaws (Reference Réus, Valvassori, Machado, Martins, Gavioli and Quevedo20).

Inhibitory avoidance test

The inhibitory avoidance test is described elsewhere (Reference Roesler, Vianna, de-Paris and Quevedo21). For this test, an acrylic box (50 × 25 × 25 cm) with a floor of parallel stainless steel bars (diameter: 1 mm; spacing: 1 cm) and a platform (7 × 7 cm; height 2.5 cm) was used. The animals were placed on the platform and the latency to step down on the grid with all four paws was measured with an automatic device. Training sessions were conducted 10 or 30 days after the surgery. Immediately after stepping down on the grid, the animals received an electric shock (0.3 mA/2 s). In the test sessions carried out 24 h after the training, no electric shock was induced and the step-down latency period (maximum of 180 s) was used as a measure of retention. The behavioural tests were always conducted by the same person that was unrecognisable for the examined animals.

Determination of blood serum levels of 17β-oestradiol by radioimmunoassay (RIA)

Seven days after the inhibitory avoidance test, rats were anaesthetised with thiopental (50 mg/kg) and blood samples were collected by the intracardiac puncture techniques. The blood samples were stored in Eppendorf tubes at 4°C. The blood samples were subsequently centrifuged (5000 rpm/5 min) to separate the serum. The serum was then stored at –20°C until 17β-oestradiol concentrations were determined using RIA. The RIA uses luminescent antibodies, which specifically bind to the examined hormone. These antibodies are immobilised on an ELISA plate with 96 wells. Therefore, the serum was deposited on the plates with the antibodies. After that, the plates were washed before the samples were reconstituted in butter solution, and 17β-oestradiol was added. After a short reaction time, the plates were washed again and analysed using the quantitative luminescence. The concentration of 17β-oestradiol in the blood serum of the animals was inversely proportional to the amount of luminescence recorded. The functional sensitivity of the test was 5 pg/ml.

Detection of DNA damage by Comet Assay

Comet Assay was used to assess the long-term effects of age and ovariectomy on the DNA damage in the hippocampus tissue samples of female rats. This test is a rapid, simple and sensitive technique to detect damage to the DNA molecules in individual cells. It is also considered to be a sentinel test, signalling possible damage to genetic material (Reference Izquierdo and Medina22).

Comet Assay was carried out under alkaline conditions as described by Singh et al. (Reference Singh, McCoy, Tice and Schneider23) and modified by Tice et al. (Reference Tice, Agurell and Anderson24). The cell samples from different organ tissues were obtained according to Tice et al. (Reference Tice, Agurell and Anderson24). Each hippocampus tissue sample was placed in cold phosphate-buffered saline solution before being finely minced with a syringe plunger to obtain a homogenous cell suspension. To calculate the damage index (DI), the cells were visually separated into five classes according to the tail size (0 = no tails to 4 = maximum-length tails). A single DNA damage score was accordingly obtained for each sample and consequently for each group studied. The DI of any group could range from 0 (completely undamaged = 100 cells × 0) to 400 (maximum damage = 100 cells × 4). The damage frequency (DF in %) was calculated for each sample on the basis of the number of cells with tail compared with those without the tail (Reference Collins25).

Statistical analysis

The results obtained from the behavioural tests are expressed as median values ± interquartile interval (0.25–0.75). The differences between the training tests were evaluated by the Wilcoxon test. In the inhibitory avoidance test, the comparisons between groups were evaluated by the Kruskal–Wallis test, followed by the post hoc Dunn tests if necessary. Locomotor and exploratory activities and open-field crossings as well as 17β-oestradiol concentrations in the blood samples are all reported as mean values ± SEM values and assessed by the analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests, followed by the Student–Newman–Keuls tests. For the Comet Assay, the statistical analysis of the differences between DF and DI were reported as mean values ± SEM values and assessed by ANOVA, followed by the Tukey HSD tests. P-values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. All analyses were performed using the SPSS statistical software package (SPSS 12.0; SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

The results obtained from our object recognition tests (see Fig. 1) indicated that the adult rats and aged rats – ovariectomised or not – spent the same time exploring two equal objects exposed to them for the first time. When the animals were exposed to two different objects 24 h after the training, the adult rats spent more time exploring the unknown object, indicating that they recognised the previously shown object. Non-ovariectomised aged rats spent the same time exploring both objects, indicating that the ageing process impaired the recognition memory. However, aged ovariectomised rats showed statistically significant differences between the training and test sessions, indicating that some recognition memory had been preserved (p < 0.05, ANOVA, Tukey).

Fig. 1 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on object recognition memory in adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats. Bars represent median values ± interquartile range. *p < 0.05 when compared between training and test sessions within the same group (n = 12).

In the inhibitory avoidance tests (see Fig. 2), the adult rats had a significantly increased latency to step down from the platform during the test sessions compared with the training sessions. This is indicative of an acquisition of memory as a result of the training (p < 0.05, Kruskal–Wallis, Dunn). Aged rats, independent of ovariectomisation, did not exhibit any significant differences in latency between the sessions, suggesting an impairment of aversive memory in these rats. We did not observe any significant differences in the number of crossings and rearings or exploratory activity as evident from the open-field tests for all groups analysed. This suggests that the spontaneous locomotor activity was not affected by ageing or ovariectomy procedures in the long term (see Fig. 3). Moreover, aged rats showed, independent of ovariectomisation, lower blood concentrations of 17β-oestradiol compared with non-ovariectomised adult rats (see Fig. 4). However, stress can increase the synthesis of ovarian hormones in female rats. We are, therefore, unable to separate the long-term effects of stress and ovariectomisation from the increased 17β-oestradiol production levels observed in the female rats (p < 0.05, ANOVA, Tukey) of this study. On the basis of the Comet Assay results (Fig. 5), which was made using the hippocampus cells, the non-ovariectomised aged rats had a significant increase in DNA damage (p < 0.05, ANOVA, Tukey) compared with the adult rats, for both index and frequency of DNA damages. Difference between the adult rats and aged rats that suffered early ovariectomy was observed.

Fig. 2 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on the latency time of adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats submitted to inhibitory avoidance test. Bars represent median values ± interquartile range of the latency to step down from the platform. *p < 0.05 when compared between training and test sessions within the same group (n = 12). #p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovariectomised adult rats.

Fig. 3 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on crossing and rearing numbers of adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats submitted to open-field tests. Bars represent the medium values ± SEM values (n = 12).

Fig. 4 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on serum concentration of 17β-oestradiol in adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats. Bars represent the medium values ± SEM values (n = 12). *p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovariectomised adult rats. #p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovarectomised aged rats.

Fig. 5 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on DNA damage in the hippocampus of adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats. The Comet Assay produced the parameters damage index and damage frequency. Bars represent the medium values ± SEM values (n = 12). *p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovariectomised adult rats.

Discussion

This study examined the changes in memory acquisition of adult (6 months) or aged (18 months) female Wistar rats in two animal memory models. We also evaluated the DNA damage in the hippocampus tissue samples of these rats. Our results indicate that non-ovariectomised aged rats display an affected memory in both behavioural tests and increased levels of DNA damage in the hippocampus compared with the adult rats.

The cognitive performance of the animals was analysed using the object recognition and the inhibitory avoidance tests. Reports in the scientific literature suggest that the cognitive perception of aged rats should be examined by using a test that eliminates the elements of motivation and favourable physical conditions (e.g. the Morris water maze test) (Reference Luparini, Del Vecchio, Barillari, Magnani and Prosdocimi26). To eliminate any influence of potential deficiencies of the locomotor system in aged rats, we used the open-field test. We did not observe any alterations of the locomotor system in aged rats as a result of an ovariectomy when compared with adult animals.

The inhibitory avoidance test is a memory model task, which is used to study the acquisition and retention of memory in rodents. It has already been reported that rodents naturally suffer from an age-related cognitive decline, and the results of our inhibitory avoidance tests in this study are in agreement with these findings (Reference Martinez and Rigter27Reference Winocur and Hasher29). We furthermore used an object recognition task to assess the recognition memory in our rodents. The results of these tests are also in accordance with the previous reports, which suggest that senescence induces memory deficiencies in rodents (Reference Vannucchi, Scali, Kopf, Pepeu and Casamenti28,Reference Scali, Giovannini, Prosperi, Bartolini and Pepeu30,Reference Wallace, Frankfurt, Arellanos, Inagaki and Luine31).

Considering that some clinical tests suggest deleterious effects of combined replacement with oestrogen and testosterone in some kinds of cognitive tasks in postmenopause women (Reference Lethaby, Hogervorst, Richards, Yesufu and Yaffe32), these observations suggest that gonadal hormones can harm memory in elderly women. These results depend, however, on many factors such as patient age, type of menopause (natural or surgical), mode of hormone administration (oral, transdermal or intramuscular) and hormone dosage (Reference Markowska and Savonenko33). Ovariectomised rats have been used to examine the contributions of ovarian hormones towards learning and memory in different stages of their development. Some studies have shown that the lack of gonadal hormones promotes cognitive impairment in rodents, especially in the first month after surgery (Reference Monteiro, de Mattos, Ben, Netto and Wyse34,Reference Wallace, Luine, Arellanos and Frankfurt35). It is worth highlighting here that only very few studies have evaluated the relationship between ovarian hormones and their long-term effects on the cognitive performance in older, ovariectomised animals. Markowska and Savonenko (Reference Markowska and Savonenko33) found that ovariectomies had a negative impact on the working memory of middle-age rats. However, the spontaneous alteration tests, place recognition tests and space memory tests evaluated by the Morris water maze test were unaffected. In contrast, Bimonte-Nelson et al. (Reference Bimonte-Nelson, Singleton, Hunter, Price, Moore and Granholm36) showed that ovariectomies improved the space memory of aged rats in the long term (more than 3 months) that was evaluated by the radial maze tests. Chaves and colleagues have reported similar results. They suggested that the surgical removal of the ovarian glands in adult rats could induce short-term alterations in the structures and functions of oestradiol target neurons in the brain, which in turn could be responsible for short-term effects on emotional behaviour (Reference de Chaves, Moretti and Castro19). However, compensatory mechanisms could take place in the long term, thus supporting the theory that ovarian hormones have a temporary modulating effect on behavioural despair. Pan et al. (Reference Pan, Li, Yeung and Xu37) demonstrated via Morris water maze tests that the phytoestrogen or oestradiol treatments can improve the acquisition and retention of spatial memory in ovariectomised rats in the long term. Interestingly, aged female monkeys that were ovariectomised when young had a significant increase in space memory compared with non-ovariectomised control monkeys (Reference Lacreuse, Herndon and Moss38).

In the present study, young rats (3 months of age) were ovariectomised. After 15 months, they were submitted to the object recognition and inhibitory avoidance tests. Adult rats (6 months of age) and non-ovariectomised aged rats (18 months of age) were used as control groups. Our results showed that ovariectomy improved the performance of aged rats during the object recognition tests in the long term. However, we did not observe any significant differences in the recognition memory tests between ovariectomised and non-ovariectomised aged rats. These observations suggest that ovariectomy can, in the long term, protect against recognition memory deficiencies caused by ageing. Bimonte-Nelson et al. (Reference Bimonte-Nelson, Singleton, Hunter, Price, Moore and Granholm36) reported a short-term (3 weeks after surgery) impairment of the spatial memory after ovariectomy, which was restored 12 weeks after the surgical removal of the glands. These results demonstrate the need for further studies to understand the function and impact of gonadal hormones (or the lack thereof) on the memory of rats. Heikkinen et al. (Reference Heikkinen, Puoliväli and Tanila39) observed that a prolonged treatment (40 days) of aged, 24-month-old female mice with oestrogen resulted in varying effects on their learning performance, depending on the memory task. For example, a decreased number of reference memory errors in the win-stay RAM task but no effect on the number of working memory errors in the T-maze test was observed. Moreover, ovariectomised mice showed improved performance in the position discrimination test in the T-maze.

The underlying biological and/or biochemical principles that explain the mechanisms through which ovariectomy could improve the memory of aged rats in the long term still remain unknown. The prolonged privation of gonadal hormones can induce many neurochemical reactions. It is, for example, known that the gradual failure of the ovaries during senescence results in a concomitant reduction of oestradiol levels, whereas increased progesterone levels can be observed in the postmenopausal phase of female rats (Reference Bimonte-Nelson, Singleton, Hunter, Price, Moore and Granholm36). Ovariectomised animals, however, show an abrupt increase in both oestradiol and progesterone levels (Reference Bimonte-Nelson, Singleton, Hunter, Price, Moore and Granholm36). Previous studies have shown that progesterone rules for spatial and work memory (Reference Bimonte-Nelson, Singleton, Williams and Granholm40,Reference Bimonte-Nelson, Francis, Umphlet and Granholm41). It could be argued that the surgical removal of the ovaries causes an increase in the blood progesterone levels, and thus could mitigate the cognitive deficiencies caused by ageing and the physiological failure of ovaries in the long term. However, Hua et al. (Reference Hua, Reiss and Tang42) have observed that the progesterone and combined progesterone/Vitamin D treatments can preserve spatial memory.

Several studies have investigated the relationship between DNA damage and ageing. The accumulation of age-dependent damage can be demonstrated by the cytogenetic and molecular assays (Reference Singh, Danner and Tice43Reference Rossi, Seita, Czechowicz, Bhattacharya, Bryder and Weissman47). Heuser and colleagues have studied the basal levels of DNA damage in mice of different ages using the Comet Assay. Their results demonstrate a strong relationship between age, the inefficiency of DNA repair mechanisms and/or the accumulation of DNA damage (Reference Heuser, de Andrade, Peres, Gomes de Macedo Braga and Bogo Chies48).

The results presented here show that non-ovariectomised aged rats exhibit increased levels of DNA damage in the hippocampus tissue compared with adult rats. This is in agreement with the previously reported data (Reference Heuser, de Andrade, Peres, Gomes de Macedo Braga and Bogo Chies48). However, we also observed that rats that were ovariectomised 15 months before the experimental tests did not show any significant differences with respect to DNA damage in the hippocampus when compared with adult rats.

Previous studies have shown that extended privation of oestrogen can cause irreversible alterations to the function and structure of oestrogen-specific neurons in the brain (Reference Schumacher, Coirini and McEwen49Reference Tominaga, Yamauchi and Shuto51). However, these changes could be compensated for in the long term by the removal of the ovaries: our results showed comparable results with respect to DNA damage in the hippocampus and to the recognition memory tests for ovariectomised and adult rats. In short, these results do not agree with the notion that sexual hormones are essential for a normal cognitive operation in rodents. Verma and Rana (Reference Verma and Rana52) reported a significantly induced breakage of strands in the DNA of the hepatic and renal cells as a result of an ovariectomy. Andersen and colleagues evaluated the genetic damage in different target organs induced by the sleep deprivation associated with an ovariectomy in vivo. They observed that DNA damage occurs in the brain cells independent of the phase of the oestrous cycle (Reference Andersen, Ribeiro and Alvarenga53).

Our data suggest that the beneficial long-term effects of an ovariectomy on the memory could depend on the kind of memory evaluated. It seems that the aversive memory deficits caused by ageing do not change as a result of an ovariectomy. Our results also suggest that oestrogen is not the only hormone responsible for these behavioural and molecular alterations, as non-ovariectomised aged rats also showed decreased blood oestrogen levels compared with adult rats. The impact of the abrupt decrease in plasmatic gonadal hormone levels on ovariectomised rats and the gradual decrease of plasmatic gonadal hormone levels in non-ovariectomised aged rats could be crucial for the explanation of these observed differences with respect to the performance in the memory tests and genotoxic damage in the hippocampus.

Acknowledgements

This research was financially supported by grants from the Programa de Pós-graduação, em Ciências da Saúde of the University of Southern Santa Catarina (UNESC).

Authors Contributions

Daniela Dimer Leffa (graduate student) interpreted the data of the Comet Assay and assisted writing the manuscript for this paper. Adriani Paganini Damiani (graduate student) analysed the data of the Comet Assay. Daiane Dal Col Damazio (graduate student) assisted in writing the manuscript for this paper. Naiana Pereira Guerra (graduate student) performed the ovariectomy surgery. Morgana Moretti (graduate student) carried out the behavioural tests. Geovana Gomes da Silva de Brito (graduate student) conducted the vaginal cytology tests. Carina Rodrigues Boeck (Professor) was responsible for the final approval of the manuscript version to be published. Elaine Cristina Gavioli (Professor) was responsible for the experimental design. Vanessa Moraes de Andrade (Professor) was responsible for the final approval of the manuscript version to be published.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

1.King, CM, Bristow-Craig, HE, Gillespie, ES, Barnett, YA. In vivo antioxidant status, DNA damage, mutation and DNA repair capacity in cultured lymphocytes from healthy 75- to 80-year-old humans. Mutat Res 1997;377:137147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Masoro, EJ. Dietary restriction and aging. J Am Geriatr Soc 1993;41:994999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Sohal, RS, Dubey, A. Mitochondrial oxidative damage, hydrogen peroxide release, and aging. Free Radic Biol Med 1994;16:621626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Ivancsits, S, Diem, E, Jahn, O, Rüdiger, HW. Age-related effects on induction of DNA strand breaks by intermittent exposure to electromagnetic fields. Mech Ageing Dev 2003;124:847850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Kankofer, M, Radzki, RP, Bieńko, M, Albera, E. Anti-oxidative/oxidative status of rat liver after ovariectomy. J Vet Med A Physiol Pathol Clin Med 2007;54:225229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Pilšáková, L, Riečanský, I, Jagla, F. The physiological actions of isoflavone phytoestrogens. Physiol Res 2010;59:651664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Greendale, GA, Lee, NP, Arriola, ER. The menopause. Lancet 1999;353:571580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Sugioka, K, Shimosegawa, Y, Nakano, M. Estrogens as natural antioxidants of membrane phospholipid peroxidation. FEBS Lett 1987;210:3739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Green, PS, Gordon, K, Simpkins, JW. Phenolic A ring requirement for the neuroprotective effects of steroids. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 1997;63:229235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Luine, VN. Sex steroids and cognitive function. J Neuroendocrinol 2008;20:866872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Persky, AM, Green, PS, Stubley, Let al. Protective effect of estrogens against oxidative damage to heart and skeletal muscle in vivo and in vitro. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 2000;223:5966.Google ScholarPubMed
12.Gibbs, RB, Gabor, R, Cox, T, Johnson, DA. Effects of raloxifene and estradiol on hippocampal acetylcholine release and spatial learning in the rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004;29:741748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Luine, VN, Jacome, LF, Maclusky, NJ. Rapid enhancement of visual and place memory by estrogens in rats. Endocrinology 2003;144:28362844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Rhodes, ME, Frye, CA. Androgens in the hippocampus can alter, and be altered by, ictal activity. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2004;78:483493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Robertson, MC, Owens, RE, Klindt, J, Friesen, HG. Ovariectomy leads to a rapid increase in rat placental lactogen secretion. Endocrinology 1984;114:18051811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Marcondes, FK, Bianchi, FJ, Tanno, AP. Determination of the estrous cycle phases of rats: some helpful considerations. Braz J Biol 2002;62:609614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Marcondes, FK, Miguel, KJ, Melo, LL, Spadari-Bratfisch, RC. Estrous cycle influences the response of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Physiol Behav 2001;74:435440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Zuluaga, MJ, Agrati, D, Pereira, M, Uriarte, N, Fernández-Guasti, A, Ferreira, A. Experimental anxiety in the black and white model in cycling, pregnant and lactating rats. Physiol Behav 2005;84:279286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.de Chaves, G, Moretti, M, Castro, AAet al. Effects of long-term ovariectomy on anxiety and behavioral despair in rats. Physiol Behav 2009;97:420425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Réus, GZ, Valvassori, SS, Machado, RA, Martins, MR, Gavioli, EC, Quevedo, J. Acute treatment with low doses of memantine does not impair aversive, non-associative and recognition memory in rats. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 2008;376:295300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21.Roesler, R, Vianna, MR, de-Paris, F, Quevedo, J. Memory-enhancing treatments do not reverse the impairment of inhibitory avoidance retention induced by NMDA receptor blockade. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1999;72:252258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22.Izquierdo, I, Medina, JH. Memory formation: the sequence of biochemical events in the hippocampus and its connection to activity in other brain structures. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1997;68:285316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Singh, NP, McCoy, MT, Tice, RR, Schneider, EL. A simple technique for quantization of low levels of DNA damage in individual cells. Exp Cell Res 1988;175:184191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24.Tice, RR, Agurell, E, Anderson, Det al. Single cell gel/Comet Assay: guidelines for in vitro and in vivo genetic toxicology testing. Environ Mol Mutagen 2000;35:206221.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Collins, AR. The Comet Assay for DNA damage and repair: principles, applications, and limitations. Mol Biotechnol 2004;26:249261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Luparini, MR, Del Vecchio, A, Barillari, G, Magnani, M, Prosdocimi, M. Cognitive impairment in old rats: a comparison of object displacement, object recognition and water maze. Aging (Milano) 2000;12:264273.Google ScholarPubMed
27.Martinez, JL Jr, Rigter, H. Assessment of retention capacities in old rats. Behav Neural Biol 1983;39:181191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Vannucchi, MG, Scali, C, Kopf, SR, Pepeu, G, Casamenti, F. Selective muscarinic antagonists differentially affect in vivo acetylcholine release and memory performances of young and aged rats. Neuroscience 1997;79:837846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29.Winocur, G, Hasher, L. Aging and time-of-day effects on cognition in rats. Behav Neurosci 1999;113:991997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Scali, C, Giovannini, MG, Prosperi, C, Bartolini, L, Pepeu, G. Tacrine administration enhances extracellular acetylcholine in vivo and restores the cognitive impairment in aged rats. Pharmacol Res 1997;36:463469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Wallace, M, Frankfurt, M, Arellanos, A, Inagaki, T, Luine, V. Impaired recognition memory and decreased prefrontal cortex spine density in aged female rats. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007;1097:5457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32.Lethaby, A, Hogervorst, E, Richards, M, Yesufu, A, Yaffe, K. Hormone replacement therapy for cognitive function in postmenopausal women. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2008;1:CD003122.Google Scholar
33.Markowska, AL, Savonenko, AV. Effectiveness of estrogen replacement in restoration of cognitive function after long-term estrogen withdrawal in aging rats. J Neurosci 2002;22:1098510995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Monteiro, SC, de Mattos, CB, Ben, J, Netto, CA, Wyse, AT. Ovariectomy impairs spatial memory: prevention and reversal by a soy isoflavone diet. Metab Brain Dis 2008;23:243253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Wallace, M, Luine, V, Arellanos, A, Frankfurt, M. Ovariectomized rats show decreased recognition memory and spine density in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. Brain Res 2006;1126:176182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Bimonte-Nelson, HA, Singleton, RS, Hunter, CL, Price, KL, Moore, AB, Granholm, AC. Ovarian hormones and cognition in the aged female rat: I. Long-term, but not short-term, ovariectomy enhances spatial performance. Behav Neurosci 2003;117:13951406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37.Pan, M, Li, Z, Yeung, V, Xu, RJ. Dietary supplementation of soy germ phytoestrogens or estradiol improves spatial memory performance and increases gene expression of BDNF, TrkB receptor and synaptic factors in ovariectomized rats. Nutr Metab (Lond) 2010;15:775.Google Scholar
38.Lacreuse, A, Herndon, JG, Moss, MB. Cognitive function in aged ovariectomized female rhesus monkeys. Behav Neurosci 2000;114:506513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39.Heikkinen, T, Puoliväli, J, Tanila, H. Effects of long-term ovariectomy and estrogen treatment on maze learning in aged mice. Exp Gerontol 2004;39:12771283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Bimonte-Nelson, HA, Singleton, RS, Williams, BJ, Granholm, AC. Ovarian hormones and cognition in the aged female rat: II. Progesterone supplementation reverses the cognitive enhancing effects of ovariectomy. Behav Neurosci 2004;118:707714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Bimonte-Nelson, HA, Francis, KR, Umphlet, CD, Granholm, AC. Progesterone reverses the spatial memory enhancements initiated by tonic and cyclic oestrogen therapy in middle-aged ovariectomized female rats. Eur J Neurosci 2006;24:229242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Hua, F, Reiss, JI, Tang, Het al. Progesterone and low-dose vitamin D hormone treatment enhances sparing of memory following traumatic brain injury. Horm Behav 2012;61:642651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43.Singh, NP, Danner, DB, Tice, RRet al. Basal DNA damage in individual human lymphocytes with age. Mutat Res 1991;256:16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Heuser, VD, da Silva, J, Moriske, HJ, Dias, JF, Yoneama, ML, de Freitas, TR. Genotoxicity biomonitoring in regions exposed to vehicle emissions using the Comet Assay and the micronucleus test in native rodent Ctenomys minutus. Environ Mol Mutagen 2002;40:227235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Chevanne, M, Caldini, R, Tombaccini, D, Mocali, A, Gori, G, Paoletti, F. Comparative levels of DNA breaks and sensitivity to oxidative stress in aged and senescent human fibroblasts: a distinctive pattern for centenarians. Biogerontology 2003;4:97104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.López-Diazguerrero, NE, Luna-López, A, Gutiérrez-Ruiz, MC, Zentella, A, Königsberg, M. Susceptibility of DNA to oxidative stressors in young and aging mice. Life Sci 2005;77:28402854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47.Rossi, DJ, Seita, J, Czechowicz, A, Bhattacharya, D, Bryder, D, Weissman, IL. Hematopoietic stem cell quiescence attenuates DNA damage response and permits DNA damage accumulation during aging. Cell Cycle 2007;6:23712376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48.Heuser, VD, de Andrade, VM, Peres, A, Gomes de Macedo Braga, LM, Bogo Chies, JA. Influence of age and sex on spontaneous DNA damage detected by micronucleus test and Comet Assay the peripheral blood cells of mice. Cell Biol Int 2008;32:12231229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
49.Schumacher, M, Coirini, H, McEwen, BS. Regulation of high-affinity GABAA receptors in the dorsal hippocampus by estradiol and progesterone. Brain Res 1989;487:178183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50.Bossé, R, DiPaolo, T. The modulation of brain dopamine and GABAA receptors by estradiol: a clue for CNS changes occurring at menopause. Cell Mol Neurobiol 1996;16:199212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51.Tominaga, K, Yamauchi, A, Shuto, Het al. Ovariectomy aggravates convulsions and hippocampal gamma-aminobutyric acid inhibition induced by cyclosporin A in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 2001;430:243249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52.Verma, Y, Rana, SV. Modulation of phase-II enzyme activities in benzene treated ovariectomized rats. Environ Toxicol Pharmacol 2011;31:371377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53.Andersen, ML, Ribeiro, DA, Alvarenga, TAet al. Are endogenous sex hormones related to DNA damage in paradoxically sleep-deprived female rats? Horm Behav 2010;57:216221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on object recognition memory in adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats. Bars represent median values ± interquartile range. *p < 0.05 when compared between training and test sessions within the same group (n = 12).

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on the latency time of adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats submitted to inhibitory avoidance test. Bars represent median values ± interquartile range of the latency to step down from the platform. *p < 0.05 when compared between training and test sessions within the same group (n = 12). #p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovariectomised adult rats.

Figure 2

Fig. 3 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on crossing and rearing numbers of adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats submitted to open-field tests. Bars represent the medium values ± SEM values (n = 12).

Figure 3

Fig. 4 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on serum concentration of 17β-oestradiol in adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats. Bars represent the medium values ± SEM values (n = 12). *p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovariectomised adult rats. #p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovarectomised aged rats.

Figure 4

Fig. 5 Long-term effects of ovariectomy (OVX) on DNA damage in the hippocampus of adult rats, non-ovariectomised and ovariectomised aged rats. The Comet Assay produced the parameters damage index and damage frequency. Bars represent the medium values ± SEM values (n = 12). *p < 0.05 when compared with non-ovariectomised adult rats.